首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications最新文献

英文 中文
Anecdotes impact medical decisions even when presented with statistical information or decision aids. 即使有统计信息或决策辅助工具,轶事也会影响医疗决策。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-26 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00577-3
Emily N Line, Sara Jaramillo, Micah Goldwater, Zachary Horne

People are inundated with popular press reports about medical research concerning what is healthy, get advice from doctors, and hear personal anecdotes. How do people integrate conflicting anecdotal and statistical information when making medical decisions? In four experiments (N = 4126), we tested how people use conflicting information to judge the efficacy of artificial and real medical treatments. Participants read an anecdote from someone in a clinical trial, or who had undergone a medical treatment previously, for whom the medical treatment was ineffective. We found that reading anecdotes for either artificial or real medical treatments shifted participants' beliefs about the efficacy of a medical treatment. We observed this result even when the anecdote was uninformative, was paired with an icon array, or when participants were provided with thorough medical decision aids about reproductive health procedures. Our findings highlight the pervasive effect of anecdotes on medical decision making.

人们经常会看到大众媒体报道有关什么是健康的医学研究,得到医生的建议,听到个人轶事。在做出医疗决策时,人们如何整合相互矛盾的传闻和统计信息?在四个实验中(N = 4126),我们测试了人们如何利用相互矛盾的信息来判断人工和真实医疗方法的疗效。实验参与者阅读了临床试验中的某人或曾接受过某种医疗的某人的轶事,对他们来说,该医疗是无效的。我们发现,阅读人工或真实医疗方法的轶事会改变参与者对医疗方法疗效的看法。即使轶事没有提供信息、与图标阵列搭配在一起,或者参与者获得了有关生殖健康程序的全面医疗决策辅助工具,我们也能观察到这一结果。我们的研究结果凸显了轶事对医疗决策的普遍影响。
{"title":"Anecdotes impact medical decisions even when presented with statistical information or decision aids.","authors":"Emily N Line, Sara Jaramillo, Micah Goldwater, Zachary Horne","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00577-3","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00577-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People are inundated with popular press reports about medical research concerning what is healthy, get advice from doctors, and hear personal anecdotes. How do people integrate conflicting anecdotal and statistical information when making medical decisions? In four experiments (N = 4126), we tested how people use conflicting information to judge the efficacy of artificial and real medical treatments. Participants read an anecdote from someone in a clinical trial, or who had undergone a medical treatment previously, for whom the medical treatment was ineffective. We found that reading anecdotes for either artificial or real medical treatments shifted participants' beliefs about the efficacy of a medical treatment. We observed this result even when the anecdote was uninformative, was paired with an icon array, or when participants were provided with thorough medical decision aids about reproductive health procedures. Our findings highlight the pervasive effect of anecdotes on medical decision making.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"51"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11345347/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142056889","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correcting fake news headlines after repeated exposure: memory and belief accuracy in younger and older adults. 反复接触后纠正假新闻标题:年轻人和老年人的记忆和信念准确性。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-26 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00585-3
Paige L Kemp, Vanessa M Loaiza, Colleen M Kelley, Christopher N Wahlheim

The efficacy of fake news corrections in improving memory and belief accuracy may depend on how often adults see false information before it is corrected. Two experiments tested the competing predictions that repeating fake news before corrections will either impair or improve memory and belief accuracy. These experiments also examined whether fake news exposure effects would differ for younger and older adults due to age-related differences in the recollection of contextual details. Younger and older adults read real and fake news headlines that appeared once or thrice. Next, they identified fake news corrections among real news headlines. Later, recognition and cued recall tests assessed memory for real news, fake news, if corrections occurred, and beliefs in retrieved details. Repeating fake news increased detection and remembering of corrections, correct real news retrieval, and erroneous fake news retrieval. No age differences emerged for detection of corrections, but younger adults remembered corrections better than older adults. At test, correct fake news retrieval for earlier-detected corrections was associated with better real news retrieval. This benefit did not differ between age groups in recognition but was greater for younger than older adults in cued recall. When detected corrections were not remembered at test, repeated fake news increased memory errors. Overall, both age groups believed correctly retrieved real news more than erroneously retrieved fake news to a similar degree. These findings suggest that fake news repetition effects on subsequent memory accuracy depended on age differences in recollection-based retrieval of fake news and that it was corrected.

假新闻更正在改善记忆和信念准确性方面的效果可能取决于成年人在假新闻更正之前看到假消息的频率。有两个实验测试了相互竞争的预测,即在更正前重复假新闻会损害或改善记忆和信念的准确性。这些实验还检验了年轻人和老年人接触假新闻的效果是否会因年龄相关的上下文细节回忆差异而不同。年轻人和老年人阅读了一次或三次出现的真假新闻标题。接下来,他们在真实新闻标题中识别出假新闻更正。随后,识别和提示回忆测试评估了对真实新闻、假新闻、是否出现更正以及对检索到的细节的信念的记忆。重复假新闻增加了对更正的检测和记忆、正确的真实新闻检索和错误的假新闻检索。在发现更正方面没有出现年龄差异,但年轻人对更正的记忆好于老年人。在测试中,对早期发现的更正进行正确的假新闻检索与更好的真实新闻检索相关。这种益处在不同年龄组的识别中没有差异,但在诱导回忆中,年轻成人的益处大于老年人。如果在测试时没有记住检测到的更正,重复假新闻会增加记忆错误。总体而言,两个年龄组的人对正确检索到的真实新闻的相信程度都高于错误检索到的假新闻。这些研究结果表明,假新闻重复对后续记忆准确性的影响取决于基于回忆的假新闻检索的年龄差异以及假新闻被纠正的情况。
{"title":"Correcting fake news headlines after repeated exposure: memory and belief accuracy in younger and older adults.","authors":"Paige L Kemp, Vanessa M Loaiza, Colleen M Kelley, Christopher N Wahlheim","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00585-3","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00585-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The efficacy of fake news corrections in improving memory and belief accuracy may depend on how often adults see false information before it is corrected. Two experiments tested the competing predictions that repeating fake news before corrections will either impair or improve memory and belief accuracy. These experiments also examined whether fake news exposure effects would differ for younger and older adults due to age-related differences in the recollection of contextual details. Younger and older adults read real and fake news headlines that appeared once or thrice. Next, they identified fake news corrections among real news headlines. Later, recognition and cued recall tests assessed memory for real news, fake news, if corrections occurred, and beliefs in retrieved details. Repeating fake news increased detection and remembering of corrections, correct real news retrieval, and erroneous fake news retrieval. No age differences emerged for detection of corrections, but younger adults remembered corrections better than older adults. At test, correct fake news retrieval for earlier-detected corrections was associated with better real news retrieval. This benefit did not differ between age groups in recognition but was greater for younger than older adults in cued recall. When detected corrections were not remembered at test, repeated fake news increased memory errors. Overall, both age groups believed correctly retrieved real news more than erroneously retrieved fake news to a similar degree. These findings suggest that fake news repetition effects on subsequent memory accuracy depended on age differences in recollection-based retrieval of fake news and that it was corrected.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"55"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11345346/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142056890","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distress reactions and susceptibility to misinformation for an analogue trauma event. 模拟创伤事件的苦恼反应和对错误信息的易感性。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-26 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00582-6
Prerika R Sharma, Emily R Spearing, Kimberley A Wade, Laura Jobson

Accuracy of memory is critical in legal and clinical contexts. These contexts are often linked with high levels of emotional distress and social sources that can provide potentially distorting information about stressful events. This study investigated how distress was associated with susceptibility to misinformation about a trauma analogue event. We employed an experimental design whereby in Phase 1, participants (N = 243, aged 20-72, 122 females, 117 males, 4 gender diverse) watched a trauma film (car crash) and heard an audio summary that contained misinformation (misled items), true reminders (consistent items), and no reminders (control items) about the film. Participants rated their total distress, and symptoms of avoidance, intrusions, and hyperarousal, in response to the film. They then completed cued recall, recognition, and source memory tasks. One week later in Phase 2, participants (N = 199) completed the same measures again. Generalised linear mixed models were used. A significant misinformation effect was found, and importantly, participants with higher distress levels showed a smaller misinformation effect, owing to especially poor memory for consistent items compared to their less distressed counterparts. Distress was also associated with improved source memory for misled items. Avoidance of the film's reminders was associated with a smaller misinformation effect during immediate retrieval and a larger misinformation effect during delayed retrieval. Findings suggest that distress is associated with decreased susceptibility to misinformation in some cases, but also associated with poorer memory accuracy in general. Limitations are discussed, and the need for further research is highlighted.

在法律和临床环境中,记忆的准确性至关重要。这些情境往往与高度的情绪困扰和社会来源有关,而社会来源可能会提供有关压力事件的失真信息。本研究调查了情绪困扰与对创伤模拟事件错误信息的易感性之间的关系。我们采用了一种实验设计,在第一阶段,参与者(人数 = 243,年龄在 20-72 岁之间,女性 122 人,男性 117 人,4 人性别不同)观看了一部创伤影片(车祸),并听到了一段音频摘要,其中包含关于该影片的错误信息(误导项目)、真实提醒(一致项目)和无提醒(对照项目)。受试者对自己因影片而产生的全部痛苦以及回避、侵扰和过度焦虑症状进行评分。然后,他们完成了提示回忆、识别和来源记忆任务。一周后的第二阶段,参与者(N = 199)再次完成相同的测量。研究采用了广义线性混合模型。结果发现,错误信息效应非常明显,而且重要的是,与受困扰程度较低的受试者相比,受困扰程度较高的受试者对一致项目的记忆能力特别差,因此错误信息效应较小。苦恼还与误导项目的源记忆改善有关。在即时检索过程中,回避电影的提醒与较小的误导效应相关,而在延迟检索过程中,回避电影的提醒与较大的误导效应相关。研究结果表明,在某些情况下,痛苦与对错误信息的易感性降低有关,但总体上也与记忆准确性较差有关。本文讨论了研究的局限性,并强调了进一步研究的必要性。
{"title":"Distress reactions and susceptibility to misinformation for an analogue trauma event.","authors":"Prerika R Sharma, Emily R Spearing, Kimberley A Wade, Laura Jobson","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00582-6","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00582-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Accuracy of memory is critical in legal and clinical contexts. These contexts are often linked with high levels of emotional distress and social sources that can provide potentially distorting information about stressful events. This study investigated how distress was associated with susceptibility to misinformation about a trauma analogue event. We employed an experimental design whereby in Phase 1, participants (N = 243, aged 20-72, 122 females, 117 males, 4 gender diverse) watched a trauma film (car crash) and heard an audio summary that contained misinformation (misled items), true reminders (consistent items), and no reminders (control items) about the film. Participants rated their total distress, and symptoms of avoidance, intrusions, and hyperarousal, in response to the film. They then completed cued recall, recognition, and source memory tasks. One week later in Phase 2, participants (N = 199) completed the same measures again. Generalised linear mixed models were used. A significant misinformation effect was found, and importantly, participants with higher distress levels showed a smaller misinformation effect, owing to especially poor memory for consistent items compared to their less distressed counterparts. Distress was also associated with improved source memory for misled items. Avoidance of the film's reminders was associated with a smaller misinformation effect during immediate retrieval and a larger misinformation effect during delayed retrieval. Findings suggest that distress is associated with decreased susceptibility to misinformation in some cases, but also associated with poorer memory accuracy in general. Limitations are discussed, and the need for further research is highlighted.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"53"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11345351/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142056907","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Don't believe them! Reducing misinformation influence through source discreditation. 不要相信他们!通过对信息来源进行鉴定,减少错误信息的影响。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-26 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00581-7
Ullrich K H Ecker, Toby Prike, Antonia B Paver, Rosie J Scott, Briony Swire-Thompson

Misinformation often continues to influence people's reasoning even after it has been corrected. Therefore, an important aim of applied cognition research is to identify effective measures to counter misinformation. One frequently recommended but hitherto insufficiently tested strategy is source discreditation, that is, attacking the credibility of a misinformation source. In two experiments, we tested whether immediate source discreditation could reduce people's subsequent reliance on fictional event-related misinformation. In Experiment 1, the discreditation targeted a person source of misinformation, pointing to a conflict of interest. This intervention was compared with a commonly employed message-focused correction and a combination of correction and discreditation. The discreditation alone was effective, but less effective than a correction, with the combination of both most effective. Experiment 2 compared discreditations that targeted a person versus a media source of misinformation, pointing either to a conflict of interest or a poor track record of communication. Discreditations were effective for both types of sources, although track-record discreditations were less effective when the misinformation source was a media outlet compared to a person. Results demonstrate that continued influence of misinformation is shaped by social as well as cognitive factors and that source discreditation is a broadly applicable misinformation countermeasure.

即使在错误信息被纠正之后,它仍会继续影响人们的推理。因此,应用认知研究的一个重要目标就是找出有效的措施来对抗错误信息。一个经常被推荐但迄今为止尚未经过充分测试的策略是来源可信度,即攻击错误信息来源的可信度。在两个实验中,我们测试了立即诋毁信息源是否能减少人们随后对虚构事件相关错误信息的依赖。在实验 1 中,诋毁针对的是一个人的错误信息来源,指出其存在利益冲突。这一干预措施与通常采用的以信息为重点的更正以及更正与诋毁相结合的方法进行了比较。单独的诋毁是有效的,但不如更正有效,而两者的结合最为有效。实验 2 比较了针对错误信息的个人和媒体来源的诋毁,指出其存在利益冲突或传播记录不佳。对这两类信息源的诋毁都很有效,但与个人相比,当错误信息来源是媒体时,记录诋毁的效果较差。研究结果表明,错误信息的持续影响受社会因素和认知因素的影响,对信息来源进行诋毁是一种广泛适用的错误信息应对措施。
{"title":"Don't believe them! Reducing misinformation influence through source discreditation.","authors":"Ullrich K H Ecker, Toby Prike, Antonia B Paver, Rosie J Scott, Briony Swire-Thompson","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00581-7","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00581-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Misinformation often continues to influence people's reasoning even after it has been corrected. Therefore, an important aim of applied cognition research is to identify effective measures to counter misinformation. One frequently recommended but hitherto insufficiently tested strategy is source discreditation, that is, attacking the credibility of a misinformation source. In two experiments, we tested whether immediate source discreditation could reduce people's subsequent reliance on fictional event-related misinformation. In Experiment 1, the discreditation targeted a person source of misinformation, pointing to a conflict of interest. This intervention was compared with a commonly employed message-focused correction and a combination of correction and discreditation. The discreditation alone was effective, but less effective than a correction, with the combination of both most effective. Experiment 2 compared discreditations that targeted a person versus a media source of misinformation, pointing either to a conflict of interest or a poor track record of communication. Discreditations were effective for both types of sources, although track-record discreditations were less effective when the misinformation source was a media outlet compared to a person. Results demonstrate that continued influence of misinformation is shaped by social as well as cognitive factors and that source discreditation is a broadly applicable misinformation countermeasure.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"52"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11345350/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142056908","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cue relevance drives early quitting in visual search. 视觉搜索中的线索相关性会导致过早退出。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-26 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00587-1
Jeff Moher, Anna Delos Reyes, Trafton Drew

Irrelevant salient distractors can trigger early quitting in visual search, causing observers to miss targets they might otherwise find. Here, we asked whether task-relevant salient cues can produce a similar early quitting effect on the subset of trials where those cues fail to highlight the target. We presented participants with a difficult visual search task and used two cueing conditions. In the high-predictive condition, a salient cue in the form of a red circle highlighted the target most of the time a target was present. In the low-predictive condition, the cue was far less accurate and did not reliably predict the target (i.e., the cue was often a false positive). These were contrasted against a control condition in which no cues were presented. In the high-predictive condition, we found clear evidence of early quitting on trials where the cue was a false positive, as evidenced by both increased miss errors and shorter response times on target absent trials. No such effects were observed with low-predictive cues. Together, these results suggest that salient cues which are false positives can trigger early quitting, though perhaps only when the cues have a high-predictive value. These results have implications for real-world searches, such as medical image screening, where salient cues (referred to as computer-aided detection or CAD) may be used to highlight potentially relevant areas of images but are sometimes inaccurate.

在视觉搜索中,不相关的突出干扰物可能会引发早期放弃,导致观察者错过本可以找到的目标。在这里,我们想知道,与任务相关的突出线索是否会在这些线索未能突出目标的试验中产生类似的提早放弃效应。我们向参与者提出了一个困难的视觉搜索任务,并使用了两种提示条件。在高预测条件下,目标出现的大部分时间里,红色圆圈形式的突出提示都会突出目标。在低预测条件下,提示的准确性要低得多,而且不能可靠地预测目标(即提示经常是假阳性)。这些情况与没有提示的对照条件形成了鲜明对比。在高预测条件下,我们发现有明显的证据表明,在提示为假阳性的试验中,会出现过早放弃的情况,这表现在目标缺失试验中的失误增加和反应时间缩短。而在低预测性线索条件下,则没有观察到这种影响。总之,这些结果表明,假阳性的突出线索可能会引发早期放弃,但也许只有当这些线索具有较高的预测价值时才会如此。这些结果对现实世界中的搜索(如医学影像筛查)有一定的影响,在现实世界中,突出线索(称为计算机辅助检测或CAD)可用于突出图像中潜在的相关区域,但有时并不准确。
{"title":"Cue relevance drives early quitting in visual search.","authors":"Jeff Moher, Anna Delos Reyes, Trafton Drew","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00587-1","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00587-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Irrelevant salient distractors can trigger early quitting in visual search, causing observers to miss targets they might otherwise find. Here, we asked whether task-relevant salient cues can produce a similar early quitting effect on the subset of trials where those cues fail to highlight the target. We presented participants with a difficult visual search task and used two cueing conditions. In the high-predictive condition, a salient cue in the form of a red circle highlighted the target most of the time a target was present. In the low-predictive condition, the cue was far less accurate and did not reliably predict the target (i.e., the cue was often a false positive). These were contrasted against a control condition in which no cues were presented. In the high-predictive condition, we found clear evidence of early quitting on trials where the cue was a false positive, as evidenced by both increased miss errors and shorter response times on target absent trials. No such effects were observed with low-predictive cues. Together, these results suggest that salient cues which are false positives can trigger early quitting, though perhaps only when the cues have a high-predictive value. These results have implications for real-world searches, such as medical image screening, where salient cues (referred to as computer-aided detection or CAD) may be used to highlight potentially relevant areas of images but are sometimes inaccurate.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"54"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11345343/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142056906","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evidence-based scientific thinking and decision-making in everyday life. 日常生活中以证据为基础的科学思维和决策。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-07 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00578-2
Caitlin Dawson, Hanna Julku, Milla Pihlajamäki, Johanna K Kaakinen, Jonathan W Schooler, Jaana Simola

In today's knowledge economy, it is critical to make decisions based on high-quality evidence. Science-related decision-making is thought to rely on a complex interplay of reasoning skills, cognitive styles, attitudes, and motivations toward information. By investigating the relationship between individual differences and behaviors related to evidence-based decision-making, our aim was to better understand how adults engage with scientific information in everyday life. First, we used a data-driven exploratory approach to identify four latent factors in a large set of measures related to cognitive skills and epistemic attitudes. The resulting structure suggests that key factors include curiosity and positive attitudes toward science, prosociality, cognitive skills, and openmindedness to new information. Second, we investigated whether these factors predicted behavior in a naturalistic decision-making task. In the task, participants were introduced to a real science-related petition and were asked to read six online articles related to the petition, which varied in scientific quality, while deciding how to vote. We demonstrate that curiosity and positive science attitudes, cognitive flexibility, prosociality and emotional states, were related to engaging with information and discernment of evidence reliability. We further found that that social authority is a powerful cue for source credibility, even above the actual quality and relevance of the sources. Our results highlight that individual motivating factors toward information engagement, like curiosity, and social factors such as social authority are important drivers of how adults judge the credibility of everyday sources of scientific information.

在当今的知识经济时代,根据高质量的证据做出决策至关重要。与科学相关的决策被认为依赖于推理技能、认知风格、态度和对信息的动机等复杂的相互作用。通过研究个体差异与循证决策相关行为之间的关系,我们的目的是更好地了解成年人在日常生活中是如何接触科学信息的。首先,我们采用数据驱动的探索性方法,在大量与认知技能和认识态度相关的测量中识别出四个潜在因素。由此得出的结构表明,关键因素包括对科学的好奇心和积极态度、亲社会性、认知技能和对新信息的开放性。其次,我们研究了这些因素是否能预测自然决策任务中的行为。在这个任务中,参与者被介绍给一个真实的与科学有关的请愿书,并被要求阅读六篇与请愿书有关的在线文章,这些文章的科学质量各不相同,同时决定如何投票。我们证明,好奇心和积极的科学态度、认知灵活性、亲社会性和情绪状态与参与信息和辨别证据可靠性有关。我们进一步发现,社会权威是信息来源可信度的有力线索,甚至高于信息来源的实际质量和相关性。我们的研究结果突出表明,个人参与信息的动机因素(如好奇心)和社会因素(如社会权威)是成年人判断日常科学信息来源可信度的重要驱动力。
{"title":"Evidence-based scientific thinking and decision-making in everyday life.","authors":"Caitlin Dawson, Hanna Julku, Milla Pihlajamäki, Johanna K Kaakinen, Jonathan W Schooler, Jaana Simola","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00578-2","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00578-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In today's knowledge economy, it is critical to make decisions based on high-quality evidence. Science-related decision-making is thought to rely on a complex interplay of reasoning skills, cognitive styles, attitudes, and motivations toward information. By investigating the relationship between individual differences and behaviors related to evidence-based decision-making, our aim was to better understand how adults engage with scientific information in everyday life. First, we used a data-driven exploratory approach to identify four latent factors in a large set of measures related to cognitive skills and epistemic attitudes. The resulting structure suggests that key factors include curiosity and positive attitudes toward science, prosociality, cognitive skills, and openmindedness to new information. Second, we investigated whether these factors predicted behavior in a naturalistic decision-making task. In the task, participants were introduced to a real science-related petition and were asked to read six online articles related to the petition, which varied in scientific quality, while deciding how to vote. We demonstrate that curiosity and positive science attitudes, cognitive flexibility, prosociality and emotional states, were related to engaging with information and discernment of evidence reliability. We further found that that social authority is a powerful cue for source credibility, even above the actual quality and relevance of the sources. Our results highlight that individual motivating factors toward information engagement, like curiosity, and social factors such as social authority are important drivers of how adults judge the credibility of everyday sources of scientific information.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"50"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11306497/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141898627","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Grammatical structures of emoji in Japanese-language text conversations. 日语文本对话中表情符号的语法结构。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-29 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00571-9
Kazuki Sekine, Manaka Ikuta

Emojis have become a ubiquitous part of everyday text communication worldwide. Cohn et al. (Cognit Res Princ Implic 4(1):1-18, 2019) studied the grammatical structure of emoji usage among English speakers and found a correlation between the sequence of emojis used and English word order, tending towards an subject-verb-object (SVO) sequence. However, it remains unclear whether emoji usage follows a universal grammar or whether it is influenced by native language grammar. Therefore, this study explored the potential influence of Japanese grammar on emoji usage by Japanese speakers. Twenty adults, all native Japanese speakers, participated in pairs. In Experiment 1, participants engaged in conversations through Google Hangouts on iPads. The experiment consisted of four conversation rounds of approximately 8 min each. The first two rounds involved one participant using only written Japanese and the other using only emojis and punctuation, with roles reversed in the second round. The third round required both participants to use only emojis and punctuation. The results indicated that participants preferred subject-object-verb (SOV) or object-verb (OV) sequences, with OV patterns being more common. This pattern reflects a distinctive attribute of Japanese grammatical structure, marked by the frequent omission of the subject. Experiment 2 substituted emojis for words, showing nouns were more commonly replaced than verbs due to the difficulty in conveying complex meanings. Reduced subject replacements again emphasised Japanese grammatical structure. In essence, emoji usage reflects native language structures, but complexities are challenging to convey, resulting in simplified sequences. This study offers insights for enhancing emoji-based communication and interface design, with implications for translation and broader communication.

表情符号已成为全球日常文本交流中无处不在的一部分。Cohn等人(Cognit Res Princ Implic 4(1):1-18,2019)研究了英语使用者使用表情符号的语法结构,发现表情符号的使用顺序与英语词序之间存在相关性,趋向于主谓宾(SVO)顺序。然而,emoji 的使用是遵循通用语法,还是受母语语法的影响,目前仍不清楚。因此,本研究探讨了日语语法对日语使用者使用 emoji 的潜在影响。20 名以日语为母语的成年人以两人一组的形式参加了实验。在实验 1 中,参与者通过 iPad 上的 Google Hangouts 进行对话。实验包括四轮对话,每轮约 8 分钟。在前两轮对话中,一名参与者只使用书面日语,另一名参与者只使用表情符号和标点符号。第三轮要求两位参与者都只使用表情符号和标点符号。结果表明,参与者更喜欢主语-宾语-谓语(SOV)或宾语-谓语(OV)序列,其中 OV 模式更为常见。这种模式反映了日语语法结构的一个独特属性,即经常省略主语。实验 2 将表情符号替换为单词,结果显示,由于难以表达复杂的含义,名词比动词更常被替换。减少主语替换再次强调了日语的语法结构。从本质上讲,表情符号的使用反映了母语的结构,但复杂的意思难以表达,从而导致了简化的序列。这项研究为加强基于表情符号的交流和界面设计提供了启示,对翻译和更广泛的交流都有影响。
{"title":"Grammatical structures of emoji in Japanese-language text conversations.","authors":"Kazuki Sekine, Manaka Ikuta","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00571-9","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00571-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emojis have become a ubiquitous part of everyday text communication worldwide. Cohn et al. (Cognit Res Princ Implic 4(1):1-18, 2019) studied the grammatical structure of emoji usage among English speakers and found a correlation between the sequence of emojis used and English word order, tending towards an subject-verb-object (SVO) sequence. However, it remains unclear whether emoji usage follows a universal grammar or whether it is influenced by native language grammar. Therefore, this study explored the potential influence of Japanese grammar on emoji usage by Japanese speakers. Twenty adults, all native Japanese speakers, participated in pairs. In Experiment 1, participants engaged in conversations through Google Hangouts on iPads. The experiment consisted of four conversation rounds of approximately 8 min each. The first two rounds involved one participant using only written Japanese and the other using only emojis and punctuation, with roles reversed in the second round. The third round required both participants to use only emojis and punctuation. The results indicated that participants preferred subject-object-verb (SOV) or object-verb (OV) sequences, with OV patterns being more common. This pattern reflects a distinctive attribute of Japanese grammatical structure, marked by the frequent omission of the subject. Experiment 2 substituted emojis for words, showing nouns were more commonly replaced than verbs due to the difficulty in conveying complex meanings. Reduced subject replacements again emphasised Japanese grammatical structure. In essence, emoji usage reflects native language structures, but complexities are challenging to convey, resulting in simplified sequences. This study offers insights for enhancing emoji-based communication and interface design, with implications for translation and broader communication.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"49"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11286883/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141789458","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring the linguistic complexity of third-grade numerical literacy. 探索三年级数字识字的语言复杂性。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00575-5
Ella Shalit, Dror Dotan

Reading numbers aloud, a central aspect of numerical literacy, is a challenging skill to acquire, but the origins of this difficulty remain poorly understood. To investigate this matter, we examined the performance of 127 third- and fourth-grade children who read aloud, in Hebrew, numbers with 2-5 digits. We found several key observations. First, we observed a substantial variation among the 3rd graders-7% and 59% errors in the top and bottom deciles, respectively. Second, the task difficulty stemmed from syntactic processing: Most errors were distortions of the number's syntax, as opposed to digit substitutions or transpositions, and the main factor affecting a specific number's difficulty was not its magnitude, as is commonly assumed, but rather its syntactic structure. Third, number reading performance was not predicted by a school-like task that assessed syntactic-conceptual knowledge of the decimal system structure, but rather by knowledge of specific syntactic-verbal rules, suggesting that the syntactic-verbal knowledge is separate from the syntactic-conceptual knowledge. Last, there was a double dissociation between 4-digit numbers and 5-digit numbers, which in Hebrew have completely different syntactic structures: Half of the children showed a significant advantage in one number length compared to the other, with equal numbers of children preferring either length. This indicates that the different syntactic-verbal rules are learned relatively independently of each other, with little or no generalization from one rule to another. In light of these findings, we propose that schools should specifically teach number reading, with focus on specific syntactic-verbal rules.

朗读数字是数字读写能力的一个核心方面,是一项极具挑战性的技能,但人们对这一困难的根源却知之甚少。为了研究这个问题,我们对 127 名三、四年级儿童用希伯来语朗读 2-5 位数字的表现进行了研究。我们发现了几个重要现象。首先,我们观察到三年级学生之间的差异很大,最高分和最低分的错误率分别为 7% 和 59%。其次,任务难度源于句法处理:大多数错误都是对数字句法的扭曲,而不是数字的替换或移位,而且影响特定数字难度的主要因素并非通常认为的数字大小,而是其句法结构。第三,数字阅读成绩并不是由评估十进制系统结构的句法-概念知识的学校类任务来预测的,而是由特定的句法-言语规则知识来预测的,这表明句法-言语知识与句法-概念知识是分离的。最后,希伯来语中句法结构完全不同的 4 位数和 5 位数之间存在双重分离:半数儿童在一种数字长度上比另一种数字长度有明显优势,但也有相同数量的儿童偏好两种数字长度。这表明,不同的句法-言语规则是相对独立地学习的,很少或根本没有从一种规则泛化到另一种规则。鉴于这些研究结果,我们建议学校应专门教授数字阅读,重点放在特定的句法--语言规则上。
{"title":"Exploring the linguistic complexity of third-grade numerical literacy.","authors":"Ella Shalit, Dror Dotan","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00575-5","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00575-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Reading numbers aloud, a central aspect of numerical literacy, is a challenging skill to acquire, but the origins of this difficulty remain poorly understood. To investigate this matter, we examined the performance of 127 third- and fourth-grade children who read aloud, in Hebrew, numbers with 2-5 digits. We found several key observations. First, we observed a substantial variation among the 3rd graders-7% and 59% errors in the top and bottom deciles, respectively. Second, the task difficulty stemmed from syntactic processing: Most errors were distortions of the number's syntax, as opposed to digit substitutions or transpositions, and the main factor affecting a specific number's difficulty was not its magnitude, as is commonly assumed, but rather its syntactic structure. Third, number reading performance was not predicted by a school-like task that assessed syntactic-conceptual knowledge of the decimal system structure, but rather by knowledge of specific syntactic-verbal rules, suggesting that the syntactic-verbal knowledge is separate from the syntactic-conceptual knowledge. Last, there was a double dissociation between 4-digit numbers and 5-digit numbers, which in Hebrew have completely different syntactic structures: Half of the children showed a significant advantage in one number length compared to the other, with equal numbers of children preferring either length. This indicates that the different syntactic-verbal rules are learned relatively independently of each other, with little or no generalization from one rule to another. In light of these findings, we propose that schools should specifically teach number reading, with focus on specific syntactic-verbal rules.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"48"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11255179/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141634955","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceptions of artificial intelligence system's aptitude to judge morality and competence amidst the rise of Chatbots. 在聊天机器人兴起之际,对人工智能系统判断道德和能力的看法。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00573-7
Manuel Oliveira, Justus Brands, Judith Mashudi, Baptist Liefooghe, Ruud Hortensius

This paper examines how humans judge the capabilities of artificial intelligence (AI) to evaluate human attributes, specifically focusing on two key dimensions of human social evaluation: morality and competence. Furthermore, it investigates the impact of exposure to advanced Large Language Models on these perceptions. In three studies (combined N = 200), we tested the hypothesis that people will find it less plausible that AI is capable of judging the morality conveyed by a behavior compared to judging its competence. Participants estimated the plausibility of AI origin for a set of written impressions of positive and negative behaviors related to morality and competence. Studies 1 and 3 supported our hypothesis that people would be more inclined to attribute AI origin to competence-related impressions compared to morality-related ones. In Study 2, we found this effect only for impressions of positive behaviors. Additional exploratory analyses clarified that the differentiation between the AI origin of competence and morality judgments persisted throughout the first half year after the public launch of popular AI chatbot (i.e., ChatGPT) and could not be explained by participants' general attitudes toward AI, or the actual source of the impressions (i.e., AI or human). These findings suggest an enduring belief that AI is less adept at assessing the morality compared to the competence of human behavior, even as AI capabilities continued to advance.

本文研究了人类如何判断人工智能(AI)评估人类属性的能力,特别关注人类社会评价的两个关键维度:道德和能力。此外,本文还研究了接触先进的大型语言模型对这些看法的影响。在三项研究(总人数=200)中,我们测试了这样一个假设:与判断能力相比,人们会认为人工智能判断行为所传达的道德性的能力不太可信。受试者对一组与道德和能力有关的正面和负面行为的书面印象进行了人工智能起源可信度估计。研究 1 和研究 3 支持了我们的假设,即与道德相关的印象相比,人们更倾向于将人工智能的起源归因于能力相关的印象。在研究 2 中,我们发现只有对积极行为的印象才会产生这种效应。更多的探索性分析表明,在流行的人工智能聊天机器人(即 ChatGPT)公开发布后的前半年中,能力和道德判断的人工智能来源之间的差异一直存在,这并不能用参与者对人工智能的总体态度或印象的实际来源(即人工智能还是人类)来解释。这些研究结果表明,即使人工智能的能力在不断进步,但人们始终认为,与人类行为的能力相比,人工智能不太擅长评估道德。
{"title":"Perceptions of artificial intelligence system's aptitude to judge morality and competence amidst the rise of Chatbots.","authors":"Manuel Oliveira, Justus Brands, Judith Mashudi, Baptist Liefooghe, Ruud Hortensius","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00573-7","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00573-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This paper examines how humans judge the capabilities of artificial intelligence (AI) to evaluate human attributes, specifically focusing on two key dimensions of human social evaluation: morality and competence. Furthermore, it investigates the impact of exposure to advanced Large Language Models on these perceptions. In three studies (combined N = 200), we tested the hypothesis that people will find it less plausible that AI is capable of judging the morality conveyed by a behavior compared to judging its competence. Participants estimated the plausibility of AI origin for a set of written impressions of positive and negative behaviors related to morality and competence. Studies 1 and 3 supported our hypothesis that people would be more inclined to attribute AI origin to competence-related impressions compared to morality-related ones. In Study 2, we found this effect only for impressions of positive behaviors. Additional exploratory analyses clarified that the differentiation between the AI origin of competence and morality judgments persisted throughout the first half year after the public launch of popular AI chatbot (i.e., ChatGPT) and could not be explained by participants' general attitudes toward AI, or the actual source of the impressions (i.e., AI or human). These findings suggest an enduring belief that AI is less adept at assessing the morality compared to the competence of human behavior, even as AI capabilities continued to advance.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"47"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11255178/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141634956","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does using artificial intelligence assistance accelerate skill decay and hinder skill development without performers' awareness? 使用人工智能辅助是否会在表演者毫无察觉的情况下加速技能衰减并阻碍技能发展?
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-12 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00572-8
Brooke N Macnamara, Ibrahim Berber, M Cenk Çavuşoğlu, Elizabeth A Krupinski, Naren Nallapareddy, Noelle E Nelson, Philip J Smith, Amy L Wilson-Delfosse, Soumya Ray

Artificial intelligence in the workplace is becoming increasingly common. These tools are sometimes used to aid users in performing their task, for example, when an artificial intelligence tool assists a radiologist in their search for abnormalities in radiographic images. The use of artificial intelligence brings a wealth of benefits, such as increasing the efficiency and efficacy of performance. However, little research has been conducted to determine how the use of artificial intelligence assistants might affect the user's cognitive skills. In this theoretical perspective, we discuss how artificial intelligence assistants might accelerate skill decay among experts and hinder skill acquisition among learners. Further, we discuss how AI assistants might also prevent experts and learners from recognizing these deleterious effects. We then discuss the types of questions: use-inspired basic cognitive researchers, applied researchers, and computer science researchers should seek to answer. We conclude that multidisciplinary research from use-inspired basic cognitive research, domain-specific applied research, and technical research (e.g., human factors research, computer science research) is needed to (a) understand these potential consequences, (b) design artificial intelligence systems to mitigate these impacts, and (c) develop training and use protocols to prevent negative impacts on users' cognitive skills. Only by answering these questions from multidisciplinary perspectives can we harness the benefits of artificial intelligence in the workplace while preventing negative impacts on users' cognitive skills.

人工智能在工作场所的应用越来越普遍。这些工具有时用于协助用户完成任务,例如,人工智能工具可以协助放射科医生搜索放射图像中的异常情况。人工智能的使用带来了很多好处,比如提高工作效率和效能。然而,对于人工智能助手的使用会如何影响用户的认知能力,却鲜有研究。在这一理论视角中,我们将讨论人工智能助手可能如何加速专家的技能衰减,并阻碍学习者的技能习得。此外,我们还讨论了人工智能助手可能如何阻止专家和学习者认识到这些有害影响。然后,我们讨论了使用启发基础认知研究人员、应用研究人员和计算机科学研究人员应该回答的问题类型。我们的结论是,需要从使用启发的基础认知研究、特定领域的应用研究和技术研究(如人为因素研究、计算机科学研究)开展多学科研究,以便:(a) 了解这些潜在后果;(b) 设计人工智能系统以减轻这些影响;(c) 制定培训和使用协议以防止对用户认知技能的负面影响。只有从多学科角度回答这些问题,我们才能在工作场所利用人工智能的好处,同时防止对用户认知技能的负面影响。
{"title":"Does using artificial intelligence assistance accelerate skill decay and hinder skill development without performers' awareness?","authors":"Brooke N Macnamara, Ibrahim Berber, M Cenk Çavuşoğlu, Elizabeth A Krupinski, Naren Nallapareddy, Noelle E Nelson, Philip J Smith, Amy L Wilson-Delfosse, Soumya Ray","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00572-8","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00572-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Artificial intelligence in the workplace is becoming increasingly common. These tools are sometimes used to aid users in performing their task, for example, when an artificial intelligence tool assists a radiologist in their search for abnormalities in radiographic images. The use of artificial intelligence brings a wealth of benefits, such as increasing the efficiency and efficacy of performance. However, little research has been conducted to determine how the use of artificial intelligence assistants might affect the user's cognitive skills. In this theoretical perspective, we discuss how artificial intelligence assistants might accelerate skill decay among experts and hinder skill acquisition among learners. Further, we discuss how AI assistants might also prevent experts and learners from recognizing these deleterious effects. We then discuss the types of questions: use-inspired basic cognitive researchers, applied researchers, and computer science researchers should seek to answer. We conclude that multidisciplinary research from use-inspired basic cognitive research, domain-specific applied research, and technical research (e.g., human factors research, computer science research) is needed to (a) understand these potential consequences, (b) design artificial intelligence systems to mitigate these impacts, and (c) develop training and use protocols to prevent negative impacts on users' cognitive skills. Only by answering these questions from multidisciplinary perspectives can we harness the benefits of artificial intelligence in the workplace while preventing negative impacts on users' cognitive skills.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"46"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-07-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11239631/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141591699","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1