首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications最新文献

英文 中文
Delay discounting predicts COVID-19 vaccine booster willingness.
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-23 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00609-y
Julia G Halilova, Samuel Fynes-Clinton, Caitlin M Terao, Donna Rose Addis, R Shayna Rosenbaum

Developing ways to predict and encourage vaccine booster uptake are necessary for durable immunity responses. In a multi-nation sample, recruited in June-August 2021, we assessed delay discounting (one's tendency to choose smaller immediate rewards over larger future rewards), COVID-19 vaccination status, demographics, and distress level. Participants who reported being vaccinated were invited back one year later (n = 2547) to report their willingness to receive a booster dose, along with reasons for their decision. After controlling for demographic variables and distress level, a greater tendency to discount future rewards was associated with reduced willingness to receive a booster dose. Thematic coding revealed that the most common reason for booster willingness was protection against COVID-19, and for unwillingness was non-necessity. The results identify delay discounting as a behavioral predictor of booster willingness that may be used to inform tailored approaches to increase booster uptake (e.g., trust in science vs. vaccine mandates).

{"title":"Delay discounting predicts COVID-19 vaccine booster willingness.","authors":"Julia G Halilova, Samuel Fynes-Clinton, Caitlin M Terao, Donna Rose Addis, R Shayna Rosenbaum","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00609-y","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00609-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Developing ways to predict and encourage vaccine booster uptake are necessary for durable immunity responses. In a multi-nation sample, recruited in June-August 2021, we assessed delay discounting (one's tendency to choose smaller immediate rewards over larger future rewards), COVID-19 vaccination status, demographics, and distress level. Participants who reported being vaccinated were invited back one year later (n = 2547) to report their willingness to receive a booster dose, along with reasons for their decision. After controlling for demographic variables and distress level, a greater tendency to discount future rewards was associated with reduced willingness to receive a booster dose. Thematic coding revealed that the most common reason for booster willingness was protection against COVID-19, and for unwillingness was non-necessity. The results identify delay discounting as a behavioral predictor of booster willingness that may be used to inform tailored approaches to increase booster uptake (e.g., trust in science vs. vaccine mandates).</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"10 1","pages":"1"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-01-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11757841/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143025238","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotions in misinformation studies: distinguishing affective state from emotional response and misinformation recognition from acceptance. 错误信息研究中的情绪:区分情感状态与情绪反应、错误信息识别与接受。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-18 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00607-0
Jula Lühring, Apeksha Shetty, Corinna Koschmieder, David Garcia, Annie Waldherr, Hannah Metzler

Prior studies indicate that emotions, particularly high-arousal emotions, may elicit rapid intuitive thinking, thereby decreasing the ability to recognize misinformation. Yet, few studies have distinguished prior affective states from emotional reactions to false news, which could influence belief in falsehoods in different ways. Extending a study by Martel et al. (Cognit Res: Principles Implic 5: 1-20, 2020), we conducted a pre-registered online survey experiment in Austria (N = 422), investigating associations of emotions and discernment of false and real news related to COVID-19. We found no associations of prior affective state with discernment, but observed higher anger and less joy in response to false compared to real news. Exploratory analyses, including automated analyses of open-ended text responses, suggested that anger arose for different reasons in different people depending on their prior beliefs. In our educated and left-leaning sample, higher anger was often related to recognizing the misinformation as such, rather than accepting the false claims. We conclude that studies need to distinguish between prior affective state and emotional response to misinformation and consider individuals' prior beliefs as determinants of emotions.

先前的研究表明,情绪,特别是高唤醒情绪,可能会引发快速的直觉思维,从而降低识别错误信息的能力。然而,很少有研究将先前的情感状态与对虚假新闻的情绪反应区分开来,这可能以不同的方式影响人们对虚假新闻的信念。我们扩展了Martel等人的研究(Cognit Res: Principles Implic 5: 1- 20,2020),在奥地利进行了一项预注册的在线调查实验(N = 422),调查了与COVID-19相关的情绪和虚假新闻和真实新闻的识别之间的关联。我们没有发现先前的情感状态与洞察力的关联,但观察到与真实新闻相比,虚假新闻的反应更高,快乐更少。探索性分析,包括对开放式文本回复的自动分析,表明不同的人产生愤怒的原因不同,这取决于他们先前的信念。在我们的受过教育和左倾的样本中,更高的愤怒往往与认识到错误信息有关,而不是接受错误的说法。我们的结论是,研究需要区分先前的情感状态和对错误信息的情绪反应,并考虑个体的先前信念是情绪的决定因素。
{"title":"Emotions in misinformation studies: distinguishing affective state from emotional response and misinformation recognition from acceptance.","authors":"Jula Lühring, Apeksha Shetty, Corinna Koschmieder, David Garcia, Annie Waldherr, Hannah Metzler","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00607-0","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00607-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prior studies indicate that emotions, particularly high-arousal emotions, may elicit rapid intuitive thinking, thereby decreasing the ability to recognize misinformation. Yet, few studies have distinguished prior affective states from emotional reactions to false news, which could influence belief in falsehoods in different ways. Extending a study by Martel et al. (Cognit Res: Principles Implic 5: 1-20, 2020), we conducted a pre-registered online survey experiment in Austria (N = 422), investigating associations of emotions and discernment of false and real news related to COVID-19. We found no associations of prior affective state with discernment, but observed higher anger and less joy in response to false compared to real news. Exploratory analyses, including automated analyses of open-ended text responses, suggested that anger arose for different reasons in different people depending on their prior beliefs. In our educated and left-leaning sample, higher anger was often related to recognizing the misinformation as such, rather than accepting the false claims. We conclude that studies need to distinguish between prior affective state and emotional response to misinformation and consider individuals' prior beliefs as determinants of emotions.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"82"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-12-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11656008/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142848040","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Acquiring complex concepts through classification versus observation. 通过分类与观察获得复杂概念。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-16 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00608-z
Daniel Corral, Shana K Carpenter

We report six experiments that examine how two essential components of a category-learning paradigm, training and feedback, can be manipulated to maximize learning and transfer of real-world, complex concepts. Some subjects learned through classification and were asked to classify hypothetical experiment scenarios as either true or non-true experiments; others learned through observation, wherein these same scenarios were presented along with the corresponding category label. Additionally, some subjects were presented correct-answer feedback (the category label), whereas others were presented explanation feedback (the correct answer and a detailed explanation). For classification training, this feedback was presented after each classification judgment; for observation training this feedback was presented simultaneously with the hypothetical experiment. After the learning phase, subjects completed a posttest that included one task that involved classifying novel hypothetical scenarios and another task comprising multiple-choice questions about novel scenarios, in which subjects had to specify the issue with the scenario or indicate how it could be fixed. The posttest either occurred immediately after the learning phase (Experiments 1-2), 10 min later (Experiments 3-4), two days later (Experiment 5), or one week later (Experiment 6). Explanation feedback generally led to better learning and transfer than correct-answer feedback. However, although subjects showed clear evidence of learning and transfer, posttest performance did not differ between classification and observation training. Critically, various learning theories and principles (e.g., retrieval practice, generation, active learning) predict a classification advantage. Our results thus call into question the extent to which such theories and principles extend to the transfer of learning.

我们报告了六个实验,研究了如何操纵类别学习范式的两个基本组成部分,训练和反馈,以最大限度地提高现实世界中复杂概念的学习和迁移。一些被试通过分类学习,并被要求将假设的实验场景分为真实实验和非真实实验;其他人通过观察学习,其中这些相同的场景与相应的类别标签一起呈现。此外,一些被试被给予正确答案反馈(类别标签),而另一些被试被给予解释反馈(正确答案和详细解释)。对于分类训练,在每次分类判断后给出该反馈;对于观察训练,这种反馈与假设实验同时呈现。在学习阶段之后,受试者完成了一个后测,其中一个任务涉及对新的假设场景进行分类,另一个任务包括关于新场景的多项选择题,其中受试者必须说明场景中的问题或指出如何解决问题。后测可以在学习阶段后立即(实验1-2)、10分钟后(实验3-4)、两天后(实验5)或一周后(实验6)进行。解释反馈通常比正确答案反馈更能促进学习和迁移。然而,尽管受试者表现出明显的学习和迁移证据,但分类训练和观察训练之间的测试后表现并没有差异。重要的是,各种学习理论和原则(例如,检索实践,生成,主动学习)预测了分类优势。因此,我们的研究结果对这些理论和原则延伸到学习迁移的程度提出了质疑。
{"title":"Acquiring complex concepts through classification versus observation.","authors":"Daniel Corral, Shana K Carpenter","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00608-z","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00608-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We report six experiments that examine how two essential components of a category-learning paradigm, training and feedback, can be manipulated to maximize learning and transfer of real-world, complex concepts. Some subjects learned through classification and were asked to classify hypothetical experiment scenarios as either true or non-true experiments; others learned through observation, wherein these same scenarios were presented along with the corresponding category label. Additionally, some subjects were presented correct-answer feedback (the category label), whereas others were presented explanation feedback (the correct answer and a detailed explanation). For classification training, this feedback was presented after each classification judgment; for observation training this feedback was presented simultaneously with the hypothetical experiment. After the learning phase, subjects completed a posttest that included one task that involved classifying novel hypothetical scenarios and another task comprising multiple-choice questions about novel scenarios, in which subjects had to specify the issue with the scenario or indicate how it could be fixed. The posttest either occurred immediately after the learning phase (Experiments 1-2), 10 min later (Experiments 3-4), two days later (Experiment 5), or one week later (Experiment 6). Explanation feedback generally led to better learning and transfer than correct-answer feedback. However, although subjects showed clear evidence of learning and transfer, posttest performance did not differ between classification and observation training. Critically, various learning theories and principles (e.g., retrieval practice, generation, active learning) predict a classification advantage. Our results thus call into question the extent to which such theories and principles extend to the transfer of learning.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"81"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-12-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11649615/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142829963","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The roles of cognitive dissonance and normative reasoning in attributions of minds to robots. 认知失调和规范推理在机器人思维归因中的作用。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-12 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00604-3
Lewis J Baker, Hongyue Li, Hugo Hammond, Christopher B Jaeger, Anne Havard, Jonathan D Lane, Caroline E Harriott, Daniel T Levin

As a wide variety of intelligent technologies become part of everyday life, researchers have explored how people conceptualize agents that in some ways act and think like living things but are clearly machines. Much of this work draws upon the idea that people readily default to generalizing human-like properties to such agents, and only pare back on these generalizations with added thought. However, recent findings have also documented that people are sometimes initially hesitant to attribute minds to a machine but are more willing to do so with additional thought. In the current experiments, we hypothesized that these attribution-increasing reconsiderations could be spurred by situation-induced cognitive dissonance. In two experiments, participants completed a belief activation exercise designed to induce cognitive dissonance (writing arguments for or against prominent beliefs), viewed a video of an ambiguously intentional robot, and completed measures of cognitive dissonance. In both experiments, cognitive dissonance was associated with increased attributions of mind to the robot. Our findings provide evidence that people sometimes increase their attributions of minds when experiencing cognitive conflict, but also that activation of change-inducing concepts may impact attributions of a mind without producing conscious cognitive conflict in participants.

随着各种各样的智能技术成为日常生活的一部分,研究人员已经探索了人们如何将智能体概念化,这些智能体在某些方面像生物一样行动和思考,但显然是机器。这项工作的大部分都是基于这样一种观点,即人们很容易默认地将类似人类的属性概括为这些代理,并且只会在这些概括中添加更多的想法。然而,最近的研究结果也表明,人们有时一开始会犹豫是否要把思维归因于机器,但经过额外的思考后,他们更愿意这样做。在当前的实验中,我们假设这些增加归因的重新考虑可能是由情境诱发的认知失调所激发的。在两个实验中,参与者完成了一个旨在诱发认知失调的信念激活练习(写下支持或反对重要信念的论据),观看了一个意图模糊的机器人的视频,并完成了认知失调的测量。在这两个实验中,认知失调与机器人的心理属性增加有关。我们的研究结果提供了证据,证明人们在经历认知冲突时有时会增加他们的思维归因,但也证明了变化诱导概念的激活可能会影响思维归因,而不会在参与者中产生有意识的认知冲突。
{"title":"The roles of cognitive dissonance and normative reasoning in attributions of minds to robots.","authors":"Lewis J Baker, Hongyue Li, Hugo Hammond, Christopher B Jaeger, Anne Havard, Jonathan D Lane, Caroline E Harriott, Daniel T Levin","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00604-3","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00604-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>As a wide variety of intelligent technologies become part of everyday life, researchers have explored how people conceptualize agents that in some ways act and think like living things but are clearly machines. Much of this work draws upon the idea that people readily default to generalizing human-like properties to such agents, and only pare back on these generalizations with added thought. However, recent findings have also documented that people are sometimes initially hesitant to attribute minds to a machine but are more willing to do so with additional thought. In the current experiments, we hypothesized that these attribution-increasing reconsiderations could be spurred by situation-induced cognitive dissonance. In two experiments, participants completed a belief activation exercise designed to induce cognitive dissonance (writing arguments for or against prominent beliefs), viewed a video of an ambiguously intentional robot, and completed measures of cognitive dissonance. In both experiments, cognitive dissonance was associated with increased attributions of mind to the robot. Our findings provide evidence that people sometimes increase their attributions of minds when experiencing cognitive conflict, but also that activation of change-inducing concepts may impact attributions of a mind without producing conscious cognitive conflict in participants.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"80"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11635073/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142814558","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Older adults' recognition of medical terminology in hospital noise. 老年人对医院噪声中医学术语的识别。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-05 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00606-1
Tessa Bent, Melissa Baese-Berk, Brian Puckett, Erica Ryherd, Sydney Perry, Natalie A Manley

Word identification accuracy is modulated by many factors including linguistic characteristics of words (frequent vs. infrequent), listening environment (noisy vs. quiet), and listener-related differences (older vs. younger). Nearly, all studies investigating these factors use high-familiarity words and noise signals that are either energetic maskers (e.g., white noise) or informational maskers composed of competing talkers (e.g., multitalker babble). Here, we expand on these findings by examining younger and older listeners' speech-in-noise perception for words varying in both frequency and familiarity within a simulated hospital noise that has important non-speech information. The method was inspired by the real-world challenges aging patients can face in understanding less familiar medical terminology used by healthcare professionals in noisy hospital environments. Word familiarity data from older and young adults were collected for 800 medically related terms. Familiarity ratings were highly correlated between the two age groups. Older adults' transcription accuracy for sentences with medical terminology that vary in their familiarity and frequency was assessed across four listening conditions: hospital noise, speech-shaped noise, amplitude-modulated speech-shaped noise, and quiet. Listeners were less accurate in noise conditions than in a quiet condition and were more impacted by hospital noise than either speech-shaped noise. Sentences with low-familiarity and low-frequency medical words combined with hospital noise were particularly detrimental for older adults compared to younger adults. The results impact our theoretical understanding of speech perception in noise and highlight real-world consequences of older adults' difficulties with speech-in-noise and specifically noise containing competing, non-speech information.

单词识别的准确性受到许多因素的调节,包括单词的语言特征(频繁与不频繁),听力环境(嘈杂与安静)以及听者相关的差异(年龄与年龄)。几乎所有调查这些因素的研究都使用高度熟悉的单词和噪声信号,这些信号要么是能量掩蔽物(如白噪声),要么是由相互竞争的说话者组成的信息掩蔽物(如多说话者的胡言乱语)。在这里,我们对这些发现进行了扩展,通过检查年轻和年长的听众在模拟的医院噪声中对具有重要非言语信息的单词的频率和熟悉度变化的语音感知。该方法的灵感来自于现实世界中老年患者在嘈杂的医院环境中理解医疗专业人员使用的不太熟悉的医学术语时可能面临的挑战。从老年人和年轻人中收集了800个医学相关术语的词汇熟悉度数据。熟悉度评分在两个年龄组之间高度相关。老年人对医学术语句子的转录准确性在四种听力条件下进行了评估:医院噪音、语音噪音、调幅语音噪音和安静。听者在嘈杂的环境下比在安静的环境下更不准确,而医院的噪音比两种语音噪音更容易影响听者的判断。与年轻人相比,低熟悉度和低频率医学词汇加上医院噪音的句子对老年人尤其有害。研究结果影响了我们对噪声环境下言语感知的理论理解,并突出了老年人在噪声环境下言语感知困难的现实后果,特别是包含竞争性非言语信息的噪声。
{"title":"Older adults' recognition of medical terminology in hospital noise.","authors":"Tessa Bent, Melissa Baese-Berk, Brian Puckett, Erica Ryherd, Sydney Perry, Natalie A Manley","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00606-1","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00606-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Word identification accuracy is modulated by many factors including linguistic characteristics of words (frequent vs. infrequent), listening environment (noisy vs. quiet), and listener-related differences (older vs. younger). Nearly, all studies investigating these factors use high-familiarity words and noise signals that are either energetic maskers (e.g., white noise) or informational maskers composed of competing talkers (e.g., multitalker babble). Here, we expand on these findings by examining younger and older listeners' speech-in-noise perception for words varying in both frequency and familiarity within a simulated hospital noise that has important non-speech information. The method was inspired by the real-world challenges aging patients can face in understanding less familiar medical terminology used by healthcare professionals in noisy hospital environments. Word familiarity data from older and young adults were collected for 800 medically related terms. Familiarity ratings were highly correlated between the two age groups. Older adults' transcription accuracy for sentences with medical terminology that vary in their familiarity and frequency was assessed across four listening conditions: hospital noise, speech-shaped noise, amplitude-modulated speech-shaped noise, and quiet. Listeners were less accurate in noise conditions than in a quiet condition and were more impacted by hospital noise than either speech-shaped noise. Sentences with low-familiarity and low-frequency medical words combined with hospital noise were particularly detrimental for older adults compared to younger adults. The results impact our theoretical understanding of speech perception in noise and highlight real-world consequences of older adults' difficulties with speech-in-noise and specifically noise containing competing, non-speech information.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"79"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-12-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11621266/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142787411","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Development of a self-report measure of GPS uses and its relationship with environmental knowledge and self-efficacy and pleasure in exploring. GPS使用的自我报告测量的发展及其与环境知识、自我效能和探索乐趣的关系。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-28 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00605-2
L Miola, V Muffato, A Boldrini, F Pazzaglia, C Meneghetti

The widely utilized Global Positioning System (GPS) plays a crucial role in everyday navigation. The literature has predominantly focused on GPS use for reaching destinations rather than exploring its various strategic applications and relations with individual factors. The current paper is intended to develop a GPS Uses Scale assessing a variety of GPS uses for wayfinding and other GPS uses (Study 1). We also examine whether GPS uses are related to gender, age, self-efficacy and pleasure in exploring, dependence on GPS devices, and environment knowledge (Study 2). In Study 1, 365 participants completed the new GPS Uses Scale and the McGill GPS questionnaire, for assessing validity. Results from the confirmatory factor analysis confirmed a structure as five-level factors, good reliability, and validity. In Study 2, 200 participants completed the GPS Uses Scale, self-efficacy and pleasure in exploring scale, GPS dependence scale, and a sketch map task after learning a virtual city from a video. Results from the linear model showed that those who use GPS for strategic purposes reported higher self-efficacy and pleasure in exploring as well as dependence on GPS. Moreover, those who use GPS for orientation purposes reported higher dependency on GPS and had higher scores on the map task (environment knowledge). Men were less likely to use GPS for orientation. The present paper outlines the importance of assessing the various uses of GPS, suggesting self-efficacy and dependence on GPS, and contributes to its strategic use.

广泛应用的全球定位系统(GPS)在日常导航中起着至关重要的作用。文献主要集中在GPS用于到达目的地,而不是探索其各种战略应用和与个人因素的关系。本论文旨在开发一种GPS使用量表,评估GPS在寻路和其他GPS用途中的各种用途(研究1)。我们还研究了GPS使用是否与性别、年龄、自我效能和探索乐趣、对GPS设备的依赖以及环境知识有关(研究2)。在研究1中,365名参与者完成了新的GPS使用量表和麦吉尔GPS问卷,以评估效度。验证性因子分析结果证实结构为五层次因子,信度、效度较好。在研究2中,200名参与者在通过视频学习虚拟城市后完成了GPS使用量表、自我效能感和探索乐趣量表、GPS依赖量表和草图任务。线性模型的结果显示,那些将GPS用于战略目的的人报告了更高的自我效能感和探索乐趣以及对GPS的依赖。此外,那些使用GPS定位的人对GPS的依赖程度更高,在地图任务(环境知识)上得分更高。男性不太可能使用GPS定位。本文概述了评估GPS各种用途的重要性,表明了对GPS的自我效能和依赖,并有助于其战略使用。
{"title":"Development of a self-report measure of GPS uses and its relationship with environmental knowledge and self-efficacy and pleasure in exploring.","authors":"L Miola, V Muffato, A Boldrini, F Pazzaglia, C Meneghetti","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00605-2","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00605-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The widely utilized Global Positioning System (GPS) plays a crucial role in everyday navigation. The literature has predominantly focused on GPS use for reaching destinations rather than exploring its various strategic applications and relations with individual factors. The current paper is intended to develop a GPS Uses Scale assessing a variety of GPS uses for wayfinding and other GPS uses (Study 1). We also examine whether GPS uses are related to gender, age, self-efficacy and pleasure in exploring, dependence on GPS devices, and environment knowledge (Study 2). In Study 1, 365 participants completed the new GPS Uses Scale and the McGill GPS questionnaire, for assessing validity. Results from the confirmatory factor analysis confirmed a structure as five-level factors, good reliability, and validity. In Study 2, 200 participants completed the GPS Uses Scale, self-efficacy and pleasure in exploring scale, GPS dependence scale, and a sketch map task after learning a virtual city from a video. Results from the linear model showed that those who use GPS for strategic purposes reported higher self-efficacy and pleasure in exploring as well as dependence on GPS. Moreover, those who use GPS for orientation purposes reported higher dependency on GPS and had higher scores on the map task (environment knowledge). Men were less likely to use GPS for orientation. The present paper outlines the importance of assessing the various uses of GPS, suggesting self-efficacy and dependence on GPS, and contributes to its strategic use.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"78"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11604907/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142751925","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Fixation durations on familiar items are longer due to attenuation of exploration. 由于探索的减弱,对熟悉项目的固定持续时间更长。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-14 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00602-5
Tal Nahari, Eran Eldar, Yoni Pertzov

Previous studies have shown that fixations on familiar stimuli tend to be longer than on unfamiliar stimuli, theorized to be a result of retrieval of information from memory. We hypothesize that extended fixations are due to a lesser need to explore an already familiar stimulus. Participant's gaze was tracked as they tried to encode or retrieve a familiar face displayed either alone or alongside other unfamiliar faces. Regardless of the memory task (encodingretrieval), longer fixation durations were observed when a single familiar face was presented alone, and not when presented among unfamiliar ones. Thus, fixations were not prolonged when it was possible to explore other, unfamiliar stimuli. We conclude that prolonged fixations on familiar stimuli reflect a lesser need to explore an already familiar percept. The results underscore how memory representations influence active sensing, yielding fresh insights into efficient deployment of attention resources. We conclude that fixation durations could be used in applied memory detection tests, preferably together with other measures and when the familiar stimulus is presented alone.

以往的研究表明,对熟悉刺激物的凝视时间往往比对陌生刺激物的凝视时间长,这被认为是从记忆中检索信息的结果。我们的假设是,对已经熟悉的刺激物进行探索的需要较少,从而导致固着时间延长。当受试者试图编码或检索一张单独或与其他陌生面孔同时显示的熟悉面孔时,他们的视线会被跟踪。无论记忆任务是什么(编码/检索),当一张熟悉的面孔单独出现时,都会观察到较长的凝视持续时间,而当熟悉的面孔与陌生面孔同时出现时,凝视持续时间则不会延长。因此,当可以探索其他不熟悉的刺激物时,固着时间不会延长。我们的结论是,在熟悉的刺激物上的定格时间延长,反映出探索已经熟悉的感知的需求较小。这些结果强调了记忆表征是如何影响主动感知的,为有效调配注意力资源提供了新的见解。我们的结论是,在应用记忆检测测试中可以使用定影持续时间,最好是与其他测量方法一起使用,并且在熟悉的刺激单独出现时使用。
{"title":"Fixation durations on familiar items are longer due to attenuation of exploration.","authors":"Tal Nahari, Eran Eldar, Yoni Pertzov","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00602-5","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00602-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous studies have shown that fixations on familiar stimuli tend to be longer than on unfamiliar stimuli, theorized to be a result of retrieval of information from memory. We hypothesize that extended fixations are due to a lesser need to explore an already familiar stimulus. Participant's gaze was tracked as they tried to encode or retrieve a familiar face displayed either alone or alongside other unfamiliar faces. Regardless of the memory task (encodingretrieval), longer fixation durations were observed when a single familiar face was presented alone, and not when presented among unfamiliar ones. Thus, fixations were not prolonged when it was possible to explore other, unfamiliar stimuli. We conclude that prolonged fixations on familiar stimuli reflect a lesser need to explore an already familiar percept. The results underscore how memory representations influence active sensing, yielding fresh insights into efficient deployment of attention resources. We conclude that fixation durations could be used in applied memory detection tests, preferably together with other measures and when the familiar stimulus is presented alone.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"77"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11564497/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630248","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Different facets of age perception in people with developmental prosopagnosia and "super-recognisers". 发育性前睑闭锁症患者和 "超级识别者 "的不同年龄感知。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-13 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00603-4
Janice Attard-Johnson, Olivia Dark, Ebony Murray, Sarah Bate

The interplay between facial age and facial identity is evident from several scenarios experienced in daily life, such as when recognising a face several decades after the last exposure. However, the link between age and identity processing, and how age perception abilities might diverge in individuals with different face processing abilities, has scarcely been considered. Furthermore, the approach used to test age perception ability may also influence outcome, but the effect of different paradigms on performance is not yet known. Across three studies, we compare super-recognisers (SRs), people with developmental prosopagnosia (DPs), and a group of neurotypical controls, on three age perception paradigms. There were no differences on the numeric age estimation task (i.e. providing precise age estimates for a series of faces; Study 1), and numeric age estimation task with added noise-distortion to stimuli (Study 2). However, SRs were more accurate when instructed to classify ambient faces as either over- or under- the age of 18 compared to both DPs and controls (Study 3). Thus, there may be nuanced differences in age processing which can be tapped into using separate paradigms; however, given that the difference is only with SRs it remains unclear to what extent these are linked to facial identity processing.

脸部年龄与脸部身份之间的相互作用在日常生活中的一些场景中显而易见,例如在最后一次接触一张脸的几十年后再去识别它。然而,关于年龄与身份处理之间的联系,以及年龄感知能力如何在具有不同面部处理能力的个体中产生差异,却很少有人考虑过。此外,用于测试年龄感知能力的方法也可能影响结果,但不同范式对结果的影响尚不清楚。在三项研究中,我们比较了超级识别者(SRs)、发展性面容失认症患者(DPs)和一组神经典型对照组在三种年龄感知范式上的表现。在数字年龄估计任务(即为一系列面孔提供精确的年龄估计;研究 1)和在刺激物中添加噪音失真的数字年龄估计任务(研究 2)上没有差异。然而,与民主党人和对照组相比,当被要求将环境中的面孔划分为超过或低于18岁时,SR的准确性更高(研究3)。因此,在年龄处理方面可能存在细微差别,可以使用不同的范式来挖掘这种差异;但是,鉴于这种差异仅出现在 SR 身上,因此仍不清楚这些差异在多大程度上与面部身份处理有关。
{"title":"Different facets of age perception in people with developmental prosopagnosia and \"super-recognisers\".","authors":"Janice Attard-Johnson, Olivia Dark, Ebony Murray, Sarah Bate","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00603-4","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00603-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The interplay between facial age and facial identity is evident from several scenarios experienced in daily life, such as when recognising a face several decades after the last exposure. However, the link between age and identity processing, and how age perception abilities might diverge in individuals with different face processing abilities, has scarcely been considered. Furthermore, the approach used to test age perception ability may also influence outcome, but the effect of different paradigms on performance is not yet known. Across three studies, we compare super-recognisers (SRs), people with developmental prosopagnosia (DPs), and a group of neurotypical controls, on three age perception paradigms. There were no differences on the numeric age estimation task (i.e. providing precise age estimates for a series of faces; Study 1), and numeric age estimation task with added noise-distortion to stimuli (Study 2). However, SRs were more accurate when instructed to classify ambient faces as either over- or under- the age of 18 compared to both DPs and controls (Study 3). Thus, there may be nuanced differences in age processing which can be tapped into using separate paradigms; however, given that the difference is only with SRs it remains unclear to what extent these are linked to facial identity processing.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"76"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11557776/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630246","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Self-evaluations and the language of the beholder: objective performance and language solidarity predict L2 and L1 self-evaluations in bilingual adults. 自我评价与被观察者的语言:客观表现和语言团结可预测双语成人的 L2 和 L1 自我评价。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-04 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00592-4
Esteban Hernández-Rivera, Alessia Kalogeris, Mehrgol Tiv, Debra Titone

People are often asked to self-evaluate their abilities, and these evaluations may not always reflect objective reality. Here, we investigated this issue for bilingual adults' self-evaluations of language proficiency and usage. We specifically examined how people's self-reported language solidarity impacted their first- (L1) and second-language (L2) self-evaluations, while statistically controlling for their objective language performance (i.e. LexTALE). We also investigated whether this impact varied for value-laden evaluations (e.g. how "good" am I at my L2) vs. usage-based evaluations (e.g. how often do I use my L2) for two sociolinguistically distinct groups (i.e. English-L1 speakers vs. French-L1 speakers in Montreal). Starting with value-laden self-evaluations, we found that French-L1 speakers with more favourable L2-English solidarity tended to underestimate their objective L2 ability, whereas French-L1 speakers with less favourable L2-English solidarity more accurately estimated their objective L2 ability. In contrast, English-L1 speakers with more favourable L2-French solidarity more accurately estimated their objective L2 ability than those with less favourable L2-French solidarity who underestimated their L2-French abilities. Turning to usage-based self-evaluations, we found that participants' self-evaluations were generally more accurate reflections of their performance, in a manner that was less affected by individual differences in self-reported language solidarity. This implies that language solidarity (or perhaps language attitudes more generally) can implicitly or explicitly impact bilingual adults' language self-evaluations when these evaluations are value-laden. These data suggest that people's language attitudes can bias how they perceive their abilities, although self-evaluations based on language use may be less susceptible to bias than those that are value-laden. These data have implications for the study of language and cognition that depend on self-assessments of individual differences and are relevant to work on how people self-assess their abilities generally.

人们经常被要求对自己的能力进行自我评价,而这些评价可能并不总是反映客观现实。在这里,我们研究了双语成人对语言能力和使用的自我评价。在对客观语言表现(即 LexTALE)进行统计控制的同时,我们特别考察了人们自我报告的语言团结程度对其第一语言(L1)和第二语言(L2)自我评价的影响。我们还研究了在两个社会语言不同的群体中(即在蒙特利尔讲英语(L1)的人与讲法语(L1)的人),这种影响是否会因价值评价(如我的 L2 有多 "好")和使用评价(如我多经常使用我的 L2)而有所不同。从价值导向的自我评价入手,我们发现,L2-E1团结度较高的讲法语-L1的人往往低估了自己的客观L2能力,而L2-E1团结度较低的讲法语-L1的人则更准确地估计了自己的客观L2能力。与此相反,L2-F1团结程度较高的英语-L1学习者比L2-F1团结程度较低的英语-L1学习者更准确地估计了自己的客观L2能力,后者则低估了自己的L2-F2能力。至于以使用为基础的自我评价,我们发现参与者的自我评价通常更准确地反映了他们的表现,而这种方式受自我报告的语言团结程度的个体差异的影响较小。这意味着,语言团结(或者更普遍的语言态度)会或隐或显地影响双语成人的语言自我评价,如果这些评价是有价值的。这些数据表明,人们的语言态度会对他们如何看待自己的能力产生偏差,尽管基于语言使用的自我评价可能比那些带有价值取向的自我评价更不容易受到偏差的影响。这些数据对依赖于个体差异自我评估的语言和认知研究具有重要意义,同时也与人们如何对自身能力进行自我评估的研究相关。
{"title":"Self-evaluations and the language of the beholder: objective performance and language solidarity predict L2 and L1 self-evaluations in bilingual adults.","authors":"Esteban Hernández-Rivera, Alessia Kalogeris, Mehrgol Tiv, Debra Titone","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00592-4","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00592-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People are often asked to self-evaluate their abilities, and these evaluations may not always reflect objective reality. Here, we investigated this issue for bilingual adults' self-evaluations of language proficiency and usage. We specifically examined how people's self-reported language solidarity impacted their first- (L1) and second-language (L2) self-evaluations, while statistically controlling for their objective language performance (i.e. LexTALE). We also investigated whether this impact varied for value-laden evaluations (e.g. how \"good\" am I at my L2) vs. usage-based evaluations (e.g. how often do I use my L2) for two sociolinguistically distinct groups (i.e. English-L1 speakers vs. French-L1 speakers in Montreal). Starting with value-laden self-evaluations, we found that French-L1 speakers with more favourable L2-English solidarity tended to underestimate their objective L2 ability, whereas French-L1 speakers with less favourable L2-English solidarity more accurately estimated their objective L2 ability. In contrast, English-L1 speakers with more favourable L2-French solidarity more accurately estimated their objective L2 ability than those with less favourable L2-French solidarity who underestimated their L2-French abilities. Turning to usage-based self-evaluations, we found that participants' self-evaluations were generally more accurate reflections of their performance, in a manner that was less affected by individual differences in self-reported language solidarity. This implies that language solidarity (or perhaps language attitudes more generally) can implicitly or explicitly impact bilingual adults' language self-evaluations when these evaluations are value-laden. These data suggest that people's language attitudes can bias how they perceive their abilities, although self-evaluations based on language use may be less susceptible to bias than those that are value-laden. These data have implications for the study of language and cognition that depend on self-assessments of individual differences and are relevant to work on how people self-assess their abilities generally.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"75"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11535130/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142569832","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction: Distress reactions and susceptibility to misinformation for an analogue trauma event. 更正:模拟创伤事件的苦恼反应和对错误信息的易感性。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-29 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00601-6
Prerika R Sharma, Emily R Spearing, Kimberley A Wade, Laura Jobson
{"title":"Correction: Distress reactions and susceptibility to misinformation for an analogue trauma event.","authors":"Prerika R Sharma, Emily R Spearing, Kimberley A Wade, Laura Jobson","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00601-6","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00601-6","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"72"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11519261/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142523302","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1