首页 > 最新文献

Thinking & Reasoning最新文献

英文 中文
Different incubation tasks in insight problem solving: evidence for unconscious analytic thought 顿悟问题解决中的不同孵化任务:无意识分析思维的证据
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-07-11 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2022.2096694
Laura Caravona, L. Macchi
Abstract This paper explores the effect of different types of incubation task (visual, numerical and verbal) with various levels of attentional focus and cognitive effort (non-demanding, low-demanding and high-demanding) on the resolution of insight problems. The most effective was found to be the low-demanding task (regardless of its nature), which although requiring attentional focus, leaves resources available for the unconscious analytical restructuring process, obtaining a high percentage of success in solving the problem shortly after completion of the incubation task. Overall findings support the hypothesis of Unconscious Analytic Thought (UAT), according to which the restructuring required in insight problem solving implies a covert thinking process that includes a relevant, analytic and goal-oriented search. The findings are discussed in the light of UAT and are compared with the main theories of insight in problem solving.
摘要本文探讨了不同类型的潜伏任务(视觉、数字和语言)和不同水平的注意焦点和认知努力(非要求、低要求和高要求)对顿悟问题解决的影响。研究发现,最有效的是要求较低的任务(无论其性质如何),这种任务虽然需要集中注意力,但却为无意识的分析重组过程留下了资源,在完成孵化任务后不久,成功解决问题的比例很高。总体研究结果支持无意识分析思维(UAT)的假设,根据该假设,解决顿悟问题所需的重构意味着一个隐蔽的思维过程,包括相关的、分析的和目标导向的搜索。在此基础上对研究结果进行了讨论,并与解决问题的顿悟的主要理论进行了比较。
{"title":"Different incubation tasks in insight problem solving: evidence for unconscious analytic thought","authors":"Laura Caravona, L. Macchi","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2022.2096694","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2022.2096694","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This paper explores the effect of different types of incubation task (visual, numerical and verbal) with various levels of attentional focus and cognitive effort (non-demanding, low-demanding and high-demanding) on the resolution of insight problems. The most effective was found to be the low-demanding task (regardless of its nature), which although requiring attentional focus, leaves resources available for the unconscious analytical restructuring process, obtaining a high percentage of success in solving the problem shortly after completion of the incubation task. Overall findings support the hypothesis of Unconscious Analytic Thought (UAT), according to which the restructuring required in insight problem solving implies a covert thinking process that includes a relevant, analytic and goal-oriented search. The findings are discussed in the light of UAT and are compared with the main theories of insight in problem solving.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"18 1","pages":"559 - 593"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-07-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"75590206","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The link between transitive reasoning and mathematics achievement in preadolescence: the role of relational processing and deductive reasoning 传递推理与青春期前数学成就之间的联系:关系加工和演绎推理的作用
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-07-05 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2022.2095031
T. Wong, Kinga Morsanyi
Abstract The link between logic and mathematics has been recognized by theorists from different fields, but empirical investigations on this link remain scarce, and even less is known about the mechanisms underlying such a link. The current study aimed to address this issue by investigating the relations between transitive reasoning and children’s mathematical competence, and exploring the potential mechanisms involved. A sample of 101 sixth graders were assessed on their transitive reasoning skills, their mathematics achievement, various potential mediators, as well as general cognitive factors (serving as control variables). The results showed that children’s transitive reasoning performance was significantly related to their mathematics achievement beyond the effects of general cognitive factors, and this relation was mediated by fraction knowledge and Relation to Operands understanding. The findings not only confirmed the relation between transitive reasoning and mathematical competence, but also shed light on the mechanisms underlying such a relation.
逻辑与数学之间的联系已经被不同领域的理论家所认识,但对这种联系的实证研究仍然很少,对这种联系的机制了解更少。本研究旨在探讨传递推理与儿童数学能力之间的关系,并探讨其中的潜在机制。对101名六年级学生的传递推理能力、数学成绩、各种潜在中介因素以及一般认知因素(作为控制变量)进行了评估。结果表明,在一般认知因素的影响下,儿童的传递推理能力与数学成绩之间存在显著的关系,这种关系在分数知识和对操作数理解的关系中起中介作用。这些发现不仅证实了传递推理和数学能力之间的关系,而且揭示了这种关系背后的机制。
{"title":"The link between transitive reasoning and mathematics achievement in preadolescence: the role of relational processing and deductive reasoning","authors":"T. Wong, Kinga Morsanyi","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2022.2095031","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2022.2095031","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The link between logic and mathematics has been recognized by theorists from different fields, but empirical investigations on this link remain scarce, and even less is known about the mechanisms underlying such a link. The current study aimed to address this issue by investigating the relations between transitive reasoning and children’s mathematical competence, and exploring the potential mechanisms involved. A sample of 101 sixth graders were assessed on their transitive reasoning skills, their mathematics achievement, various potential mediators, as well as general cognitive factors (serving as control variables). The results showed that children’s transitive reasoning performance was significantly related to their mathematics achievement beyond the effects of general cognitive factors, and this relation was mediated by fraction knowledge and Relation to Operands understanding. The findings not only confirmed the relation between transitive reasoning and mathematical competence, but also shed light on the mechanisms underlying such a relation.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"26 1","pages":"531 - 558"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-07-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72927973","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Information about the human causes of global warming influences causal attribution, concern, and policy support related to global warming 关于全球变暖的人类原因的信息影响与全球变暖有关的因果归因、关注和政策支持
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2022.2030407
Parrish Bergquist, J. Marlon, Matthew H. Goldberg, Abel Gustafson, S. Rosenthal, A. Leiserowitz
Abstract Scientists know that human activities, primarily fossil fuel combustion, are causing Earth’s temperature to increase. Yet in 2021, only 60% of the US population understood that human activities are the primary cause of global warming. We experimentally test whether information about the human causes of global warming influences Americans’ beliefs and concerns about global warming and support for climate policies. We find that communicating information about the human-causes of global warming increases public understanding that global warming is human-caused. This information, both alone and with additional information about climate impacts and policy solutions, also increases public concern about global warming and support for climate policies, although the effects on climate concern and policy support are smaller. Importantly, the treatment effects are consistent across political party, with no backlash effects among Republicans. This suggests that when informed about climate change causes, impacts and solutions, most Americans can update their own climate change beliefs, risk perceptions, and policy support.
科学家们知道,人类活动,主要是化石燃料的燃烧,正在导致地球温度升高。然而在2021年,只有60%的美国人明白人类活动是全球变暖的主要原因。我们通过实验检验了关于全球变暖的人类原因的信息是否会影响美国人对全球变暖的信念和关注以及对气候政策的支持。我们发现,传播有关全球变暖的人为原因的信息,增加了公众对全球变暖是人为造成的认识。这些信息,无论是单独的信息还是与有关气候影响和政策解决方案的其他信息一起,也增加了公众对全球变暖的关注和对气候政策的支持,尽管对气候关注和政策支持的影响较小。重要的是,治疗效果在整个政党中是一致的,在共和党人中没有反弹效应。这表明,当了解气候变化的原因、影响和解决方案时,大多数美国人可以更新自己的气候变化信念、风险认知和政策支持。
{"title":"Information about the human causes of global warming influences causal attribution, concern, and policy support related to global warming","authors":"Parrish Bergquist, J. Marlon, Matthew H. Goldberg, Abel Gustafson, S. Rosenthal, A. Leiserowitz","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2022.2030407","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2022.2030407","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Scientists know that human activities, primarily fossil fuel combustion, are causing Earth’s temperature to increase. Yet in 2021, only 60% of the US population understood that human activities are the primary cause of global warming. We experimentally test whether information about the human causes of global warming influences Americans’ beliefs and concerns about global warming and support for climate policies. We find that communicating information about the human-causes of global warming increases public understanding that global warming is human-caused. This information, both alone and with additional information about climate impacts and policy solutions, also increases public concern about global warming and support for climate policies, although the effects on climate concern and policy support are smaller. Importantly, the treatment effects are consistent across political party, with no backlash effects among Republicans. This suggests that when informed about climate change causes, impacts and solutions, most Americans can update their own climate change beliefs, risk perceptions, and policy support.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"21 1","pages":"465 - 486"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"78065176","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Flexible use of tactics in Sudoku 灵活使用数独战术
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-06-30 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2022.2091040
Thea Behrens, Max Räuker, Michelle Kalbfleisch, F. Jäkel
Abstract When solving problems people flexibly apply different tactics. Here, we use Sudoku to study this flexibility. In a think-aloud study participants use at least two tactics, a cell-based and a number-based tactic, and have personal preferences for one or the other. Response times in two follow-up experiments indicate that participants can be biased towards either tactic by task instructions and task requirements. We argue that previous research often used biasing task designs and therefore underestimated participants’ flexibility and overestimated the importance of a problem’s complexity. Furthermore, our experiments demonstrate that if a tactic does not lead to a solution, participants are able to switch to the other. We model each tactic and we show that only by incorporating switching we can fit the data.
在解决问题时,人们灵活地运用不同的策略。在这里,我们用数独来研究这种灵活性。在一项有声思考研究中,参与者至少使用两种策略,一种基于细胞的策略和一种基于数字的策略,并且对其中一种有个人偏好。两个后续实验的反应时间表明,参与者可以根据任务指示和任务要求偏向于任何一种策略。我们认为,以前的研究经常使用有偏见的任务设计,因此低估了参与者的灵活性,高估了问题复杂性的重要性。此外,我们的实验表明,如果一种策略不能带来解决方案,参与者可以切换到另一种策略。我们对每一种策略都进行了建模,并证明只有通过合并交换才能拟合数据。
{"title":"Flexible use of tactics in Sudoku","authors":"Thea Behrens, Max Räuker, Michelle Kalbfleisch, F. Jäkel","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2022.2091040","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2022.2091040","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract When solving problems people flexibly apply different tactics. Here, we use Sudoku to study this flexibility. In a think-aloud study participants use at least two tactics, a cell-based and a number-based tactic, and have personal preferences for one or the other. Response times in two follow-up experiments indicate that participants can be biased towards either tactic by task instructions and task requirements. We argue that previous research often used biasing task designs and therefore underestimated participants’ flexibility and overestimated the importance of a problem’s complexity. Furthermore, our experiments demonstrate that if a tactic does not lead to a solution, participants are able to switch to the other. We model each tactic and we show that only by incorporating switching we can fit the data.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"16 1","pages":"488 - 530"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-06-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"78138626","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Voters’ wishful thinking in an unprecedented event of three national elections repeated within one year: fast thinking, bias, high emotions and potential rationality 在一年内接连三次全国大选这一史无前例的事件中,选民们一厢情愿的想法是:思维敏捷、偏见、情绪高涨、潜在的理性
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-05-18 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2022.2071990
Refael Tikochinski, Elisha Y. Babad
Abstract Wishful thinking (WT) of Israeli voters was measured in the unprecedented event of three failing national elections repeated within one year. WT is considered as Type 1 fast/intuitive thinking leading to bias. A novel method for measuring WT – including relevant campaign information and distinguishing between “WT for self” and “WT for others” – was introduced. WT components of voters in leading and trailing camps were compared across the three elections to examine whether patterns would be consistent or haphazard. Despite the presumed uncontrolled nature of WT, the patterns were not haphazard but rather consistent. We also tried to debias WT by applying a novel variation of incentive intervention to shift respondents from fast thinking to slow, rational Type 2 thinking. Unlike past studies, the method was extremely effective in debiasing WT and leading to realistic predictions. However, this was true only for self-WT and not for WT for others.
以色列选民的一厢情愿(WT)是在一年内连续三次失败的全国选举中前所未有的事件中测量的。WT被认为是导致偏见的1型快速/直觉思维。提出了一种新的测量小波变换的方法,包括相关的运动信息,并区分“为自己的小波变换”和“为他人的小波变换”。在三次选举中,比较了领先阵营和落后阵营中选民的WT成分,以检查模式是一致的还是偶然的。尽管假定WT不受控制的性质,但模式不是偶然的,而是一致的。我们还试图通过应用一种新颖的激励干预来消除WT的偏见,将受访者从快速思维转变为缓慢、理性的2型思维。与过去的研究不同,该方法在消除小波变换偏倚方面非常有效,并导致了现实的预测。然而,这只适用于自身WT,而不适用于他人WT。
{"title":"Voters’ wishful thinking in an unprecedented event of three national elections repeated within one year: fast thinking, bias, high emotions and potential rationality","authors":"Refael Tikochinski, Elisha Y. Babad","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2022.2071990","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2022.2071990","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Wishful thinking (WT) of Israeli voters was measured in the unprecedented event of three failing national elections repeated within one year. WT is considered as Type 1 fast/intuitive thinking leading to bias. A novel method for measuring WT – including relevant campaign information and distinguishing between “WT for self” and “WT for others” – was introduced. WT components of voters in leading and trailing camps were compared across the three elections to examine whether patterns would be consistent or haphazard. Despite the presumed uncontrolled nature of WT, the patterns were not haphazard but rather consistent. We also tried to debias WT by applying a novel variation of incentive intervention to shift respondents from fast thinking to slow, rational Type 2 thinking. Unlike past studies, the method was extremely effective in debiasing WT and leading to realistic predictions. However, this was true only for self-WT and not for WT for others.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"38 1","pages":"250 - 275"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-05-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"88752399","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Elaborative feedback and instruction improve cognitive reflection but do not transfer to related tasks 精细化的反馈和指导改善了认知反射,但不能转移到相关任务
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-05-11 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2022.2075035
D. Calvillo, Jonathan Bratton, Victoria C. Velazquez, Thomas J. Smelter, D. Crum
Abstract Cognitive reflection, or the ability to inhibit intuitive and incorrect responses in favour of correct responses, predicts performance on a variety of cognitive tasks. The present study examined interventions to improve cognitive reflection. In two experiments, college students (N = 491) were assigned to one of three conditions, completed two versions of a cognitive reflection test (CRT), and then completed transfer tasks. Between the two CRTs, some participants were provided with elaborative feedback, others were instructed to consider additional responses for their initial responses and the final group was a control. In both experiments, CRT performance increased between the first and second CRT in the feedback and instruction groups, but not in the control group. There was little evidence, however, for transfer to other tasks. These results suggest that cognitive reflection performance can be improved with brief interventions but that this improvement may not transfer to related tasks.
认知反射,或抑制直觉和错误反应的能力,有利于正确的反应,预测在各种认知任务中的表现。本研究考察了改善认知反射的干预措施。在两个实验中,491名大学生被分配到三种条件中的一种,完成两种版本的认知反射测试(CRT),然后完成迁移任务。在两组crt之间,一些参与者得到了详细的反馈,另一些参与者被指示考虑对他们最初的回答进行额外的回答,最后一组是对照组。在这两个实验中,反馈组和指导组的CRT表现在第一次和第二次CRT之间有所提高,而对照组则没有。然而,几乎没有证据表明他们可以转移到其他任务中。这些结果表明,认知反射表现可以通过简短的干预得到改善,但这种改善可能不会转移到相关任务中。
{"title":"Elaborative feedback and instruction improve cognitive reflection but do not transfer to related tasks","authors":"D. Calvillo, Jonathan Bratton, Victoria C. Velazquez, Thomas J. Smelter, D. Crum","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2022.2075035","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2022.2075035","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Cognitive reflection, or the ability to inhibit intuitive and incorrect responses in favour of correct responses, predicts performance on a variety of cognitive tasks. The present study examined interventions to improve cognitive reflection. In two experiments, college students (N = 491) were assigned to one of three conditions, completed two versions of a cognitive reflection test (CRT), and then completed transfer tasks. Between the two CRTs, some participants were provided with elaborative feedback, others were instructed to consider additional responses for their initial responses and the final group was a control. In both experiments, CRT performance increased between the first and second CRT in the feedback and instruction groups, but not in the control group. There was little evidence, however, for transfer to other tasks. These results suggest that cognitive reflection performance can be improved with brief interventions but that this improvement may not transfer to related tasks.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"64 1","pages":"276 - 304"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-05-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"73218782","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Bullshit receptivity, problem solving, and metacognition: simply the BS, not better than all the rest 接受胡扯,解决问题,和元认知:只是胡扯,并不比其他的都好
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-05-02 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2022.2066724
Tim George, Marta K. Mielicki
Abstract People are often inaccurate in their predictions of performance on a variety of cognitive tasks. We tested whether receptivity to bullshit – the tendency to perceive meaningless statements as profound – would relate to the accuracy of metacognitive judgments on several problem-solving tasks. Individuals who were highly receptive to bullshit were less accurate in their predictions of performance on creative problem-solving tasks, but not on verbal analogy or recall tasks. Further, individuals with high BS receptivity were less able to discriminate between solvable and unsolvable problems when making metacognitive judgments. These findings support the possibility that the tendency to perceive semantic connections where none exist, as indicated by high bullshit receptivity, may lead to inaccurate predictions of performance on tasks that require noticing and utilizing distant semantic connections.
人们对各种认知任务的预测往往是不准确的。我们测试了对胡扯的接受度——将无意义的陈述理解为深刻的倾向——是否与在几个问题解决任务中元认知判断的准确性有关。高度接受胡扯的人在创造性解决问题的任务中对表现的预测不太准确,但在口头类比或回忆任务中则不然。此外,高BS接受度的个体在进行元认知判断时,区分可解决和不可解决问题的能力较差。这些发现支持了一种可能性,即在不存在语义连接的情况下感知语义连接的倾向,正如高废话接受度所表明的那样,可能导致对需要注意和利用远距离语义连接的任务的表现预测不准确。
{"title":"Bullshit receptivity, problem solving, and metacognition: simply the BS, not better than all the rest","authors":"Tim George, Marta K. Mielicki","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2022.2066724","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2022.2066724","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract People are often inaccurate in their predictions of performance on a variety of cognitive tasks. We tested whether receptivity to bullshit – the tendency to perceive meaningless statements as profound – would relate to the accuracy of metacognitive judgments on several problem-solving tasks. Individuals who were highly receptive to bullshit were less accurate in their predictions of performance on creative problem-solving tasks, but not on verbal analogy or recall tasks. Further, individuals with high BS receptivity were less able to discriminate between solvable and unsolvable problems when making metacognitive judgments. These findings support the possibility that the tendency to perceive semantic connections where none exist, as indicated by high bullshit receptivity, may lead to inaccurate predictions of performance on tasks that require noticing and utilizing distant semantic connections.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"5 1","pages":"213 - 249"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-05-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87610504","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Out of the blue: on the suddenness of perceived chance events Out of the blue:突如其来的意外事件
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-09 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2022.2047105
K. Teigen, Alf Børre Kanten
Abstract People commonly use terms like ‘random’, ‘by chance’, or ‘accidentally’ when they describe occurrences that sidestep the normal course of events, with no apparent causal link to ongoing activities. Such intrusive events are typically perceived as happening all of a sudden. This was demonstrated in seven experiments (N = 1299) by asking people to identify statements they believed belonged to stories about chance events, and by comparing chance vs. non-chance events from their own life and from the lives of others. Suddenness also appeared to be more strongly associated with beginnings than with endings of an episode. Thus, statements about sudden beginnings of a career or a relationship were more readily believed to happen ‘by chance’ than ‘not by chance’. But life-changing events did not become more accidental simply by being placed in the beginning of a ‘life chapter’. These studies highlight a central but neglected feature of perceived randomness.
人们通常使用“随机”、“偶然”或“意外”等术语来描述那些避开正常过程的事件,这些事件与正在进行的活动没有明显的因果关系。这种侵入性事件通常被认为是突然发生的。这在七个实验(N = 1299)中得到了证明,通过要求人们识别他们认为属于偶然事件故事的陈述,并通过比较他们自己和他人生活中的偶然事件与非偶然事件。突然性似乎与事件的开始而不是结束有更强的联系。因此,人们更容易相信一段事业或一段感情的突然开始是“偶然的”,而不是“偶然的”。但改变人生的事件不会因为被放在“人生篇章”的开头而变得更加偶然。这些研究强调了感知随机性的一个核心但被忽视的特征。
{"title":"Out of the blue: on the suddenness of perceived chance events","authors":"K. Teigen, Alf Børre Kanten","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2022.2047105","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2022.2047105","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract People commonly use terms like ‘random’, ‘by chance’, or ‘accidentally’ when they describe occurrences that sidestep the normal course of events, with no apparent causal link to ongoing activities. Such intrusive events are typically perceived as happening all of a sudden. This was demonstrated in seven experiments (N = 1299) by asking people to identify statements they believed belonged to stories about chance events, and by comparing chance vs. non-chance events from their own life and from the lives of others. Suddenness also appeared to be more strongly associated with beginnings than with endings of an episode. Thus, statements about sudden beginnings of a career or a relationship were more readily believed to happen ‘by chance’ than ‘not by chance’. But life-changing events did not become more accidental simply by being placed in the beginning of a ‘life chapter’. These studies highlight a central but neglected feature of perceived randomness.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"37 1","pages":"137 - 175"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-03-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"85043660","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Metacognitive control in single- vs. dual-process theory 单过程与双过程理论中的元认知控制
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-07 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2022.2047106
C. Dewey
Abstract Recent work in cognitive modelling has found that most of the data that has been cited as evidence for the dual-process theory (DPT) of reasoning is best explained by non-linear, “monotonic” one-process models. In this paper, I consider an important caveat of this research: it uses models that are committed to unrealistic assumptions about how effectively task conditions can isolate Type-1 and Type-2 reasoning. To avoid this caveat, I develop a coordinated theoretical, experimental, and modelling strategy to better test DPT. First, I propose that Type-1 and Type-2 reasoning are defined as reasoning that precedes and follows metacognitive control, respectively. Second, I argue that reasoning that precedes and follows metacognitive control can be effectively isolated using debiasing paradigms that manipulate metacognitive heuristics (e.g., processing fluency) to prevent or trigger metacognitive control, respectively. Third, I argue that monotonic modelling can allow us to decisively test DPT only when we use them to analyse data from this particular kind of debiasing paradigm.
最近在认知建模方面的研究发现,大多数被引用作为推理双过程理论(DPT)证据的数据都可以用非线性的、“单调的”单过程模型来最好地解释。在本文中,我考虑了这项研究的一个重要警告:它使用的模型致力于不切实际的假设,即任务条件如何有效地隔离类型1和类型2推理。为了避免这种警告,我开发了一个协调的理论、实验和建模策略来更好地测试DPT。首先,我建议将1型和2型推理分别定义为在元认知控制之前和之后的推理。其次,我认为,在元认知控制之前和之后的推理可以使用去偏见范式有效地隔离,这些范式分别操纵元认知启发式(例如,处理流畅性)来防止或触发元认知控制。第三,我认为,只有当我们使用单调模型来分析这种特殊的去偏范式的数据时,单调模型才能允许我们果断地测试DPT。
{"title":"Metacognitive control in single- vs. dual-process theory","authors":"C. Dewey","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2022.2047106","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2022.2047106","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Recent work in cognitive modelling has found that most of the data that has been cited as evidence for the dual-process theory (DPT) of reasoning is best explained by non-linear, “monotonic” one-process models. In this paper, I consider an important caveat of this research: it uses models that are committed to unrealistic assumptions about how effectively task conditions can isolate Type-1 and Type-2 reasoning. To avoid this caveat, I develop a coordinated theoretical, experimental, and modelling strategy to better test DPT. First, I propose that Type-1 and Type-2 reasoning are defined as reasoning that precedes and follows metacognitive control, respectively. Second, I argue that reasoning that precedes and follows metacognitive control can be effectively isolated using debiasing paradigms that manipulate metacognitive heuristics (e.g., processing fluency) to prevent or trigger metacognitive control, respectively. Third, I argue that monotonic modelling can allow us to decisively test DPT only when we use them to analyse data from this particular kind of debiasing paradigm.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"116 1","pages":"177 - 212"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-03-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"80372308","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Searching for the cognitive basis of anti-vaccination attitudes 寻找反疫苗接种态度的认知基础
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-04 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2022.2046158
M. Lindeman, Annika M. Svedholm-Häkkinen, T. Riekki
Abstract Research on the reasons for vaccine hesitancy has largely focused on factors directly related to vaccines. In contrast, the present study focused on cognitive factors that are not conceptually related to vaccines but that have been linked to other epistemically suspect beliefs such as conspiracy theories and belief in fake news. This survey was conducted before the Covid-19 pandemic (N = 356). The results showed that anti-vaccination attitudes decreased slightly with cognitive abilities and analytic thinking styles, and strongly with scientific literacy. In addition, anti-vaccination attitudes increased slightly with teleological bias and strongly with an intuitive thinking style, ontological biases, and religious and paranormal beliefs. The results suggest that the same cognitive mechanisms that predispose to other epistemically suspect beliefs may predispose to anti-vaccination attitudes as well. The findings also indicate that pro-vaccination communication should focus on early prevention and that interventions against vaccine hesitancy should strive to be intuitively appealing.
对疫苗犹豫原因的研究主要集中在与疫苗直接相关的因素上。相比之下,本研究侧重于认知因素,这些因素在概念上与疫苗无关,但与其他认知上可疑的信念有关,如阴谋论和对假新闻的信仰。本次调查是在新冠肺炎大流行之前进行的(N = 356)。结果表明,反疫苗接种态度随认知能力和分析思维方式的变化而略有下降,随科学素养的变化而明显下降。此外,反疫苗接种态度在目的论偏见中略有增加,而在直觉思维方式、本体论偏见、宗教和超自然信仰中强烈增加。结果表明,倾向于其他认知怀疑信念的相同认知机制也可能倾向于反疫苗接种态度。研究结果还表明,支持接种疫苗的宣传应侧重于早期预防,针对疫苗犹豫的干预措施应努力在直观上吸引人。
{"title":"Searching for the cognitive basis of anti-vaccination attitudes","authors":"M. Lindeman, Annika M. Svedholm-Häkkinen, T. Riekki","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2022.2046158","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2022.2046158","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Research on the reasons for vaccine hesitancy has largely focused on factors directly related to vaccines. In contrast, the present study focused on cognitive factors that are not conceptually related to vaccines but that have been linked to other epistemically suspect beliefs such as conspiracy theories and belief in fake news. This survey was conducted before the Covid-19 pandemic (N = 356). The results showed that anti-vaccination attitudes decreased slightly with cognitive abilities and analytic thinking styles, and strongly with scientific literacy. In addition, anti-vaccination attitudes increased slightly with teleological bias and strongly with an intuitive thinking style, ontological biases, and religious and paranormal beliefs. The results suggest that the same cognitive mechanisms that predispose to other epistemically suspect beliefs may predispose to anti-vaccination attitudes as well. The findings also indicate that pro-vaccination communication should focus on early prevention and that interventions against vaccine hesitancy should strive to be intuitively appealing.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"31 1","pages":"111 - 136"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-03-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77888160","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
期刊
Thinking & Reasoning
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1