首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications最新文献

英文 中文
How do students reason about statistical sampling with computer simulations? An integrative review from a grounded cognition perspective. 学生如何利用计算机模拟进行统计抽样推理?从基础认知的角度进行综合评述。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-31 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00561-x
Sebahat Gok, Robert L Goldstone

Interactive computer simulations are commonly used as pedagogical tools to support students' statistical reasoning. This paper examines whether and how these simulations enable their intended effects. We begin by contrasting two theoretical frameworks-dual processes and grounded cognition-in the context of people's conceptions about statistical sampling, setting the stage for the potential benefits of simulations in learning such conceptions. Then, we continue with reviewing the educational literature on statistical sampling simulations. Our review tentatively suggests benefits of the simulations for building statistical habits of mind. However, challenges seem to persist when more specific concepts and skills are investigated. With and without simulations, students have difficulty forming an aggregate view of data, interpreting sampling distributions, showing a process-based understanding of the law of large numbers, making statistical inferences, and context-independent reasoning. We propose that grounded cognition offers a framework for understanding these findings, highlighting the bidirectional relationship between perception and conception, perceptual design features, and guided perceptual routines for supporting students' meaning making from simulations. Finally, we propose testable instructional strategies for using simulations in statistics education.

交互式计算机模拟通常被用作辅助学生进行统计推理的教学工具。本文将探讨这些模拟是否以及如何实现其预期效果。首先,我们从人们对统计抽样的概念出发,对比了两个理论框架--双重过程和基础认知,为模拟在学习此类概念方面的潜在优势奠定了基础。然后,我们继续回顾有关统计抽样模拟的教育文献。我们的回顾初步表明了模拟对培养统计思维习惯的益处。然而,在研究更具体的概念和技能时,挑战似乎依然存在。无论是否进行模拟,学生都难以形成对数据的总体看法、解释抽样分布、展示对大数定律的基于过程的理解、进行统计推断以及独立于上下文的推理。我们提出,基础认知为理解这些发现提供了一个框架,强调了感知与概念之间的双向关系、感知设计特征以及支持学生从模拟中获得意义的引导性感知常规。最后,我们提出了在统计教育中使用模拟的可检验的教学策略。
{"title":"How do students reason about statistical sampling with computer simulations? An integrative review from a grounded cognition perspective.","authors":"Sebahat Gok, Robert L Goldstone","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00561-x","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00561-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Interactive computer simulations are commonly used as pedagogical tools to support students' statistical reasoning. This paper examines whether and how these simulations enable their intended effects. We begin by contrasting two theoretical frameworks-dual processes and grounded cognition-in the context of people's conceptions about statistical sampling, setting the stage for the potential benefits of simulations in learning such conceptions. Then, we continue with reviewing the educational literature on statistical sampling simulations. Our review tentatively suggests benefits of the simulations for building statistical habits of mind. However, challenges seem to persist when more specific concepts and skills are investigated. With and without simulations, students have difficulty forming an aggregate view of data, interpreting sampling distributions, showing a process-based understanding of the law of large numbers, making statistical inferences, and context-independent reasoning. We propose that grounded cognition offers a framework for understanding these findings, highlighting the bidirectional relationship between perception and conception, perceptual design features, and guided perceptual routines for supporting students' meaning making from simulations. Finally, we propose testable instructional strategies for using simulations in statistics education.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"33"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11139845/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141180893","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Seeing the truck, but missing the cyclist: effects of blur on duration thresholds for road hazard detection. 看到卡车,却没看到骑自行车的人:模糊对道路危险检测持续时间阈值的影响。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-20 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00557-7
Silvia Guidi, Anna Kosovicheva, Benjamin Wolfe

Drivers must respond promptly to a wide range of possible road hazards, from trucks veering into their lane to pedestrians stepping onto the road. While drivers' vision is tested at the point of licensure, visual function can degrade, and drivers may not notice how these changes impact their ability to notice and respond to events in the world in a timely fashion. To safely examine the potential consequences of visual degradation on hazard detection, we performed two experiments examining the impact of simulated optical blur on participants' viewing duration thresholds in a hazard detection task, as a proxy for eyes-on-road duration behind the wheel. Examining this question with older and younger participants, across two experiments, we found an overall increase in viewing duration thresholds under blurred conditions, such that younger and older adults were similarly impacted by blur. Critically, in both groups, we found that the increment in thresholds produced by blur was larger for non-vehicular road hazards (pedestrians, cyclists and animals) compared to vehicular road hazards (cars, trucks and buses). This work suggests that blur poses a particular problem for drivers detecting non-vehicular road users, a population considerably more vulnerable in a collision than vehicular road users. These results also highlight the importance of taking into account the type of hazard when considering the impacts of blur on road hazard detection.

驾驶员必须及时应对各种可能的道路危险,从卡车驶入自己的车道到行人踏上马路。虽然在考取驾照时会对驾驶员的视力进行测试,但视觉功能可能会退化,驾驶员可能不会注意到这些变化会如何影响他们及时发现和应对外界事件的能力。为了安全地检查视觉退化对危险检测的潜在影响,我们进行了两项实验,检查模拟光学模糊对参与者在危险检测任务中的观察持续时间阈值的影响,以此来替代驾驶时的路面观察持续时间。在两次实验中,我们分别对老年人和年轻人进行了研究,发现在模糊条件下,观察持续时间阈值总体上有所上升,因此年轻人和老年人受到模糊的影响相似。重要的是,在这两组实验中,我们发现与车辆道路危险(汽车、卡车和公共汽车)相比,模糊对非车辆道路危险(行人、骑自行车者和动物)产生的阈值增量更大。这项研究表明,模糊对驾驶员检测非车辆道路使用者造成了特别的问题,而非车辆道路使用者在碰撞中比车辆道路使用者更容易受到伤害。这些结果还强调了在考虑模糊对道路危险检测的影响时,考虑危险类型的重要性。
{"title":"Seeing the truck, but missing the cyclist: effects of blur on duration thresholds for road hazard detection.","authors":"Silvia Guidi, Anna Kosovicheva, Benjamin Wolfe","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00557-7","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00557-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Drivers must respond promptly to a wide range of possible road hazards, from trucks veering into their lane to pedestrians stepping onto the road. While drivers' vision is tested at the point of licensure, visual function can degrade, and drivers may not notice how these changes impact their ability to notice and respond to events in the world in a timely fashion. To safely examine the potential consequences of visual degradation on hazard detection, we performed two experiments examining the impact of simulated optical blur on participants' viewing duration thresholds in a hazard detection task, as a proxy for eyes-on-road duration behind the wheel. Examining this question with older and younger participants, across two experiments, we found an overall increase in viewing duration thresholds under blurred conditions, such that younger and older adults were similarly impacted by blur. Critically, in both groups, we found that the increment in thresholds produced by blur was larger for non-vehicular road hazards (pedestrians, cyclists and animals) compared to vehicular road hazards (cars, trucks and buses). This work suggests that blur poses a particular problem for drivers detecting non-vehicular road users, a population considerably more vulnerable in a collision than vehicular road users. These results also highlight the importance of taking into account the type of hazard when considering the impacts of blur on road hazard detection.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"32"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11106223/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141065863","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Boosting wisdom of the crowd for medical image annotation using training performance and task features. 利用训练成绩和任务特征提升医学图像注释的群体智慧。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-20 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00558-6
Eeshan Hasan, Erik Duhaime, Jennifer S Trueblood

A crucial bottleneck in medical artificial intelligence (AI) is high-quality labeled medical datasets. In this paper, we test a large variety of wisdom of the crowd algorithms to label medical images that were initially classified by individuals recruited through an app-based platform. Individuals classified skin lesions from the International Skin Lesion Challenge 2018 into 7 different categories. There was a large dispersion in the geographical location, experience, training, and performance of the recruited individuals. We tested several wisdom of the crowd algorithms of varying complexity from a simple unweighted average to more complex Bayesian models that account for individual patterns of errors. Using a switchboard analysis, we observe that the best-performing algorithms rely on selecting top performers, weighting decisions by training accuracy, and take into account the task environment. These algorithms far exceed expert performance. We conclude by discussing the implications of these approaches for the development of medical AI.

医疗人工智能(AI)的一个关键瓶颈是高质量的标记医疗数据集。在本文中,我们测试了大量众智算法,以标注通过基于应用的平台招募的个人最初分类的医学图像。个人将 2018 年国际皮肤病变挑战赛中的皮肤病变分为 7 个不同的类别。被招募者的地理位置、经验、培训和表现存在很大差异。我们测试了几种复杂程度不同的群众智慧算法,从简单的非加权平均到考虑到个人错误模式的更复杂的贝叶斯模型。通过配电盘分析,我们发现表现最好的算法依赖于选择表现最出色的人,根据训练准确性对决策进行加权,并将任务环境考虑在内。这些算法远远超过了专家的表现。最后,我们将讨论这些方法对医学人工智能发展的影响。
{"title":"Boosting wisdom of the crowd for medical image annotation using training performance and task features.","authors":"Eeshan Hasan, Erik Duhaime, Jennifer S Trueblood","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00558-6","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00558-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A crucial bottleneck in medical artificial intelligence (AI) is high-quality labeled medical datasets. In this paper, we test a large variety of wisdom of the crowd algorithms to label medical images that were initially classified by individuals recruited through an app-based platform. Individuals classified skin lesions from the International Skin Lesion Challenge 2018 into 7 different categories. There was a large dispersion in the geographical location, experience, training, and performance of the recruited individuals. We tested several wisdom of the crowd algorithms of varying complexity from a simple unweighted average to more complex Bayesian models that account for individual patterns of errors. Using a switchboard analysis, we observe that the best-performing algorithms rely on selecting top performers, weighting decisions by training accuracy, and take into account the task environment. These algorithms far exceed expert performance. We conclude by discussing the implications of these approaches for the development of medical AI.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"31"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11102897/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141065857","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Research on re-searching: interrupted foraging is not disrupted foraging. 关于重新搜索的研究:中断的觅食不是中断的觅食。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-15 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00556-8
Injae Hong, Jeremy M Wolfe

In classic visual search, observers typically search for the presence of a target in a scene or display. In foraging tasks, there may be multiple targets in the same display (or "patch"). Observers typically search for and collect these target items in one patch until they decide to leave that patch and move to the next one. This is a highly rule-governed behavior. The current study investigated whether these rules are disrupted when the foraging is interrupted in various manners. In Experiment 1, the foraging was briefly interrupted and then resumed in the same patch. In Experiments 2 and 3, the foraging in each patch either ended voluntarily or compulsorily after a fixed amount of time. In these cases, foraging resumed in a patch only after all patches were visited. Overall, the rules of foraging remained largely intact, though Experiment 2 shows that foraging rules can be overridden by the demand characteristics of the task. The results show that participants tended to perform approximately consistently despite interruptions. The results suggest that foraging behavior in a relatively simple foraging environment is resilient and not easily disrupted by interruption.

在传统的视觉搜索中,观察者通常会搜索场景或显示屏中是否存在目标。在觅食任务中,同一显示屏(或 "斑块")中可能有多个目标。观察者通常会在一个斑块中搜索并收集这些目标物,直到他们决定离开该斑块并转移到下一个斑块。这是一种高度受规则支配的行为。本研究调查了当觅食行为以各种方式中断时,这些规则是否会被破坏。在实验 1 中,觅食被短暂中断,然后在同一斑块中继续觅食。在实验 2 和 3 中,每个斑块中的觅食行为在固定时间后自愿或被迫结束。在这种情况下,只有在所有的斑块都被觅食完之后,才会在一个斑块中重新开始觅食。总体而言,觅食规则在很大程度上保持不变,但实验 2 表明,觅食规则可以被任务的需求特征所推翻。实验结果表明,尽管有干扰,参与者的觅食行为仍然趋于一致。结果表明,在相对简单的觅食环境中,觅食行为具有弹性,不易被中断所破坏。
{"title":"Research on re-searching: interrupted foraging is not disrupted foraging.","authors":"Injae Hong, Jeremy M Wolfe","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00556-8","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00556-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In classic visual search, observers typically search for the presence of a target in a scene or display. In foraging tasks, there may be multiple targets in the same display (or \"patch\"). Observers typically search for and collect these target items in one patch until they decide to leave that patch and move to the next one. This is a highly rule-governed behavior. The current study investigated whether these rules are disrupted when the foraging is interrupted in various manners. In Experiment 1, the foraging was briefly interrupted and then resumed in the same patch. In Experiments 2 and 3, the foraging in each patch either ended voluntarily or compulsorily after a fixed amount of time. In these cases, foraging resumed in a patch only after all patches were visited. Overall, the rules of foraging remained largely intact, though Experiment 2 shows that foraging rules can be overridden by the demand characteristics of the task. The results show that participants tended to perform approximately consistently despite interruptions. The results suggest that foraging behavior in a relatively simple foraging environment is resilient and not easily disrupted by interruption.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"30"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11096138/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140923068","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attention capture by own name decreases with speech compression. 语音压缩后,用自己的名字吸引注意力的情况会减少。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-12 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00555-9
Simon Y W Li, Alan L F Lee, Jenny W S Chiu, Robert G Loeb, Penelope M Sanderson

Auditory stimuli that are relevant to a listener have the potential to capture focal attention even when unattended, the listener's own name being a particularly effective stimulus. We report two experiments to test the attention-capturing potential of the listener's own name in normal speech and time-compressed speech. In Experiment 1, 39 participants were tested with a visual word categorization task with uncompressed spoken names as background auditory distractors. Participants' word categorization performance was slower when hearing their own name rather than other names, and in a final test, they were faster at detecting their own name than other names. Experiment 2 used the same task paradigm, but the auditory distractors were time-compressed names. Three compression levels were tested with 25 participants in each condition. Participants' word categorization performance was again slower when hearing their own name than when hearing other names; the slowing was strongest with slight compression and weakest with intense compression. Personally relevant time-compressed speech has the potential to capture attention, but the degree of capture depends on the level of compression. Attention capture by time-compressed speech has practical significance and provides partial evidence for the duplex-mechanism account of auditory distraction.

与听者相关的听觉刺激即使在无人注意的情况下也有可能吸引焦点注意力,听者自己的名字就是一种特别有效的刺激。我们报告了两个实验,分别测试了在正常语音和时间压缩语音中听者自己名字吸引注意力的潜力。在实验 1 中,39 名参与者接受了视觉单词分类任务测试,以未压缩的口语名字作为背景听觉干扰物。参与者在听到自己的名字而不是其他名字时,单词分类的速度较慢,而在最后的测试中,他们检测自己名字的速度比检测其他名字的速度更快。实验 2 采用了相同的任务范式,但听觉干扰物是经过时间压缩的名字。每个条件下有 25 名参与者参加了三种压缩水平的测试。参与者在听到自己名字时的单词分类速度再次低于听到其他名字时的速度;轻微压缩时的速度最慢,强烈压缩时的速度最慢。与个人相关的时间压缩语音具有吸引注意力的潜力,但吸引注意力的程度取决于压缩的程度。时间压缩语音对注意力的捕获具有实际意义,并为听觉分心的双工机制理论提供了部分证据。
{"title":"Attention capture by own name decreases with speech compression.","authors":"Simon Y W Li, Alan L F Lee, Jenny W S Chiu, Robert G Loeb, Penelope M Sanderson","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00555-9","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00555-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Auditory stimuli that are relevant to a listener have the potential to capture focal attention even when unattended, the listener's own name being a particularly effective stimulus. We report two experiments to test the attention-capturing potential of the listener's own name in normal speech and time-compressed speech. In Experiment 1, 39 participants were tested with a visual word categorization task with uncompressed spoken names as background auditory distractors. Participants' word categorization performance was slower when hearing their own name rather than other names, and in a final test, they were faster at detecting their own name than other names. Experiment 2 used the same task paradigm, but the auditory distractors were time-compressed names. Three compression levels were tested with 25 participants in each condition. Participants' word categorization performance was again slower when hearing their own name than when hearing other names; the slowing was strongest with slight compression and weakest with intense compression. Personally relevant time-compressed speech has the potential to capture attention, but the degree of capture depends on the level of compression. Attention capture by time-compressed speech has practical significance and provides partial evidence for the duplex-mechanism account of auditory distraction.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"29"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11089017/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140909510","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Memory and belief updating following complete and partial reminders of fake news. 完全和部分提醒假新闻后的记忆和信念更新。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-07 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00546-w
Paige L Kemp, Alyssa H Sinclair, R Alison Adcock, Christopher N Wahlheim

Fake news can have enduring effects on memory and beliefs. An ongoing theoretical debate has investigated whether corrections (fact-checks) should include reminders of fake news. The familiarity backfire account proposes that reminders hinder correction (increasing interference), whereas integration-based accounts argue that reminders facilitate correction (promoting memory integration). In three experiments, we examined how different types of corrections influenced memory for and belief in news headlines. In the exposure phase, participants viewed real and fake news headlines. In the correction phase, participants viewed reminders of fake news that either reiterated the false details (complete) or prompted recall of missing false details (partial); reminders were followed by fact-checked headlines correcting the false details. Both reminder types led to proactive interference in memory for corrected details, but complete reminders produced less interference than partial reminders (Experiment 1). However, when participants had fewer initial exposures to fake news and experienced a delay between exposure and correction, this effect was reversed; partial reminders led to proactive facilitation, enhancing correction (Experiment 2). This effect occurred regardless of the delay before correction (Experiment 3), suggesting that the effects of partial reminders depend on the number of prior fake news exposures. In all experiments, memory and perceived accuracy were better when fake news and corrections were recollected, implicating a critical role for integrative encoding. Overall, we show that when memories of fake news are weak or less accessible, partial reminders are more effective for correction; when memories of fake news are stronger or more accessible, complete reminders are preferable.

假新闻会对记忆和信念产生持久影响。理论界一直在争论纠正(事实核查)是否应包括对假新闻的提醒。熟视无睹说认为,提醒会阻碍纠正(增加干扰),而基于整合的说法则认为,提醒会促进纠正(促进记忆整合)。在三个实验中,我们考察了不同类型的纠正如何影响对新闻标题的记忆和信念。在暴露阶段,参与者观看真实和虚假的新闻标题。在纠正阶段,参与者观看假新闻的提醒,这些提醒要么重申了虚假细节(完整的),要么提示回忆缺失的虚假细节(部分的);提醒之后是经过事实检验的标题,纠正了虚假细节。两种类型的提醒都会导致对已纠正细节的记忆产生主动干扰,但完整提醒比部分提醒产生的干扰更少(实验 1)。然而,当被试者最初接触假新闻的次数较少,并且在接触和纠正之间有一定的延迟时,这种效应就会发生逆转;部分提醒会导致主动促进,增强纠正效果(实验 2)。无论纠正前的延迟时间长短,这种效果都会出现(实验 3),这表明部分提醒的效果取决于之前接触假新闻的次数。在所有实验中,当假新闻和更正被重新忆起时,记忆和感知的准确性都会更好,这意味着整合编码起着关键作用。总之,我们的研究表明,当对假新闻的记忆较弱或较难获得时,部分提醒对纠正更有效;而当对假新闻的记忆较强或更容易获得时,完整的提醒更可取。
{"title":"Memory and belief updating following complete and partial reminders of fake news.","authors":"Paige L Kemp, Alyssa H Sinclair, R Alison Adcock, Christopher N Wahlheim","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00546-w","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00546-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Fake news can have enduring effects on memory and beliefs. An ongoing theoretical debate has investigated whether corrections (fact-checks) should include reminders of fake news. The familiarity backfire account proposes that reminders hinder correction (increasing interference), whereas integration-based accounts argue that reminders facilitate correction (promoting memory integration). In three experiments, we examined how different types of corrections influenced memory for and belief in news headlines. In the exposure phase, participants viewed real and fake news headlines. In the correction phase, participants viewed reminders of fake news that either reiterated the false details (complete) or prompted recall of missing false details (partial); reminders were followed by fact-checked headlines correcting the false details. Both reminder types led to proactive interference in memory for corrected details, but complete reminders produced less interference than partial reminders (Experiment 1). However, when participants had fewer initial exposures to fake news and experienced a delay between exposure and correction, this effect was reversed; partial reminders led to proactive facilitation, enhancing correction (Experiment 2). This effect occurred regardless of the delay before correction (Experiment 3), suggesting that the effects of partial reminders depend on the number of prior fake news exposures. In all experiments, memory and perceived accuracy were better when fake news and corrections were recollected, implicating a critical role for integrative encoding. Overall, we show that when memories of fake news are weak or less accessible, partial reminders are more effective for correction; when memories of fake news are stronger or more accessible, complete reminders are preferable.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"28"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11076432/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140852936","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The psychological reality of the learned "p < .05" boundary. 学会 "p < .05 "界限的心理现实。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-03 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00553-x
V N Vimal Rao, Jeffrey K Bye, Sashank Varma

The .05 boundary within Null Hypothesis Statistical Testing (NHST) "has made a lot of people very angry and been widely regarded as a bad move" (to quote Douglas Adams). Here, we move past meta-scientific arguments and ask an empirical question: What is the psychological standing of the .05 boundary for statistical significance? We find that graduate students in the psychological sciences show a boundary effect when relating p-values across .05. We propose this psychological boundary is learned through statistical training in NHST and reading a scientific literature replete with "statistical significance". Consistent with this proposal, undergraduates do not show the same sensitivity to the .05 boundary. Additionally, the size of a graduate student's boundary effect is not associated with their explicit endorsement of questionable research practices. These findings suggest that training creates distortions in initial processing of p-values, but these might be dampened through scientific processes operating over longer timescales.

零假设统计检验(NHST)中 0.05 的界限 "让很多人非常生气,并被广泛认为是一个错误的举动"(引用道格拉斯-亚当斯的话)。在此,我们将跳出元科学的争论,提出一个经验性的问题:0.05 的统计显著性界限在心理学上的地位如何?我们发现,心理科学专业的研究生在将 p 值与 .05 相联系时,会表现出边界效应。我们认为,这种心理边界是通过 NHST 的统计培训和阅读充斥着 "统计显著性 "的科学文献学习到的。与这一提议相一致的是,本科生对 .05 临界值并不表现出同样的敏感性。此外,研究生边界效应的大小与他们对有问题的研究实践的明确认可无关。这些研究结果表明,培训会造成对 p 值的初始处理失真,但这些失真可能会通过在较长时期内运作的科学过程得到抑制。
{"title":"The psychological reality of the learned \"p < .05\" boundary.","authors":"V N Vimal Rao, Jeffrey K Bye, Sashank Varma","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00553-x","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00553-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The .05 boundary within Null Hypothesis Statistical Testing (NHST) \"has made a lot of people very angry and been widely regarded as a bad move\" (to quote Douglas Adams). Here, we move past meta-scientific arguments and ask an empirical question: What is the psychological standing of the .05 boundary for statistical significance? We find that graduate students in the psychological sciences show a boundary effect when relating p-values across .05. We propose this psychological boundary is learned through statistical training in NHST and reading a scientific literature replete with \"statistical significance\". Consistent with this proposal, undergraduates do not show the same sensitivity to the .05 boundary. Additionally, the size of a graduate student's boundary effect is not associated with their explicit endorsement of questionable research practices. These findings suggest that training creates distortions in initial processing of p-values, but these might be dampened through scientific processes operating over longer timescales.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"27"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11068716/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140866875","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distinct mechanisms of attentional suppression: exploration of trait factors underlying cued- and learned-suppression. 注意抑制的不同机制:探索提示抑制和学习抑制的特质因素。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00554-w
Matthieu Chidharom, Nancy B Carlisle

Attention allows us to focus on relevant information while ignoring distractions. Effective suppression of distracting information is crucial for efficient visual search. Recent studies have developed two paradigms to investigate attentional suppression: cued-suppression which is based on top-down control, and learned-suppression which is based on selection history. While both types of suppression reportedly engage proactive control, it remains unclear whether they rely on shared mechanisms. This study aimed to determine the relationship between cued- and learned-suppression. In a within-subjects design, 54 participants performed a cued-suppression task where pre-cues indicated upcoming target or distractor colors, and a learned-suppression task where a salient color distractor was present or absent. No significant correlation emerged between performance in the two tasks, suggesting distinct suppression mechanisms. Cued-suppression correlated with visual working memory capacity, indicating reliance on explicit control. In contrast, learned-suppression correlated with everyday distractibility, suggesting implicit control based on regularities. These results provide evidence for heterogeneous proactive control mechanisms underlying cued- and learned-suppression. While both engage inhibition, cued-suppression relies on deliberate top-down control modulated by working memory, whereas learned-suppression involves implicit suppression shaped by selection history and distractibility traits.

注意力能让我们专注于相关信息,同时忽略分散注意力的信息。有效抑制干扰信息对高效视觉搜索至关重要。最近的研究开发了两种范式来研究注意抑制:基于自上而下控制的提示抑制和基于选择历史的习得抑制。据报道,这两种类型的抑制都涉及主动控制,但它们是否依赖于共同的机制仍不清楚。本研究旨在确定提示抑制和学习抑制之间的关系。在研究对象内设计中,54 名参与者分别完成了提示抑制任务和习得抑制任务,前者的提示显示了即将出现的目标色或干扰色,而后者则显示了突出的干扰色存在或不存在。这两项任务的表现之间没有明显的相关性,这表明抑制机制各不相同。提示抑制与视觉工作记忆容量相关,表明需要依赖显性控制。与此相反,学习抑制与日常分心相关,表明基于规律性的内隐控制。这些结果为提示性抑制和习得性抑制背后的异质主动控制机制提供了证据。虽然两者都有抑制作用,但提示抑制依赖于由工作记忆调节的有意的自上而下的控制,而学习抑制则涉及由选择历史和分心特征形成的内隐抑制。
{"title":"Distinct mechanisms of attentional suppression: exploration of trait factors underlying cued- and learned-suppression.","authors":"Matthieu Chidharom, Nancy B Carlisle","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00554-w","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00554-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Attention allows us to focus on relevant information while ignoring distractions. Effective suppression of distracting information is crucial for efficient visual search. Recent studies have developed two paradigms to investigate attentional suppression: cued-suppression which is based on top-down control, and learned-suppression which is based on selection history. While both types of suppression reportedly engage proactive control, it remains unclear whether they rely on shared mechanisms. This study aimed to determine the relationship between cued- and learned-suppression. In a within-subjects design, 54 participants performed a cued-suppression task where pre-cues indicated upcoming target or distractor colors, and a learned-suppression task where a salient color distractor was present or absent. No significant correlation emerged between performance in the two tasks, suggesting distinct suppression mechanisms. Cued-suppression correlated with visual working memory capacity, indicating reliance on explicit control. In contrast, learned-suppression correlated with everyday distractibility, suggesting implicit control based on regularities. These results provide evidence for heterogeneous proactive control mechanisms underlying cued- and learned-suppression. While both engage inhibition, cued-suppression relies on deliberate top-down control modulated by working memory, whereas learned-suppression involves implicit suppression shaped by selection history and distractibility traits.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"26"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11063026/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140871809","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A bonus task boosts people's willingness to offload cognition to an algorithm. 一项奖励任务会增强人们将认知工作交给算法的意愿。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-23 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00550-0
Basil Wahn, Laura Schmitz

With the increased sophistication of technology, humans have the possibility to offload a variety of tasks to algorithms. Here, we investigated whether the extent to which people are willing to offload an attentionally demanding task to an algorithm is modulated by the availability of a bonus task and by the knowledge about the algorithm's capacity. Participants performed a multiple object tracking (MOT) task which required them to visually track targets on a screen. Participants could offload an unlimited number of targets to a "computer partner". If participants decided to offload the entire task to the computer, they could instead perform a bonus task which resulted in additional financial gain-however, this gain was conditional on a high performance accuracy in the MOT task. Thus, participants should only offload the entire task if they trusted the computer to perform accurately. We found that participants were significantly more willing to completely offload the task if they were informed beforehand that the computer's accuracy was flawless (Experiment 1 vs. 2). Participants' offloading behavior was not significantly affected by whether the bonus task was incentivized or not (Experiment 2 vs. 3). These results combined with those from our previous study (Wahn et al. in PLoS ONE 18:e0286102, 2023), which did not include a bonus task but was identical otherwise, show that the human willingness to offload an attentionally demanding task to an algorithm is considerably boosted by the availability of a bonus task-even if not incentivized-and by the knowledge about the algorithm's capacity.

随着技术的日益成熟,人类有可能将各种任务转交给算法。在这里,我们研究了人们愿意将注意力要求较高的任务转交给算法的程度是否会受到奖励任务的可用性和对算法能力的了解的影响。受试者进行了一项多目标跟踪(MOT)任务,要求他们目视跟踪屏幕上的目标。参与者可以将数量不限的目标卸载给 "电脑搭档"。如果参与者决定将整个任务卸载给计算机,他们可以执行一项奖励任务,从而获得额外的经济收益,但这种收益的条件是在 MOT 任务中表现出较高的准确性。因此,只有当参与者相信计算机能够准确完成任务时,他们才会卸下整个任务。我们发现,如果参与者事先被告知计算机的准确性是完美无瑕的,那么他们就更愿意完全卸载任务(实验 1 与实验 2)。参与者的卸载行为并没有受到是否有奖励任务的显著影响(实验 2 与实验 3)。这些结果与我们之前的研究(Wahn 等人,发表于《PLoS ONE》18:e0286102, 2023)(该研究不包括奖励任务,但其他方面完全相同)结果相结合,表明如果有奖励任务--即使没有奖励--并且知道算法的能力,人类愿意将注意力要求较高的任务卸载给算法。
{"title":"A bonus task boosts people's willingness to offload cognition to an algorithm.","authors":"Basil Wahn, Laura Schmitz","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00550-0","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00550-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>With the increased sophistication of technology, humans have the possibility to offload a variety of tasks to algorithms. Here, we investigated whether the extent to which people are willing to offload an attentionally demanding task to an algorithm is modulated by the availability of a bonus task and by the knowledge about the algorithm's capacity. Participants performed a multiple object tracking (MOT) task which required them to visually track targets on a screen. Participants could offload an unlimited number of targets to a \"computer partner\". If participants decided to offload the entire task to the computer, they could instead perform a bonus task which resulted in additional financial gain-however, this gain was conditional on a high performance accuracy in the MOT task. Thus, participants should only offload the entire task if they trusted the computer to perform accurately. We found that participants were significantly more willing to completely offload the task if they were informed beforehand that the computer's accuracy was flawless (Experiment 1 vs. 2). Participants' offloading behavior was not significantly affected by whether the bonus task was incentivized or not (Experiment 2 vs. 3). These results combined with those from our previous study (Wahn et al. in PLoS ONE 18:e0286102, 2023), which did not include a bonus task but was identical otherwise, show that the human willingness to offload an attentionally demanding task to an algorithm is considerably boosted by the availability of a bonus task-even if not incentivized-and by the knowledge about the algorithm's capacity.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"24"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11039595/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140870134","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of face coverings on audio-visual contributions to communication with conversational speech. 脸部遮挡对对话语音交流的视听贡献的影响。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-23 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00552-y
I R Jackson, E Perugia, M A Stone, G H Saunders

The use of face coverings can make communication more difficult by removing access to visual cues as well as affecting the physical transmission of speech sounds. This study aimed to assess the independent and combined contributions of visual and auditory cues to impaired communication when using face coverings. In an online task, 150 participants rated videos of natural conversation along three dimensions: (1) how much they could follow, (2) how much effort was required, and (3) the clarity of the speech. Visual and audio variables were independently manipulated in each video, so that the same video could be presented with or without a superimposed surgical-style mask, accompanied by one of four audio conditions (either unfiltered audio, or audio-filtered to simulate the attenuation associated with a surgical mask, an FFP3 mask, or a visor). Hypotheses and analyses were pre-registered. Both the audio and visual variables had a statistically significant negative impact across all three dimensions. Whether or not talkers' faces were visible made the largest contribution to participants' ratings. The study identifies a degree of attenuation whose negative effects can be overcome by the restoration of visual cues. The significant effects observed in this nominally low-demand task (speech in quiet) highlight the importance of the visual and audio cues in everyday life and that their consideration should be included in future face mask designs.

使用面部遮盖物会使人们无法获得视觉线索,并影响语音的物理传递,从而增加交流的难度。本研究旨在评估使用面部遮挡物时,视觉和听觉线索对沟通障碍的独立贡献和综合贡献。在一项在线任务中,150 名参与者从三个方面对自然对话视频进行了评分:(1) 他们能听懂多少;(2) 需要付出多少努力;(3) 语言的清晰度。每段视频中的视觉和音频变量都是独立处理的,因此在播放同一段视频时,可以叠加或不叠加外科手术式口罩,同时播放四种音频条件之一(未过滤音频,或过滤音频以模拟与外科手术口罩、FFP3 口罩或面罩相关的衰减)。假设和分析均已预先登记。在所有三个维度上,音频和视觉变量都有统计学意义上的显著负面影响。谈话者的脸是否可见对参与者的评分影响最大。这项研究确定了一定程度的衰减,其负面影响可以通过恢复视觉线索来克服。在这项名义上要求不高的任务(安静的说话环境)中观察到的重大影响突出了日常生活中视觉和听觉线索的重要性,在未来的面罩设计中应考虑到这一点。
{"title":"The impact of face coverings on audio-visual contributions to communication with conversational speech.","authors":"I R Jackson, E Perugia, M A Stone, G H Saunders","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00552-y","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00552-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The use of face coverings can make communication more difficult by removing access to visual cues as well as affecting the physical transmission of speech sounds. This study aimed to assess the independent and combined contributions of visual and auditory cues to impaired communication when using face coverings. In an online task, 150 participants rated videos of natural conversation along three dimensions: (1) how much they could follow, (2) how much effort was required, and (3) the clarity of the speech. Visual and audio variables were independently manipulated in each video, so that the same video could be presented with or without a superimposed surgical-style mask, accompanied by one of four audio conditions (either unfiltered audio, or audio-filtered to simulate the attenuation associated with a surgical mask, an FFP3 mask, or a visor). Hypotheses and analyses were pre-registered. Both the audio and visual variables had a statistically significant negative impact across all three dimensions. Whether or not talkers' faces were visible made the largest contribution to participants' ratings. The study identifies a degree of attenuation whose negative effects can be overcome by the restoration of visual cues. The significant effects observed in this nominally low-demand task (speech in quiet) highlight the importance of the visual and audio cues in everyday life and that their consideration should be included in future face mask designs.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"25"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11039583/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140871988","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1