首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications最新文献

英文 中文
The combination operation of grouping and ensemble coding for structured biological motion crowds in working memory. 工作记忆中结构化生物运动人群的分组和集合编码组合操作。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-10 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00574-6
Wei Chen, Shujuan Ye, Xin Yan, Xiaowei Ding

Massive studies have explored biological motion (BM) crowds processing for their remarkable social significance, primarily focused on uniformly distributed ones. However, real-world BM crowds often exhibit hierarchical structures rather than uniform arrangements. How such structured BM crowds are processed remains a subject of inquiry. This study investigates the representation of structured BM crowds in working memory (WM), recognizing the pivotal role WM plays in our social interactions involving BM. We propose the group-based ensemble hypothesis and test it through a member identification task. Participants were required to discern whether a presented BM belonged to a prior memory display of eight BM, each with distinct walking directions. Drawing on prominent Gestalt principles as organizational cues, we constructed structured groups within BM crowds by applying proximity and similarity cues in Experiments 1 and 2, respectively. In Experiment 3, we deliberately weakened the visibility of stimuli structures by increasing the similarity between subsets, probing the robustness of results. Consistently, our findings indicate that BM aligned with the mean direction of the subsets was more likely to be recognized as part of the memory stimuli. This suggests that WM inherently organizes structured BM crowds into separate ensembles based on organizational cues. In essence, our results illuminate the simultaneous operation of grouping and ensemble encoding mechanisms for BM crowds within WM.

大量研究探索了生物运动(BM)人群处理的显著社会意义,主要集中在均匀分布的人群上。然而,现实世界中的生物运动人群往往表现出层次结构,而不是均匀分布。如何处理这种结构化的 BM 人群仍是一个研究课题。考虑到工作记忆(WM)在涉及 BM 的社交互动中发挥着关键作用,本研究对结构化 BM 群在工作记忆(WM)中的表征进行了研究。我们提出了基于群体的集合假说,并通过一项成员识别任务对其进行了检验。参与者需要辨别一个呈现的 BM 是否属于先前记忆显示的八个 BM,每个 BM 都有不同的行走方向。在实验 1 和实验 2 中,我们利用著名的格式塔原理作为组织线索,分别通过邻近性和相似性线索在 BM 群中构建了结构化的群体。在实验 3 中,我们故意通过增加子集之间的相似性来削弱刺激结构的可见性,以探究实验结果的稳健性。我们的研究结果一致表明,与子集平均方向一致的 BM 更有可能被识别为记忆刺激的一部分。这表明,WM 会根据组织线索将结构化的 BM 群组织成独立的集合。从本质上讲,我们的研究结果阐明了在 WM 中对 BM 群同时进行分组和集合编码的机制。
{"title":"The combination operation of grouping and ensemble coding for structured biological motion crowds in working memory.","authors":"Wei Chen, Shujuan Ye, Xin Yan, Xiaowei Ding","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00574-6","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00574-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Massive studies have explored biological motion (BM) crowds processing for their remarkable social significance, primarily focused on uniformly distributed ones. However, real-world BM crowds often exhibit hierarchical structures rather than uniform arrangements. How such structured BM crowds are processed remains a subject of inquiry. This study investigates the representation of structured BM crowds in working memory (WM), recognizing the pivotal role WM plays in our social interactions involving BM. We propose the group-based ensemble hypothesis and test it through a member identification task. Participants were required to discern whether a presented BM belonged to a prior memory display of eight BM, each with distinct walking directions. Drawing on prominent Gestalt principles as organizational cues, we constructed structured groups within BM crowds by applying proximity and similarity cues in Experiments 1 and 2, respectively. In Experiment 3, we deliberately weakened the visibility of stimuli structures by increasing the similarity between subsets, probing the robustness of results. Consistently, our findings indicate that BM aligned with the mean direction of the subsets was more likely to be recognized as part of the memory stimuli. This suggests that WM inherently organizes structured BM crowds into separate ensembles based on organizational cues. In essence, our results illuminate the simultaneous operation of grouping and ensemble encoding mechanisms for BM crowds within WM.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"45"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-07-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11236836/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141564807","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Which learning techniques supported by cognitive research do students use at secondary school? Prevalence and associations with students' beliefs and achievement. 中学学生使用哪些认知研究支持的学习方法?学生使用哪些学习方法?
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-06 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00567-5
Héctor Ruiz-Martín, Fernando Blanco, Marta Ferrero

Research in cognitive science has highlighted the effectiveness of several learning techniques, and a number of studies have analyzed their prevalence among university students and their relationship with academic achievement. In this study, we surveyed a large, heterogeneous sample of secondary school students to reveal how often they use research-supported techniques in comparison with other frequent techniques, and we analyzed the association between their study strategies and school achievement. We also assessed the associations between study techniques and several students' beliefs and attitudes toward learning (self-efficacy, goal orientation, control beliefs, growth mindset, and examination anxiety). Results showed that, except for distributed practice, only those techniques that are supported by previous research yielded an association with achievement, and they exhibited higher associations with self-efficacy, growth mindset, control beliefs, and learning goal orientation than non-supported techniques.

认知科学的研究强调了几种学习技巧的有效性,许多研究分析了这些技巧在大学生中的普遍性及其与学业成绩的关系。在本研究中,我们对中学生进行了大规模的异质样本调查,以揭示与其他常用技巧相比,他们使用研究支持的技巧的频率,并分析了他们的学习策略与学业成绩之间的关联。我们还评估了学习技巧与学生的几种学习信念和态度(自我效能感、目标导向、控制信念、成长心态和考试焦虑)之间的关联。结果表明,除分散练习外,只有那些得到先前研究支持的技巧才与学习成绩有关联,而且它们与自我效能感、成长心态、控制信念和学习目标定向的关联度要高于未得到支持的技巧。
{"title":"Which learning techniques supported by cognitive research do students use at secondary school? Prevalence and associations with students' beliefs and achievement.","authors":"Héctor Ruiz-Martín, Fernando Blanco, Marta Ferrero","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00567-5","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00567-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research in cognitive science has highlighted the effectiveness of several learning techniques, and a number of studies have analyzed their prevalence among university students and their relationship with academic achievement. In this study, we surveyed a large, heterogeneous sample of secondary school students to reveal how often they use research-supported techniques in comparison with other frequent techniques, and we analyzed the association between their study strategies and school achievement. We also assessed the associations between study techniques and several students' beliefs and attitudes toward learning (self-efficacy, goal orientation, control beliefs, growth mindset, and examination anxiety). Results showed that, except for distributed practice, only those techniques that are supported by previous research yielded an association with achievement, and they exhibited higher associations with self-efficacy, growth mindset, control beliefs, and learning goal orientation than non-supported techniques.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"44"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-07-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11227488/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141545390","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The social factors behind the mask: contextual effects on trait impressions from faces wearing a face mask. 面具背后的社会因素:戴着面具的面孔对特质印象的情境影响。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-27 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00570-w
Matilde Tumino, Luciana Carraro, Luigi Castelli

The presence of face masks can significantly impact processes related to trait impressions from faces. In the present research, we focused on trait impressions from faces either wearing a mask or not by addressing how contextual factors may shape such inferences. In Study 1, we compared trait impressions from faces in a phase of the COVID-19 pandemic in which wearing masks was a normative behavior (T1) with those assessed one year later when wearing masks was far less common (T2). Results at T2 showed a reduced positivity in the trait impressions elicited by faces covered by a mask. In Study 2, it was found that trait impressions from faces were modulated by the background visual context in which the target face was embedded so that faces wearing a mask elicited more positive traits when superimposed on an indoor rather than outdoor visual context. Overall, the present studies indicate that wearing face masks may affect trait impressions from faces, but also that such impressions are highly flexible and can significantly fluctuate across time and space.

面具的存在会严重影响与人脸特征印象相关的过程。在本研究中,我们重点研究了戴面具或不戴面具的面孔给人留下的特质印象,探讨了情境因素会如何影响这种推断。在研究 1 中,我们比较了在 COVID-19 大流行的一个阶段(T1)和一年后(T2)对人脸的特质印象,在前者中,戴口罩是一种规范行为,而在后者中,戴口罩的情况远没有那么普遍。T2 的结果显示,戴面具的人脸所引起的特质印象的积极性降低了。研究 2 发现,人脸的特质印象受目标人脸所处的背景视觉环境的影响,因此戴面具的人脸叠加在室内而非室外的视觉环境中时,会引起更多积极的特质印象。总之,目前的研究表明,戴面具可能会影响对人脸的特质印象,而且这种印象非常灵活,在不同的时间和空间会有显著的波动。
{"title":"The social factors behind the mask: contextual effects on trait impressions from faces wearing a face mask.","authors":"Matilde Tumino, Luciana Carraro, Luigi Castelli","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00570-w","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00570-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The presence of face masks can significantly impact processes related to trait impressions from faces. In the present research, we focused on trait impressions from faces either wearing a mask or not by addressing how contextual factors may shape such inferences. In Study 1, we compared trait impressions from faces in a phase of the COVID-19 pandemic in which wearing masks was a normative behavior (T1) with those assessed one year later when wearing masks was far less common (T2). Results at T2 showed a reduced positivity in the trait impressions elicited by faces covered by a mask. In Study 2, it was found that trait impressions from faces were modulated by the background visual context in which the target face was embedded so that faces wearing a mask elicited more positive traits when superimposed on an indoor rather than outdoor visual context. Overall, the present studies indicate that wearing face masks may affect trait impressions from faces, but also that such impressions are highly flexible and can significantly fluctuate across time and space.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"43"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11211305/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141459814","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Different deployments of attentional breadth selectively predict UFOV task performance in older adults. 注意力广度的不同部署可选择性地预测老年人的 UFOV 任务表现。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-26 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00569-3
Nicholas J Wyche, Mark Edwards, Stephanie C Goodhew

The Useful Field of View task (UFOV) is a strong and reliable predictor of crash risk in older drivers. However, while the functional domain of attention is clearly implicated in UFOV performance, the potential role of one specific attentional process remains unclear: attentional breadth (the spatial extent of the attended region around the point of visual fixation). The goal of the present study was to systematically test the role of two distinct aspects of attentional breadth, maintaining a specific breadth of attention and resizing the attended region, in UFOV performance. To this end, 135 older adults completed the UFOV and modified Navon tasks to measure their efficiency in maintaining, contracting, and expanding the breadth of attention. We then examined individual-difference associations between these aspects of attentional breadth deployment and UFOV performance. We found that performance on UFOV Subtask 2 was associated with efficient contraction of attentional breadth (i.e., resizing the attended region to a smaller area), while Subtask 3 performance was associated with the efficiency of expanding attentional breadth (i.e., resizing the attended region to a larger area). The selectivity of these relationships appears to implicate these specific deployments of attentional breadth in how people complete the task, as it suggests that these relationships are not simply attributable to shared variance in a broader domain of cognitive functioning. The implications of these results for our understanding of UFOV, as well as future research directions that test the relative contributions of different cognitive processes in predicting task performance, are discussed.

有用视野任务(UFOV)是预测老年驾驶员车祸风险的可靠指标。然而,虽然注意力的功能域与有用视野的表现有明显的联系,但一个特定注意力过程的潜在作用仍不清楚:注意力广度(视觉固定点周围被注意区域的空间范围)。本研究的目的是系统地测试注意广度的两个不同方面(保持特定的注意广度和调整被注意区域的大小)在 UFOV 表现中的作用。为此,135 名老年人完成了 UFOV 任务和修改后的 Navon 任务,以测量他们在保持、收缩和扩大注意广度方面的效率。然后,我们研究了注意力广度部署的这些方面与 UFOV 成绩之间的个体差异关联。我们发现,UFOV Subtask 2 的成绩与注意广度的有效收缩(即把注意区域调整到更小的范围)有关,而 Subtask 3 的成绩则与注意广度的有效扩展(即把注意区域调整到更大的范围)有关。这些关系的选择性似乎与人们如何完成任务的注意广度的特定部署有关,因为它表明这些关系并不仅仅归因于认知功能更广泛领域的共同差异。本文讨论了这些结果对我们理解 UFOV 的影响,以及未来测试不同认知过程在预测任务表现中的相对贡献的研究方向。
{"title":"Different deployments of attentional breadth selectively predict UFOV task performance in older adults.","authors":"Nicholas J Wyche, Mark Edwards, Stephanie C Goodhew","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00569-3","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00569-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Useful Field of View task (UFOV) is a strong and reliable predictor of crash risk in older drivers. However, while the functional domain of attention is clearly implicated in UFOV performance, the potential role of one specific attentional process remains unclear: attentional breadth (the spatial extent of the attended region around the point of visual fixation). The goal of the present study was to systematically test the role of two distinct aspects of attentional breadth, maintaining a specific breadth of attention and resizing the attended region, in UFOV performance. To this end, 135 older adults completed the UFOV and modified Navon tasks to measure their efficiency in maintaining, contracting, and expanding the breadth of attention. We then examined individual-difference associations between these aspects of attentional breadth deployment and UFOV performance. We found that performance on UFOV Subtask 2 was associated with efficient contraction of attentional breadth (i.e., resizing the attended region to a smaller area), while Subtask 3 performance was associated with the efficiency of expanding attentional breadth (i.e., resizing the attended region to a larger area). The selectivity of these relationships appears to implicate these specific deployments of attentional breadth in how people complete the task, as it suggests that these relationships are not simply attributable to shared variance in a broader domain of cognitive functioning. The implications of these results for our understanding of UFOV, as well as future research directions that test the relative contributions of different cognitive processes in predicting task performance, are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"42"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11208374/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141451846","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Wayfinding in pairs: comparing the planning and navigation performance of dyads and individuals in a real-world environment. 结伴寻路:比较双人和个人在真实世界环境中的规划和导航性能。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-21 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00563-9
Crystal Bae, Daniel Montello, Mary Hegarty

Navigation is essential to life, and it is cognitively complex, drawing on abilities such as prospective and situated planning, spatial memory, location recognition, and real-time decision-making. In many cases, day-to-day navigation is embedded in a social context where cognition and behavior are shaped by others, but the great majority of existing research in spatial cognition has focused on individuals. The two studies we report here contribute to our understanding of social wayfinding, assessing the performance of paired and individual navigators on a real-world wayfinding task in which they were instructed to minimize time and distance traveled. In the first study, we recruited 30 pairs of friends (familiar dyads); in the second, we recruited 30 solo participants (individuals). We compare the two studies to the results of an earlier study of 30 pairs of strangers (unfamiliar dyads). We draw out differences in performance with respect to spatial, social, and cognitive considerations. Of the three conditions, solo participants were least successful in reaching the destination accurately on their initial attempt. Friends traveled more efficiently than either strangers or individuals. Working with a partner also appeared to lend confidence to wayfinders: dyads of either familiarity type were more persistent than individuals in the navigation task, even after encountering challenges or making incorrect attempts. Route selection was additionally impacted by route complexity and unfamiliarity with the study area. Navigators explicitly used ease of remembering as a planning criterion, and the resulting differences in route complexity likely influenced success during enacted navigation.

导航对生活至关重要,它在认知上非常复杂,需要利用前瞻性和情景规划、空间记忆、位置识别和实时决策等能力。在许多情况下,日常导航都是在社会环境中进行的,在这种环境中,认知和行为都受到他人的影响,但现有的绝大多数空间认知研究都集中在个人身上。我们在此报告的两项研究有助于加深我们对社会寻路的理解,这两项研究评估了配对和单独导航员在真实世界寻路任务中的表现,在这项任务中,他们被要求尽量减少所花费的时间和路程。在第一项研究中,我们招募了 30 对朋友(熟悉的二人组);在第二项研究中,我们招募了 30 名单独参与者(个人)。我们将这两项研究结果与之前对 30 对陌生人(不熟悉的二人组)的研究结果进行了比较。我们从空间、社交和认知等方面的考虑得出了表现的差异。在三种情况下,单人参与者在初次尝试时准确到达目的地的成功率最低。与陌生人或个人相比,朋友的旅行效率更高。与伙伴合作似乎也能给寻路者带来信心:在导航任务中,无论哪种熟悉类型的二人组都比个人组更能坚持,即使在遇到挑战或尝试错误之后也是如此。路线选择还受到路线复杂性和对研究区域不熟悉程度的影响。导航员明确地将易记性作为规划标准,由此产生的路线复杂性差异很可能会影响制定导航的成功率。
{"title":"Wayfinding in pairs: comparing the planning and navigation performance of dyads and individuals in a real-world environment.","authors":"Crystal Bae, Daniel Montello, Mary Hegarty","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00563-9","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00563-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Navigation is essential to life, and it is cognitively complex, drawing on abilities such as prospective and situated planning, spatial memory, location recognition, and real-time decision-making. In many cases, day-to-day navigation is embedded in a social context where cognition and behavior are shaped by others, but the great majority of existing research in spatial cognition has focused on individuals. The two studies we report here contribute to our understanding of social wayfinding, assessing the performance of paired and individual navigators on a real-world wayfinding task in which they were instructed to minimize time and distance traveled. In the first study, we recruited 30 pairs of friends (familiar dyads); in the second, we recruited 30 solo participants (individuals). We compare the two studies to the results of an earlier study of 30 pairs of strangers (unfamiliar dyads). We draw out differences in performance with respect to spatial, social, and cognitive considerations. Of the three conditions, solo participants were least successful in reaching the destination accurately on their initial attempt. Friends traveled more efficiently than either strangers or individuals. Working with a partner also appeared to lend confidence to wayfinders: dyads of either familiarity type were more persistent than individuals in the navigation task, even after encountering challenges or making incorrect attempts. Route selection was additionally impacted by route complexity and unfamiliarity with the study area. Navigators explicitly used ease of remembering as a planning criterion, and the resulting differences in route complexity likely influenced success during enacted navigation.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"40"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11189867/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141433085","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Proneness to false memory generation predicts pseudoscientific belief endorsement. 易产生虚假记忆预示着伪科学信仰的认同。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-21 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00568-4
Naroa Martínez, Itxaso Barberia, Javier Rodríguez-Ferreiro

Among cognitive factors that can influence the endorsement of pseudoscientific beliefs, our study focuses on proneness to false memory generation. In this preregistered study, we presented 170 fluent English speakers residing in the USA with a misinformation task aimed at generating false memories. In this task, they first completed an event encoding stage, in which two events were narrated through sequentially presented pictures. One day later, they read a series of sentences relating the same events but which included several inaccurate descriptions aimed at producing a misinformation effect. Finally, we measured the influence of the misinformation manipulation over false memory generation. After completing the misinformation task, participants responded to a questionnaire measuring pseudoscientific beliefs. Our results showed a positive correlation between pseudoscience endorsement and false memory rates, which indicates that the latter might be a key factor influencing susceptibility to pseudoscience. To our knowledge, this is the first study showing a link between the tendency to believe in pseudoscience and variability regarding proneness to develop false memories. Practical implications for the design of new interventions to effectively reduce pseudoscientific beliefs and their negative impact on our society are discussed.

在可能影响对伪科学信仰的认可的认知因素中,我们的研究侧重于产生虚假记忆的可能性。在这项预先登记的研究中,我们向居住在美国的 170 名英语流利者提出了一项旨在产生错误记忆的错误信息任务。在这项任务中,他们首先完成了事件编码阶段,在这一阶段中,通过顺序呈现的图片叙述了两个事件。一天后,他们阅读了一系列与相同事件有关的句子,但其中包括一些旨在产生误解效应的不准确描述。最后,我们测量了错误信息操作对错误记忆生成的影响。在完成错误信息任务后,受试者回答了一份测量伪科学信仰的问卷。我们的研究结果表明,伪科学认可与错误记忆率之间存在正相关,这表明后者可能是影响对伪科学易感性的一个关键因素。据我们所知,这是第一项显示相信伪科学的倾向与产生错误记忆的易变性之间存在联系的研究。本文讨论了设计新的干预措施以有效减少伪科学信仰及其对社会的负面影响的实际意义。
{"title":"Proneness to false memory generation predicts pseudoscientific belief endorsement.","authors":"Naroa Martínez, Itxaso Barberia, Javier Rodríguez-Ferreiro","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00568-4","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00568-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Among cognitive factors that can influence the endorsement of pseudoscientific beliefs, our study focuses on proneness to false memory generation. In this preregistered study, we presented 170 fluent English speakers residing in the USA with a misinformation task aimed at generating false memories. In this task, they first completed an event encoding stage, in which two events were narrated through sequentially presented pictures. One day later, they read a series of sentences relating the same events but which included several inaccurate descriptions aimed at producing a misinformation effect. Finally, we measured the influence of the misinformation manipulation over false memory generation. After completing the misinformation task, participants responded to a questionnaire measuring pseudoscientific beliefs. Our results showed a positive correlation between pseudoscience endorsement and false memory rates, which indicates that the latter might be a key factor influencing susceptibility to pseudoscience. To our knowledge, this is the first study showing a link between the tendency to believe in pseudoscience and variability regarding proneness to develop false memories. Practical implications for the design of new interventions to effectively reduce pseudoscientific beliefs and their negative impact on our society are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"39"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11190135/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141433083","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Trust in automation and the accuracy of human-algorithm teams performing one-to-one face matching tasks. 对自动化的信任和人类-算法团队执行一对一人脸匹配任务的准确性。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-21 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00564-8
Daniel J Carragher, Daniel Sturman, Peter J B Hancock

The human face is commonly used for identity verification. While this task was once exclusively performed by humans, technological advancements have seen automated facial recognition systems (AFRS) integrated into many identification scenarios. Although many state-of-the-art AFRS are exceptionally accurate, they often require human oversight or involvement, such that a human operator actions the final decision. Previously, we have shown that on average, humans assisted by a simulated AFRS (sAFRS) failed to reach the level of accuracy achieved by the same sAFRS alone, due to overturning the system's correct decisions and/or failing to correct sAFRS errors. The aim of the current study was to investigate whether participants' trust in automation was related to their performance on a one-to-one face matching task when assisted by a sAFRS. Participants (n = 160) completed a standard face matching task in two phases: an unassisted baseline phase, and an assisted phase where they were shown the identification decision (95% accurate) made by a sAFRS prior to submitting their own decision. While most participants improved with sAFRS assistance, those with greater relative trust in automation achieved larger gains in performance. However, the average aided performance of participants still failed to reach that of the sAFRS alone, regardless of trust status. Nonetheless, further analysis revealed a small sample of participants who achieved 100% accuracy when aided by the sAFRS. Our results speak to the importance of considering individual differences when selecting employees for roles requiring human-algorithm interaction, including identity verification tasks that incorporate facial recognition technologies.

人脸通常用于身份验证。虽然这项任务曾一度完全由人类完成,但随着技术的进步,自动面部识别系统(AFRS)已被整合到许多身份识别场景中。虽然许多最先进的自动面部识别系统都非常准确,但它们往往需要人类的监督或参与,例如由人类操作员做出最终决定。此前,我们已经证明,由于推翻了系统的正确决定和/或未能纠正 sAFRS 的错误,在模拟 AFRS(sAFRS)的协助下,人类平均无法达到单独使用相同 sAFRS 所达到的准确度水平。本研究的目的是调查参与者对自动化的信任是否与他们在一对一人脸匹配任务中的表现有关。参与者(n = 160)分两个阶段完成了一项标准的人脸匹配任务:无辅助基线阶段和辅助阶段,在辅助阶段,参与者在提交自己的决定之前,会先看到 sAFRS 所做的识别决定(准确率为 95%)。虽然大多数参与者的成绩在 sAFRS 的协助下都有所提高,但那些相对信任自动化的参与者的成绩提高幅度更大。然而,无论信任度如何,参与者的平均辅助绩效仍无法达到单独使用 sAFRS 时的水平。尽管如此,进一步的分析显示,有一小部分参与者在 sAFRS 的帮助下达到了 100% 的准确率。我们的研究结果表明,在为需要人机交互的任务(包括采用面部识别技术的身份验证任务)挑选员工时,考虑个体差异非常重要。
{"title":"Trust in automation and the accuracy of human-algorithm teams performing one-to-one face matching tasks.","authors":"Daniel J Carragher, Daniel Sturman, Peter J B Hancock","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00564-8","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00564-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The human face is commonly used for identity verification. While this task was once exclusively performed by humans, technological advancements have seen automated facial recognition systems (AFRS) integrated into many identification scenarios. Although many state-of-the-art AFRS are exceptionally accurate, they often require human oversight or involvement, such that a human operator actions the final decision. Previously, we have shown that on average, humans assisted by a simulated AFRS (sAFRS) failed to reach the level of accuracy achieved by the same sAFRS alone, due to overturning the system's correct decisions and/or failing to correct sAFRS errors. The aim of the current study was to investigate whether participants' trust in automation was related to their performance on a one-to-one face matching task when assisted by a sAFRS. Participants (n = 160) completed a standard face matching task in two phases: an unassisted baseline phase, and an assisted phase where they were shown the identification decision (95% accurate) made by a sAFRS prior to submitting their own decision. While most participants improved with sAFRS assistance, those with greater relative trust in automation achieved larger gains in performance. However, the average aided performance of participants still failed to reach that of the sAFRS alone, regardless of trust status. Nonetheless, further analysis revealed a small sample of participants who achieved 100% accuracy when aided by the sAFRS. Our results speak to the importance of considering individual differences when selecting employees for roles requiring human-algorithm interaction, including identity verification tasks that incorporate facial recognition technologies.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"41"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11190114/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141433084","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Assessing the relationship between delay discounting and decisions to engage in various protective behaviors during COVID-19. 在 COVID-19 期间,评估延迟折现与参与各种保护行为的决定之间的关系。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-18 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00566-6
Julia G Halilova, Samuel Fynes-Clinton, Donna Rose Addis, R Shayna Rosenbaum

Research suggests that discounting of delayed rewards (i.e., tendency to choose smaller immediate rewards over large later rewards) is a promising target of intervention to encourage compliance with public health measures (PHM), such as vaccination compliance. The effects of delay discounting, however, may differ across the types of PHMs, given that the benefits of vaccination, unlike other PHMs (physical distancing, handwashing, and mask-wearing), are more temporally delayed. Here, we examined whether delay discounting predicts engaging in COVID-19 PHMs in approximately 7,000 participants recruited from 13 countries in June-August 2021. After controlling for demographic and distress variables, delay discounting was a negative predictor of vaccination, but a positive predictor of physical distancing (when restrictions are in place) and handwashing. There was no significant association between delay discounting and frequency of mask-wearing. It is possible that increasing vaccination compliance may require greater emphasis on future benefits of vaccination, whereas promotion of physical distancing and hand hygiene may require greater focus on the present moment. Further research is needed to investigate the nature of this relationship and its implications for public health messaging.

研究表明,延迟奖励折扣(即倾向于选择较小的即时奖励而不是较大的后期奖励)是鼓励遵守公共卫生措施(PHM)(如遵守疫苗接种)的一个有希望的干预目标。然而,由于疫苗接种的益处与其他公共卫生措施(物理距离、洗手和戴口罩)不同,在时间上更具有延迟性,因此延迟折现的效果在不同类型的公共卫生措施中可能有所不同。在此,我们对 2021 年 6 月至 8 月从 13 个国家招募的约 7000 名参与者进行了研究,以了解延迟折扣是否能预测参与 COVID-19 PHMs 的情况。在控制了人口统计学变量和困扰变量后,延迟折现对疫苗接种有负向预测作用,但对身体疏远(当有限制时)和洗手有正向预测作用。延迟折扣与戴口罩的频率之间没有明显联系。要提高疫苗接种的依从性,可能需要更加重视疫苗接种的未来益处,而要促进身体疏远和手部卫生,可能需要更加关注当下。需要进一步研究这种关系的性质及其对公共卫生信息传播的影响。
{"title":"Assessing the relationship between delay discounting and decisions to engage in various protective behaviors during COVID-19.","authors":"Julia G Halilova, Samuel Fynes-Clinton, Donna Rose Addis, R Shayna Rosenbaum","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00566-6","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00566-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research suggests that discounting of delayed rewards (i.e., tendency to choose smaller immediate rewards over large later rewards) is a promising target of intervention to encourage compliance with public health measures (PHM), such as vaccination compliance. The effects of delay discounting, however, may differ across the types of PHMs, given that the benefits of vaccination, unlike other PHMs (physical distancing, handwashing, and mask-wearing), are more temporally delayed. Here, we examined whether delay discounting predicts engaging in COVID-19 PHMs in approximately 7,000 participants recruited from 13 countries in June-August 2021. After controlling for demographic and distress variables, delay discounting was a negative predictor of vaccination, but a positive predictor of physical distancing (when restrictions are in place) and handwashing. There was no significant association between delay discounting and frequency of mask-wearing. It is possible that increasing vaccination compliance may require greater emphasis on future benefits of vaccination, whereas promotion of physical distancing and hand hygiene may require greater focus on the present moment. Further research is needed to investigate the nature of this relationship and its implications for public health messaging.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"38"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11183030/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141421384","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of task structure and confirmation bias in alternative hypotheses evaluation. 任务结构和确认偏差对替代假设评估的影响。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-13 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00560-y
Mandeep K Dhami, Ian K Belton, Peter De Werd, Velichka Hadzhieva, Lars Wicke

We empirically examined the effectiveness of how the Analysis of Competing Hypotheses (ACH) technique structures task information to help reduce confirmation bias (Study 1) and the portrayal of intelligence analysts as suffering from such bias (Study 2). Study 1 (N = 161) showed that individuals presented with hypotheses in rows and evidence items in columns were significantly less likely to demonstrate confirmation bias, whereas those presented with the ACH-style matrix (with hypotheses in columns and evidence items in rows) or a paragraph of text (listing the evidence for each hypothesis) were not less likely to demonstrate bias. The ACH-style matrix also did not confer any benefits regarding increasing sensitivity to evidence credibility. Study 2 showed that the majority of 62 Dutch military analysts did not suffer from confirmation bias and were sensitive to evidence credibility. Finally, neither judgmental coherence nor cognitive reflection differentiated between better or worse performers in the hypotheses evaluation tasks.

我们通过实证研究考察了 "竞合假设分析"(ACH)技术如何构建任务信息,以帮助减少确认偏差(研究 1),以及将情报分析师描述为存在这种偏差(研究 2)的有效性。研究 1(N = 161)显示,将假设分成几行、证据项目分成几列呈现给受试者时,受试者出现确认偏差的可能性明显降低;而将 ACH 式矩阵(将假设分成几列、证据项目分成几行)或一段文字(列出每个假设的证据)呈现给受试者时,受试者出现确认偏差的可能性并没有降低。在提高对证据可信度的敏感度方面,ACH式矩阵也没有带来任何好处。研究 2 表明,62 名荷兰军事分析师中的大多数人都没有确认偏差,而且对证据可信度很敏感。最后,在假设评估任务中,判断连贯性和认知反思都无法区分表现较好或较差的人员。
{"title":"Effects of task structure and confirmation bias in alternative hypotheses evaluation.","authors":"Mandeep K Dhami, Ian K Belton, Peter De Werd, Velichka Hadzhieva, Lars Wicke","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00560-y","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00560-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We empirically examined the effectiveness of how the Analysis of Competing Hypotheses (ACH) technique structures task information to help reduce confirmation bias (Study 1) and the portrayal of intelligence analysts as suffering from such bias (Study 2). Study 1 (N = 161) showed that individuals presented with hypotheses in rows and evidence items in columns were significantly less likely to demonstrate confirmation bias, whereas those presented with the ACH-style matrix (with hypotheses in columns and evidence items in rows) or a paragraph of text (listing the evidence for each hypothesis) were not less likely to demonstrate bias. The ACH-style matrix also did not confer any benefits regarding increasing sensitivity to evidence credibility. Study 2 showed that the majority of 62 Dutch military analysts did not suffer from confirmation bias and were sensitive to evidence credibility. Finally, neither judgmental coherence nor cognitive reflection differentiated between better or worse performers in the hypotheses evaluation tasks.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"37"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11169332/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141311993","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Influence of protective clothing and masks on facial trustworthiness in an investment game: insights from a Chinese population study. 投资游戏中防护服和面具对面部可信度的影响:一项中国人口研究的启示。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-10 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00565-7
Weiping Wang, Zhifan Li, Xin Lin, Yu-Hao P Sun, Zhe Wang, Yong Wang

Facial features are important sources of information about perceived trustworthiness. Masks and protective clothing diminish the visibility of facial cues by either partially concealing the mouth and nose or covering the entire face. During the pandemic, the use of personal protective equipment affected and redefined who trusts whom in society. This study used the classical investment game of interpersonal trust with Chinese participants to explore the impact of occlusion on interpersonal trust. Faces with moderate initial trustworthiness were occluded by a mask or protective clothing in Experiment 1 and were digitally occluded by a square in Experiment 2, and faces with three levels of initial trustworthiness were occluded by a mask in Experiment 3. Results showed that both undergraduates (Experiment 1a) and non-student adults (Experiment 1b) perceived the faces with protective clothing as more trustworthy than faces wearing standard masks and faces not wearing masks. Faces with the top halves showing were perceived as trustworthy as full faces, while faces with the bottom halves showing were perceived as less trustworthy. The effect of masks is weak and complex. Masks reduced participants' trust in faces with high initial trustworthiness, had no effect on faces with low and moderate initial trustworthiness, and only slightly increased the trust of undergraduates in faces with moderate initial trustworthiness. Our findings indicate that the lack of information caused by occlusion and the social significance associated with occlusion collectively affect people's trust behavior in Chinese society. We believe the findings of this study will be useful in elucidating the effects of personal protective equipment usage on perceptions of trustworthiness.

面部特征是感知可信度的重要信息来源。口罩和防护服可以遮住部分口鼻或遮住整个面部,从而降低面部特征的可见度。在大流行病期间,个人防护设备的使用影响并重新定义了社会中谁信任谁。本研究采用经典的人际信任投资游戏,以中国参与者为研究对象,探讨遮盖对人际信任的影响。在实验一中,初始信任度为中等的人脸被面具或防护服遮挡,在实验二中,初始信任度为中等的人脸被正方形数字遮挡;在实验三中,初始信任度为三级的人脸被面具遮挡。结果表明,无论是大学生(实验 1a)还是非大学生成年人(实验 1b),都认为与戴标准面具和不戴面具的人脸相比,戴防护服的人脸更值得信任。露出上半身的面孔与完整的面孔一样被认为是可信的,而露出下半身的面孔则被认为可信度较低。面具的影响既微弱又复杂。面具降低了参与者对初始可信度较高的面孔的信任,对初始可信度较低和中等的面孔没有影响,只是略微增加了大学生对初始可信度中等的面孔的信任。我们的研究结果表明,在中国社会,遮挡造成的信息缺失和与遮挡相关的社会意义共同影响着人们的信任行为。我们相信,本研究的结果将有助于阐明个人防护设备的使用对信任感的影响。
{"title":"Influence of protective clothing and masks on facial trustworthiness in an investment game: insights from a Chinese population study.","authors":"Weiping Wang, Zhifan Li, Xin Lin, Yu-Hao P Sun, Zhe Wang, Yong Wang","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00565-7","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00565-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Facial features are important sources of information about perceived trustworthiness. Masks and protective clothing diminish the visibility of facial cues by either partially concealing the mouth and nose or covering the entire face. During the pandemic, the use of personal protective equipment affected and redefined who trusts whom in society. This study used the classical investment game of interpersonal trust with Chinese participants to explore the impact of occlusion on interpersonal trust. Faces with moderate initial trustworthiness were occluded by a mask or protective clothing in Experiment 1 and were digitally occluded by a square in Experiment 2, and faces with three levels of initial trustworthiness were occluded by a mask in Experiment 3. Results showed that both undergraduates (Experiment 1a) and non-student adults (Experiment 1b) perceived the faces with protective clothing as more trustworthy than faces wearing standard masks and faces not wearing masks. Faces with the top halves showing were perceived as trustworthy as full faces, while faces with the bottom halves showing were perceived as less trustworthy. The effect of masks is weak and complex. Masks reduced participants' trust in faces with high initial trustworthiness, had no effect on faces with low and moderate initial trustworthiness, and only slightly increased the trust of undergraduates in faces with moderate initial trustworthiness. Our findings indicate that the lack of information caused by occlusion and the social significance associated with occlusion collectively affect people's trust behavior in Chinese society. We believe the findings of this study will be useful in elucidating the effects of personal protective equipment usage on perceptions of trustworthiness.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"36"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11164842/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141296920","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1