首页 > 最新文献

Thinking & Reasoning最新文献

英文 中文
The skeptical import of motivated reasoning: a closer look at the evidence 动机推理的怀疑意义:仔细观察证据
3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-08 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2023.2276975
Maarten van Doorn
Central to many discussions of motivated reasoning is the idea that it runs afoul of epistemic normativity. Reasoning differently about information supporting our prior beliefs versus information contradicting those beliefs, is frequently equated with motivated irrationality. By analyzing the normative status of belief polarization, selective scrutiny, biased assimilation and the myside bias, I show this inference is often not adequately supported. Contrary to what’s often assumed, these phenomena need not indicate motivated irrationality, even though they are instances of belief-consistent information processing. Second, I engage with arguments purporting to show that belief-consistent information processing does not indicate motivated irrationality because of its mere differential treatment of confirming and non-confirming evidence, but rather because it reveals the undermining presence of an irrelevant influence, such as a desire or partisan identity-driven cognition. While linking belief-consistent reasoning to a deeper source of directional motivation to make good on the claim that it indicates motivated irrationality is indeed what’s needed, two prominent such arguments fail. The non-normativity of many reasoning processes often taken to indicate motivated irrationality is not in fact well established.
许多关于动机推理的讨论的中心思想是它与认知规范性相冲突。对支持我们先前信念的信息和与这些信念相矛盾的信息进行不同的推理,通常等同于有动机的非理性。通过分析信仰极化、选择性审查、偏见同化和我方偏见的规范地位,我表明这种推论往往得不到充分的支持。与通常的假设相反,这些现象不一定表明有动机的非理性,即使它们是信念一致的信息处理的实例。其次,我参与的论点旨在表明,信念一致的信息处理并不表明动机不合理,因为它仅仅是对确认和非确认证据的区别对待,而是因为它揭示了不相关影响的破坏存在,例如欲望或党派身份驱动的认知。虽然将信念一致的推理与方向性动机的更深层次的来源联系起来,以证明它表明动机性非理性确实是需要的,但两个突出的论点失败了。许多推理过程的非规范性通常被用来表示动机的非理性,实际上并没有很好地建立起来。
{"title":"The skeptical import of motivated reasoning: a closer look at the evidence","authors":"Maarten van Doorn","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2023.2276975","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2023.2276975","url":null,"abstract":"Central to many discussions of motivated reasoning is the idea that it runs afoul of epistemic normativity. Reasoning differently about information supporting our prior beliefs versus information contradicting those beliefs, is frequently equated with motivated irrationality. By analyzing the normative status of belief polarization, selective scrutiny, biased assimilation and the myside bias, I show this inference is often not adequately supported. Contrary to what’s often assumed, these phenomena need not indicate motivated irrationality, even though they are instances of belief-consistent information processing. Second, I engage with arguments purporting to show that belief-consistent information processing does not indicate motivated irrationality because of its mere differential treatment of confirming and non-confirming evidence, but rather because it reveals the undermining presence of an irrelevant influence, such as a desire or partisan identity-driven cognition. While linking belief-consistent reasoning to a deeper source of directional motivation to make good on the claim that it indicates motivated irrationality is indeed what’s needed, two prominent such arguments fail. The non-normativity of many reasoning processes often taken to indicate motivated irrationality is not in fact well established.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"23 34","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135390647","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
When word frequency meets word order: factors determining multiply-constrained creative association 当词频满足词序:决定多重约束创造性联想的因素
3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-10-28 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2023.2259546
Wangbing Shen, Bernhard Hommel, Yuan Yuan, Qiping Ren, Meifeng Hua, Fang Lu
AbstractCreative association is inherent and essential to creativity and insight. Here we utilised a Chinese compound Remote Associates Task (cRAT) to identify the potential impact of word order (i.e., solution position hereinafter) and word frequency on creative association across two behavioural experiments. Experiment 1 identified the effects of (a) word order and word frequency on cRAT-induced association without considering the specific strategies used during solving such problems and (b) their interaction not only on performance in solving the cRAT, including solution time and accuracy, but also on difficulty rating. Following a coarse-to-fine approach, Experiment 2 examined the effect of both factors on creative association determined according to each participant’s trial-by-trial reports regarding insight solution strategies. Main effects of word order and word frequency, and a two-way interaction, were found on the accuracy and self-rated difficulty. These findings shed light on creative association, multiply-constrained problem solving and constructing compounds.Keywords: Creative associationword frequencysolution positionword orderproblem solving Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Notes1 Word frequency was mainly determined according to a well-known and widely used word frequency dictionary, listing more than 1.8 million Chinese words or characters, published by the Beijing Language College Institute of Language Studies (Citation1986; the English version is available from the University of Virginia). If the frequency of a word was fewer than 10 times, with a percentage of cumulative frequency less than 5% (the taken threshold of 5% is largely based on a general threshold view drawn from the widely used statistical significance of p < 0.05 in psychological studies wherein an event with a probability/frequency of occurrence of less than 5% is considered a small probability or uncommon/infrequent event. Actually, the word-frequency dictionary authors also considered a word with fewer than 10 appearances, namely, word frequency, as a low-frequency word, which is also manifested in their sampling process), it was considered a low-frequency word; otherwise, it was viewed as a high-frequency word (not fewer than 10 times), occupying more than 95% in terms of percentage of cumulative frequency. It should be noted that the selected items in this study were from an original set of 192 developed items, such that some confounding variables (e.g., chronological effect) were excluded.2 In these two experiments, participants were instructed to try their best to solve each cRAT problem and simultaneously process them. To reduce the complexity and difficulty in design and statistical analysis, the three Chinese characters of a cRAT problem was controlled (not fixed but randomly), and presented simultaneously (not sequentially).3 Here we provided a brief description on the simple effect of two-way interac
摘要创造性联想是创造力和洞察力的内在和必要条件。本研究采用汉语复合词远端联想任务(cRAT),在两个行为实验中确定词序(即解决方案位置)和词频对创造性联想的潜在影响。实验1确定了(a)词序和词频在不考虑解决这些问题时使用的具体策略的情况下对cRAT诱导联想的影响;(b)它们的相互作用不仅对解决cRAT的表现(包括解决时间和准确性)有影响,而且对难度评级也有影响。实验2采用从粗到精的方法,根据每个参与者关于洞察力解决策略的逐次报告,检验了这两个因素对创造性联想的影响。词序和词频对准确性和自评难度有主要影响,并存在双向交互作用。这些发现揭示了创造性联想,多重约束问题解决和构建化合物。关键词:创造性联想词频解题位置词序解题披露声明作者未报告潜在的利益冲突。注1词频的确定主要依据北京语言学院语言研究所出版的一部著名的、被广泛使用的收录了180多万个汉字的词频词典(Citation1986;英文版可从弗吉尼亚大学获得)。如果一个词出现的频率小于10次,累计频率的百分比小于5%(5%的阈值很大程度上是基于心理学研究中广泛使用的统计学显著性p < 0.05得出的一般阈值观点,其中发生概率/频率小于5%的事件被认为是小概率或不常见/不常见的事件。)实际上,词频词典的作者也将出现次数少于10次的词,即词频,视为低频词(这也体现在他们的采样过程中),认为是低频词;否则,它被视为高频词(不少于10次),占累积频率的百分比超过95%。值得注意的是,本研究中所选择的项目来自192个开发项目的原始集合,因此排除了一些混淆变量(例如,时间顺序效应)在这两个实验中,参与者被要求尽最大努力解决每个cRAT问题并同时处理它们。为了降低设计和统计分析的复杂性和难度,对一个cRAT问题的三个汉字进行了控制(不是固定的而是随机的),并同时(不是顺序)呈现在这里,我们简要描述了双向交互对准确性的简单影响,更多细节可以联系第一作者。结果:HHHFFF > HLLIII, HHHIFF > HHHIII, HHHIFF > HLLIII, HHHIFF > LLLIFF, HHHIFF > LLLIIF, HHHIIF > HHHIII, HHHIIF > HLLIII, HHHIIF > LLLIFF, HHHIIF > LLLIIF, HHLFFF > HLLIII, HHLIFF > HLLIII, HHLIIF > HLLIII, HHLIIF > LLLIFF, HHLIIF > LLLIIF, HHLIII > HLLIII, HLLIFF > HLLIII, HLLFFF > HLLIII, HLLIIF > HLLIII > LLLFFF,和HLLIII > LLLIII,其中“>”只表示两种类型之间的区别是前者比后者更大层面的p < . 05。基于这些影响,我们相信词频可能以u形方式而不是线性方式工作,这仍然是一个悬而未决的问题,需要在未来进行更仔细的评估。本研究得到国家社会科学基金项目(20BXW118)资助。第一作者还获得了江苏省第六期高层次人才培养项目(333)和江苏省心理与认知科学大数据建设重点实验室科研基金(No. 72592162002G)的资助,袁博士获得了江苏省高等学校江苏青蓝项目和江苏省高校自然科学基金(No. 23KJB180016)的资助。
{"title":"When word frequency meets word order: factors determining multiply-constrained creative association","authors":"Wangbing Shen, Bernhard Hommel, Yuan Yuan, Qiping Ren, Meifeng Hua, Fang Lu","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2023.2259546","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2023.2259546","url":null,"abstract":"AbstractCreative association is inherent and essential to creativity and insight. Here we utilised a Chinese compound Remote Associates Task (cRAT) to identify the potential impact of word order (i.e., solution position hereinafter) and word frequency on creative association across two behavioural experiments. Experiment 1 identified the effects of (a) word order and word frequency on cRAT-induced association without considering the specific strategies used during solving such problems and (b) their interaction not only on performance in solving the cRAT, including solution time and accuracy, but also on difficulty rating. Following a coarse-to-fine approach, Experiment 2 examined the effect of both factors on creative association determined according to each participant’s trial-by-trial reports regarding insight solution strategies. Main effects of word order and word frequency, and a two-way interaction, were found on the accuracy and self-rated difficulty. These findings shed light on creative association, multiply-constrained problem solving and constructing compounds.Keywords: Creative associationword frequencysolution positionword orderproblem solving Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Notes1 Word frequency was mainly determined according to a well-known and widely used word frequency dictionary, listing more than 1.8 million Chinese words or characters, published by the Beijing Language College Institute of Language Studies (Citation1986; the English version is available from the University of Virginia). If the frequency of a word was fewer than 10 times, with a percentage of cumulative frequency less than 5% (the taken threshold of 5% is largely based on a general threshold view drawn from the widely used statistical significance of p &lt; 0.05 in psychological studies wherein an event with a probability/frequency of occurrence of less than 5% is considered a small probability or uncommon/infrequent event. Actually, the word-frequency dictionary authors also considered a word with fewer than 10 appearances, namely, word frequency, as a low-frequency word, which is also manifested in their sampling process), it was considered a low-frequency word; otherwise, it was viewed as a high-frequency word (not fewer than 10 times), occupying more than 95% in terms of percentage of cumulative frequency. It should be noted that the selected items in this study were from an original set of 192 developed items, such that some confounding variables (e.g., chronological effect) were excluded.2 In these two experiments, participants were instructed to try their best to solve each cRAT problem and simultaneously process them. To reduce the complexity and difficulty in design and statistical analysis, the three Chinese characters of a cRAT problem was controlled (not fixed but randomly), and presented simultaneously (not sequentially).3 Here we provided a brief description on the simple effect of two-way interac","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"216 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136158572","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mindset effects on the regulation of thinking time in problem-solving 心态对解决问题时思考时间的调节作用
3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-10-27 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2023.2259550
Rakefet Ackerman, Liat Levontin
AbstractUnderstanding time investment while solving problems is central to metacognitive research. By the Diminishing Criterion Model (DCM), time regulation is guided by two stopping rules: a confidence criterion that drops as time is invested in each problem and the maximum time to be invested. This combination generates curved confidence–time associations. We compared the belief that intelligence is malleable, a growth mindset, to the belief that intelligence is fixed, and to neutral control groups. We hypothesized that a growth mindset leads people to selectively invest time in problems carrying the hope of improvement. This extra time makes the curved DCM pattern curvier. In two experiments, participants primed with growth, fixed, or control mindsets solved analogies (Experiment 1) and compound remote associates (Experiment 2). As expected, in both experiments a growth mindset exhibited a curvier confidence–time pattern, while the fixed mindset and control groups replicated previous confidence–time associations. Most additional time was invested in problems with intermediate difficulty levels, suggesting strategic time allocation. The study offers useful measures for delving into factors that affect thinking time allocation.Keywords: Growth mindsetproblem-solvingmeta-reasoningmetacognitiontime regulation AcknowledgmentsWe thank Meira Ben-Gad for editorial assistance.Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Notes1 In Ackerman, Yom-Tov, et al. (2020), the confidence scale appeared on a separate page. The rest of the solving procedure was identical.2 We used this phrase as the low end of the scale consistently across the experiments, rather than a phrase conveying guessing by chance. This was done so that the scale would start at zero in both experiments, regardless of whether the task format was multiple-choice or open-ended.3 For addressing the lack of remaining self-report difference in mindset at the end of the task, we made several additional analyses. First, we analyzed the two experiments together, examining the tendency towards a fixed mindset by an Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) with experiment and group as two between-participant factors. We found a main effect of the group, F(2, 304) = 4.95, MSE = 23.47, p = .008, η2p = .032. A Tukey post-hoc test revealed that there was a significant difference between fixed and growth mindsets, p = .005, while other differences were not significant, both ps > .09. Importantly, there was no interactive effect with experiment, F(2, 304) = 0.574, MSE = 2.72, p = .564, η2p = .004, suggesting on comparable manipulation effect on both experiments. Second, we examined order effects within the set of problems each participant solved on the central analysis for this study, of confidence-time association. Indeed, the curve difference was found in the second half of the task, β = -1.34, SE = 0.39, t(2137) = 3.47, p < .001, 95% CI [-1.44, -0.33], indicating that the effe
摘要理解解决问题时的时间投入是元认知研究的核心。根据递减准则模型(DCM),时间调节由两个停止规则指导:一个是随着每个问题投入时间的增加而下降的置信准则,另一个是投入的最大时间。这种组合产生了弯曲的信心时间关联。我们比较了智力是可塑的,一种成长心态,和智力是固定的,以及中立的控制组。我们假设,成长型思维模式会导致人们有选择地把时间投入到带有改善希望的问题上。这段额外的时间使弯曲的DCM模式更加弯曲。在两个实验中,被试分别被增长型、固定型和控制型思维模式启动,分别解决了类比(实验1)和复合远程关联(实验2)。正如预期的那样,在这两个实验中,增长型思维模式呈现出曲线型的信心时间模式,而固定型思维模式和控制型思维模式重复了之前的信心时间关联。大多数额外的时间都花在了中等难度的问题上,这表明时间分配是有策略的。这项研究为深入研究影响思考时间分配的因素提供了有用的方法。关键词:成长心态解决问题元推理元认知时间调节感谢Meira Ben-Gad的编辑协助。披露声明作者未报告潜在的利益冲突。注1在Ackerman, yo - tov, et al.(2020)中,置信度量表出现在单独的页面上。其余的求解过程是相同的在整个实验中,我们一直使用这个短语作为量表的低端,而不是一个表达偶然猜测的短语。这样做是为了在两个实验中,无论任务形式是选择题还是开放式的,量表都会从零开始为了解决在任务结束时缺乏自我报告的心态差异,我们做了一些额外的分析。首先,我们一起分析了两个实验,通过方差分析(ANOVA),以实验和群体作为两个参与者之间的因素,检验了固定心态的倾向。我们发现主效应组,F(2,304) = 4.95, MSE = 23.47, p = 0.008, η2p = 0.032。Tukey事后检验显示,固定心态和成长心态之间存在显著差异,p = 0.005,而其他差异不显著,均为p = 0.09。重要的是,与实验没有交互作用,F(2,304) = 0.574, MSE = 2.72, p = 0.564, η2p = 0.004,表明两个实验的操作效果可比较。其次,我们在本研究的中心分析中检查了每个参与者解决的问题集内的顺序效应,即置信度时间关联。事实上,在任务的后半段发现了曲线差异,β = -1.34, SE = 0.39, t(2137) = 3.47, p < .001, 95% CI[-1.44, -0.33],表明这种效果一直持续到这个高要求任务的后期阶段。本研究得到了以色列科学基金会的支持。
{"title":"Mindset effects on the regulation of thinking time in problem-solving","authors":"Rakefet Ackerman, Liat Levontin","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2023.2259550","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2023.2259550","url":null,"abstract":"AbstractUnderstanding time investment while solving problems is central to metacognitive research. By the Diminishing Criterion Model (DCM), time regulation is guided by two stopping rules: a confidence criterion that drops as time is invested in each problem and the maximum time to be invested. This combination generates curved confidence–time associations. We compared the belief that intelligence is malleable, a growth mindset, to the belief that intelligence is fixed, and to neutral control groups. We hypothesized that a growth mindset leads people to selectively invest time in problems carrying the hope of improvement. This extra time makes the curved DCM pattern curvier. In two experiments, participants primed with growth, fixed, or control mindsets solved analogies (Experiment 1) and compound remote associates (Experiment 2). As expected, in both experiments a growth mindset exhibited a curvier confidence–time pattern, while the fixed mindset and control groups replicated previous confidence–time associations. Most additional time was invested in problems with intermediate difficulty levels, suggesting strategic time allocation. The study offers useful measures for delving into factors that affect thinking time allocation.Keywords: Growth mindsetproblem-solvingmeta-reasoningmetacognitiontime regulation AcknowledgmentsWe thank Meira Ben-Gad for editorial assistance.Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Notes1 In Ackerman, Yom-Tov, et al. (2020), the confidence scale appeared on a separate page. The rest of the solving procedure was identical.2 We used this phrase as the low end of the scale consistently across the experiments, rather than a phrase conveying guessing by chance. This was done so that the scale would start at zero in both experiments, regardless of whether the task format was multiple-choice or open-ended.3 For addressing the lack of remaining self-report difference in mindset at the end of the task, we made several additional analyses. First, we analyzed the two experiments together, examining the tendency towards a fixed mindset by an Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) with experiment and group as two between-participant factors. We found a main effect of the group, F(2, 304) = 4.95, MSE = 23.47, p = .008, η2p = .032. A Tukey post-hoc test revealed that there was a significant difference between fixed and growth mindsets, p = .005, while other differences were not significant, both ps &gt; .09. Importantly, there was no interactive effect with experiment, F(2, 304) = 0.574, MSE = 2.72, p = .564, η2p = .004, suggesting on comparable manipulation effect on both experiments. Second, we examined order effects within the set of problems each participant solved on the central analysis for this study, of confidence-time association. Indeed, the curve difference was found in the second half of the task, β = -1.34, SE = 0.39, t(2137) = 3.47, p &lt; .001, 95% CI [-1.44, -0.33], indicating that the effe","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"216 4","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136263580","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Elementary probabilistic operations: a framework for probabilistic reasoning 基本概率运算:概率推理的框架
3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-09-29 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2023.2259541
Siegfried Macho, Thomas Ledermann
The framework of elementary probabilistic operations (EPO) explains the structure of elementary probabilistic reasoning tasks as well as people’s performance on these tasks. The framework comprises three components: (a) Three types of probabilities: joint, marginal, and conditional probabilities; (b) three elementary probabilistic operations: combination, marginalization, and conditioning, and (c) quantitative inference schemas implementing the EPO. The formal part of the EPO framework is a computational level theory that provides a problem space representation and a classification of elementary probabilistic problems based on computational requirements for solving a problem. According to the EPO framework, current methods for improving probabilistic reasoning are of two kinds: First, reduction of Bayesian problems to a type of probabilistic problems requiring less conceptual and procedural competencies. Second, enhancing people’s utilization competence by fostering the application of quantitative inference schemas. The approach suggests new applications, including the teaching of probabilistic reasoning, using analogical problem solving in probabilistic reasoning, and new methods for analyzing errors in probabilistic problem solving.
初等概率运算框架解释了初等概率推理任务的结构以及人们在这些任务中的表现。该框架包括三个部分:(a)三种类型的概率:联合概率、边际概率和条件概率;(b)三种基本概率操作:组合、边缘化和条件作用,以及(c)实现EPO的定量推理模式。EPO框架的正式部分是一个计算级理论,它提供了一个基于解决问题的计算需求的问题空间表示和基本概率问题的分类。根据EPO框架,目前改进概率推理的方法有两种:第一,将贝叶斯问题简化为一种需要较少概念和程序能力的概率问题。第二,通过培养定量推理图式的应用,提高人们的运用能力。该方法提出了新的应用,包括概率推理的教学,在概率推理中使用类比问题解决,以及在概率问题解决中分析错误的新方法。
{"title":"Elementary probabilistic operations: a framework for probabilistic reasoning","authors":"Siegfried Macho, Thomas Ledermann","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2023.2259541","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2023.2259541","url":null,"abstract":"The framework of elementary probabilistic operations (EPO) explains the structure of elementary probabilistic reasoning tasks as well as people’s performance on these tasks. The framework comprises three components: (a) Three types of probabilities: joint, marginal, and conditional probabilities; (b) three elementary probabilistic operations: combination, marginalization, and conditioning, and (c) quantitative inference schemas implementing the EPO. The formal part of the EPO framework is a computational level theory that provides a problem space representation and a classification of elementary probabilistic problems based on computational requirements for solving a problem. According to the EPO framework, current methods for improving probabilistic reasoning are of two kinds: First, reduction of Bayesian problems to a type of probabilistic problems requiring less conceptual and procedural competencies. Second, enhancing people’s utilization competence by fostering the application of quantitative inference schemas. The approach suggests new applications, including the teaching of probabilistic reasoning, using analogical problem solving in probabilistic reasoning, and new methods for analyzing errors in probabilistic problem solving.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"118 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135199682","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Testing the underlying structure of unfounded beliefs about COVID-19 around the world 检验世界各地对COVID-19毫无根据的信念的基本结构
3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-09-28 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2023.2259539
Paweł Brzóska, Magdalena Żemojtel-Piotrowska, Jarosław Piotrowski, Bartłomiej Nowak, Peter K. Jonason, Constantine Sedikides, Mladen Adamovic, Kokou A. Atitsogbe, Oli Ahmed, Uzma Azam, Sergiu Bălțătescu, Konstantin Bochaver, Aidos Bolatov, Mario Bonato, Victor Counted, Trawin Chaleeraktrakoon, Jano Ramos-Diaz, Sonya Dragova-Koleva, Walaa Labib M. Eldesoki, Carla Sofia Esteves, Valdiney V. Gouveia, Pablo Perez de Leon, Dzintra Iliško, Jesus Alfonso D. Datu, Fanli Jia, Veljko Jovanović, Tomislav Jukić, Narine Khachatryan, Monika Kovacs, Uri Lifshin, Aitor Larzabal Fernandez, Kadi Liik, Sadia Malik, Chanki Moon, Stephan Muehlbacher, Reza Najafi, Emre Oruç, Joonha Park, Iva Poláčková Šolcová, Rahkman Ardi, Ognjen Ridic, Goran Ridic, Yadgar Ismail Said, Andrej Starc, Delia Stefenel, Kiều Thị Thanh Trà, Habib Tiliouine, Robert Tomšik, Jorge Torres-Marin, Charles S. Umeh, Eduardo Wills-Herrera, Anna Wlodarczyk, Zahir Vally, Illia Yahiiaiev
AbstractUnfounded—conspiracy and health—beliefs about COVID-19 have accompanied the pandemic worldwide. Here, we examined cross-nationally the structure and correlates of these beliefs with an 8-item scale, using a multigroup confirmatory factor analysis. We obtained a two-factor model of unfounded (conspiracy and health) beliefs with good internal structure (average CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.05, SRMR = 0.04), but a high correlation between the two factors (average latent factor correlation = 0.57). This model was replicable across 50 countries (total N = 13,579), as evidenced by metric invariance between countries (CFI = 0.96, RMSEA = 0.06, SRMS = 0.07) as well as scalar invariance across genders (CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.04, SRMS = 0.03) and educational levels (CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.04, SRMS = 0.03). Also, lower levels of education, more fear of COVID-19, and more cynicism were weakly associated with stronger conspiracy and health beliefs. The study contributes to knowledge about the structure of unfounded beliefs, and reveals the potential relevance of affective (i.e., fear of COVID-19) and cognitive (i.e., cynicism) factors along with demographics, in endorsing such beliefs. In summary, we obtained cross-cultural evidence for the distinctiveness of unfounded conspiracy and health beliefs about COVID-19 in terms of their structure and correlates.Keywords: Unfounded beliefsCOVID-19conspiracy beliefshealth beliefscross-cultural Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Additional informationFundingThe work of Magdalena Żemojtel-Piotrowska and Jarosław Piotrowski was supported by the National Science Centre, Poland under Grant [2017/26/E/HS6/00282]. The work of Peter Jonason was partially supported by the Polish National Agency for Academic Exchange under Grant [PPN/ULM/2019/1/00019/U/00001] the National Science Centre of Poland under Grant [2019/35/B/HS6/00682]. The participation of Iva Poláčková Šolcová was supported by the National Plan of Recovery “Systemic Risk Institute” under Grant [LX22NPO5101], funded by European Union - Next Generation EU (Ministry of Education, Youth and Sports, EXCELES). The work of Narine Khachatryan was supported by the RA Science Committee, in the frames of the research project No. 20TTSH-070. In accordance with the national law of several countries, there were no direct collaboration between researchers from conflicting countries.
关于COVID-19的毫无根据的阴谋和健康信念伴随着全球范围内的大流行。在这里,我们使用多组验证性因素分析,用8项量表检查了这些信念的跨国结构和相关性。我们得到了一个内部结构良好的无根据(阴谋和健康)信念的双因素模型(平均CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.05, SRMR = 0.04),但两因素之间具有较高的相关性(平均潜在因素相关性= 0.57)。该模型可在50个国家(总N = 13,579)中复制,证明了国家之间的度量不变性(CFI = 0.96, RMSEA = 0.06, SRMS = 0.07),以及性别(CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.04, SRMS = 0.03)和教育水平(CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.04, SRMS = 0.03)的标量不变性。此外,较低的教育水平、对COVID-19的更多恐惧和更多的玩世不恭与更强的阴谋论和健康信念弱相关。该研究有助于了解毫无根据的信念的结构,并揭示了情感因素(即对COVID-19的恐惧)和认知因素(即犬儒主义)以及人口统计学因素与支持此类信念的潜在相关性。总之,我们获得了跨文化证据,证明关于COVID-19的毫无根据的阴谋和健康信念在其结构和相关性方面的独特性。关键词:毫无根据的信念scovid -19阴谋信念健康信念跨文化披露声明作者未报告潜在的利益冲突。Magdalena Żemojtel-Piotrowska和Jarosław Piotrowski的工作得到了波兰国家科学中心的资助[2017/26/E/HS6/00282]。Peter Jonason的工作得到了波兰国家学术交流局(PPN/ULM/2019/1/00019/U/00001)和波兰国家科学中心(2019/35/B/HS6/00682)的部分资助。Iva Poláčková Šolcová的参与得到了国家恢复计划“系统性风险研究所”的资助[LX22NPO5101],由欧盟-下一代欧盟(教育,青年和体育部,EXCELES)资助。Narine Khachatryan的工作得到了RA科学委员会的支持,在研究项目No. 20TTSH-070的框架内。根据一些国家的国内法,来自冲突国家的研究人员之间没有直接合作。
{"title":"Testing the underlying structure of unfounded beliefs about COVID-19 around the world","authors":"Paweł Brzóska, Magdalena Żemojtel-Piotrowska, Jarosław Piotrowski, Bartłomiej Nowak, Peter K. Jonason, Constantine Sedikides, Mladen Adamovic, Kokou A. Atitsogbe, Oli Ahmed, Uzma Azam, Sergiu Bălțătescu, Konstantin Bochaver, Aidos Bolatov, Mario Bonato, Victor Counted, Trawin Chaleeraktrakoon, Jano Ramos-Diaz, Sonya Dragova-Koleva, Walaa Labib M. Eldesoki, Carla Sofia Esteves, Valdiney V. Gouveia, Pablo Perez de Leon, Dzintra Iliško, Jesus Alfonso D. Datu, Fanli Jia, Veljko Jovanović, Tomislav Jukić, Narine Khachatryan, Monika Kovacs, Uri Lifshin, Aitor Larzabal Fernandez, Kadi Liik, Sadia Malik, Chanki Moon, Stephan Muehlbacher, Reza Najafi, Emre Oruç, Joonha Park, Iva Poláčková Šolcová, Rahkman Ardi, Ognjen Ridic, Goran Ridic, Yadgar Ismail Said, Andrej Starc, Delia Stefenel, Kiều Thị Thanh Trà, Habib Tiliouine, Robert Tomšik, Jorge Torres-Marin, Charles S. Umeh, Eduardo Wills-Herrera, Anna Wlodarczyk, Zahir Vally, Illia Yahiiaiev","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2023.2259539","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2023.2259539","url":null,"abstract":"AbstractUnfounded—conspiracy and health—beliefs about COVID-19 have accompanied the pandemic worldwide. Here, we examined cross-nationally the structure and correlates of these beliefs with an 8-item scale, using a multigroup confirmatory factor analysis. We obtained a two-factor model of unfounded (conspiracy and health) beliefs with good internal structure (average CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.05, SRMR = 0.04), but a high correlation between the two factors (average latent factor correlation = 0.57). This model was replicable across 50 countries (total N = 13,579), as evidenced by metric invariance between countries (CFI = 0.96, RMSEA = 0.06, SRMS = 0.07) as well as scalar invariance across genders (CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.04, SRMS = 0.03) and educational levels (CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.04, SRMS = 0.03). Also, lower levels of education, more fear of COVID-19, and more cynicism were weakly associated with stronger conspiracy and health beliefs. The study contributes to knowledge about the structure of unfounded beliefs, and reveals the potential relevance of affective (i.e., fear of COVID-19) and cognitive (i.e., cynicism) factors along with demographics, in endorsing such beliefs. In summary, we obtained cross-cultural evidence for the distinctiveness of unfounded conspiracy and health beliefs about COVID-19 in terms of their structure and correlates.Keywords: Unfounded beliefsCOVID-19conspiracy beliefshealth beliefscross-cultural Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Additional informationFundingThe work of Magdalena Żemojtel-Piotrowska and Jarosław Piotrowski was supported by the National Science Centre, Poland under Grant [2017/26/E/HS6/00282]. The work of Peter Jonason was partially supported by the Polish National Agency for Academic Exchange under Grant [PPN/ULM/2019/1/00019/U/00001] the National Science Centre of Poland under Grant [2019/35/B/HS6/00682]. The participation of Iva Poláčková Šolcová was supported by the National Plan of Recovery “Systemic Risk Institute” under Grant [LX22NPO5101], funded by European Union - Next Generation EU (Ministry of Education, Youth and Sports, EXCELES). The work of Narine Khachatryan was supported by the RA Science Committee, in the frames of the research project No. 20TTSH-070. In accordance with the national law of several countries, there were no direct collaboration between researchers from conflicting countries.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"23 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135386283","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Responsibility attribution about mechanical devices by children and adults 儿童和成人对机械装置的责任归属
3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-09-25 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2023.2259549
Cristina Gordo, Jesica Gómez-Sánchez, Sergio Moreno-Ríos
AbstractWe investigated the causal responsibility attributions of adults and children to mechanical devices in the framework of the criticality-pivotality model. It establishes that, to assign responsibility, people consider how important a target is to reaching a positive outcome (criticality) and how much the target contributed to the actual outcome (pivotality). We also tested theoretical predictions about relations between the development of counterfactual thinking and assessments of pivotality. In Experiment 1, we replicated previous findings in adults using our task. In Experiment 2, we administered this task and a brief counterfactual reasoning questionnaire to children aged between 8 and 13 years. Results showed that children also considered both criticality and pivotality when they attributed responsibility. However, older children were more sensitive than younger ones to pivotality. Also, we found a positive correlation between children’s pivotality judgements and a measure of counterfactual thinking. Results are discussed regarding the model’s relation to counterfactual thinking.Keywords: Responsibility attributionpivotalitycriticalitydevelopment of causationcounterfactual thinking Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.Notes1 The “order” factor was used as a control in our experiment. Although we had no initial predictions, we analysed the effect of this factor in participants’ prospective and retrospective judgments. A more detailed description of the “order” factor effect can be found at DOI: 10.6084/m9.figshare.23503548Additional informationFundingThis research was funded by the Spanish Government, Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (PGC2018-095868-B-I00) and by the Junta de Andalucía -Conserjería de Universidad, Investigación e Innovación - Project (P21_00073).
摘要在关键-枢纽模型的框架下,研究了成人和儿童对机械装置的因果责任归因。它表明,为了分配责任,人们会考虑目标对达到积极结果(关键性)的重要性,以及目标对实际结果的贡献程度(枢轴性)。我们还测试了关于反事实思维发展与支点评估之间关系的理论预测。在实验1中,我们在使用我们的任务的成年人中重复了之前的发现。在实验2中,我们对8 - 13岁的儿童进行了这个任务和一个简短的反事实推理问卷。结果表明,儿童在归因责任时也同时考虑了关键性和支点性。然而,年龄较大的儿童比年龄较小的儿童更敏感。此外,我们还发现儿童的关键判断与反事实思维之间存在正相关。讨论了该模型与反事实思维的关系。关键词:责任归因关键关键因果发展反事实思维披露声明作者未报告潜在利益冲突注1本实验以“序”因素作为对照。虽然我们没有最初的预测,但我们分析了这个因素对参与者的前瞻性和回顾性判断的影响。关于“顺序”因子效应的更详细描述可以在DOI: 10.6084/m9.figshare上找到。本研究由西班牙政府经济和竞争力部(PGC2018-095868-B-I00)和Junta de Andalucía -Conserjería de Universidad, Investigación e Innovación - Project (P21_00073)资助。
{"title":"Responsibility attribution about mechanical devices by children and adults","authors":"Cristina Gordo, Jesica Gómez-Sánchez, Sergio Moreno-Ríos","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2023.2259549","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2023.2259549","url":null,"abstract":"AbstractWe investigated the causal responsibility attributions of adults and children to mechanical devices in the framework of the criticality-pivotality model. It establishes that, to assign responsibility, people consider how important a target is to reaching a positive outcome (criticality) and how much the target contributed to the actual outcome (pivotality). We also tested theoretical predictions about relations between the development of counterfactual thinking and assessments of pivotality. In Experiment 1, we replicated previous findings in adults using our task. In Experiment 2, we administered this task and a brief counterfactual reasoning questionnaire to children aged between 8 and 13 years. Results showed that children also considered both criticality and pivotality when they attributed responsibility. However, older children were more sensitive than younger ones to pivotality. Also, we found a positive correlation between children’s pivotality judgements and a measure of counterfactual thinking. Results are discussed regarding the model’s relation to counterfactual thinking.Keywords: Responsibility attributionpivotalitycriticalitydevelopment of causationcounterfactual thinking Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.Notes1 The “order” factor was used as a control in our experiment. Although we had no initial predictions, we analysed the effect of this factor in participants’ prospective and retrospective judgments. A more detailed description of the “order” factor effect can be found at DOI: 10.6084/m9.figshare.23503548Additional informationFundingThis research was funded by the Spanish Government, Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (PGC2018-095868-B-I00) and by the Junta de Andalucía -Conserjería de Universidad, Investigación e Innovación - Project (P21_00073).","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"11 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135816473","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining the role of deliberation in de-bias training 考察审议在去偏见训练中的作用
3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-09-25 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2023.2259542
Esther Boissin, Serge Caparos, Wim De Neys
AbstractDoes avoiding biased responding to reasoning problems and grasping the ­correct solution require engaging in effortful deliberation or can such solution insight be acquired more intuitively? In this study we set out to test the impact of deliberation on the efficiency of a de-bias training in which the problem logic was explained to participants. We focused on the infamous bat-and-ball problem and varied the degree of possible deliberation during the training session by manipulating time constraints and cognitive load. The results show that the less constrained the deliberation, the more participants improve. However, even under extremely stringent conditions (high time-pressure and dual task load), participants still show a significant improvement. Critically, this “intuitive” insight effect persists over two months. This suggests that deliberation helps reasoners benefit from the training, but it is not indispensable. We discuss critical applied and theoretical implications.Keywords: Reasoninginsightheuristics & biasesde-biasingintuition Disclosure statementNone.Open data statementRaw data can be downloaded from our OSF page (https://osf.io/3b4jy/?view_only=a388443c8fc34310b9f908fe847f077b).Notes1 Hence, whenever we report null findings, we cannot exclude that with a more powerful design the effects might be significant.2 Participants solved three training problems and read an explanation for each one. We focused on the problem solution times for the two problems after the first explanation (i.e., the third and fourth problem of the whole intervention).3 Given that all reading times were fixed at 8 seconds in the fast group (i.e., participants could not advance earlier), this analysis was not informative in this group.Additional informationFundingThis study was supported by the Idex Université Paris Cité ANR-18-IDEX-0001 and by a research grant (DIAGNOR, ANR-16-CE28-0010-01) from the Agence Nationale de la Recherche, France.
摘要为了避免对推理问题做出有偏见的反应并掌握正确的解决方案,是否需要进行努力的思考,或者这种解决方案的洞察力是否可以更直观地获得?在这项研究中,我们开始测试审议对向参与者解释问题逻辑的去偏见训练效率的影响。我们专注于臭名昭著的击球和球问题,并通过操纵时间限制和认知负荷来改变训练过程中可能考虑的程度。结果表明,审议约束越少,参与者的进步越多。然而,即使在极端苛刻的条件下(高时间压力和双重任务负载),参与者仍然表现出显著的改善。关键的是,这种“直觉”洞察力效应持续了两个多月。这表明深思熟虑有助于推理者从训练中受益,但它不是必不可少的。我们讨论了关键的应用和理论意义。关键词:推理洞察力、特征与偏见、偏见直觉、披露陈述开放数据声明可以从我们的OSF页面(https://osf.io/3b4jy/?view_only=a388443c8fc34310b9f908fe847f077b)下载数据库数据。注1因此,每当我们报告无效结果时,我们不能排除在更强大的设计中效果可能是显著的参与者解决了三个训练问题,并阅读了每个问题的解释。我们关注的是第一个解释后两个问题的问题解决时间(即整个干预的第三和第四个问题)考虑到快速组中所有的阅读时间都固定在8秒(即参与者不能提前阅读),该分析在该组中不具有信息性。本研究由巴黎城市Idex大学(ANR-18-IDEX-0001)和法国国家研究机构的研究基金(DIAGNOR, ANR-16-CE28-0010-01)支持。
{"title":"Examining the role of deliberation in de-bias training","authors":"Esther Boissin, Serge Caparos, Wim De Neys","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2023.2259542","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2023.2259542","url":null,"abstract":"AbstractDoes avoiding biased responding to reasoning problems and grasping the ­correct solution require engaging in effortful deliberation or can such solution insight be acquired more intuitively? In this study we set out to test the impact of deliberation on the efficiency of a de-bias training in which the problem logic was explained to participants. We focused on the infamous bat-and-ball problem and varied the degree of possible deliberation during the training session by manipulating time constraints and cognitive load. The results show that the less constrained the deliberation, the more participants improve. However, even under extremely stringent conditions (high time-pressure and dual task load), participants still show a significant improvement. Critically, this “intuitive” insight effect persists over two months. This suggests that deliberation helps reasoners benefit from the training, but it is not indispensable. We discuss critical applied and theoretical implications.Keywords: Reasoninginsightheuristics & biasesde-biasingintuition Disclosure statementNone.Open data statementRaw data can be downloaded from our OSF page (https://osf.io/3b4jy/?view_only=a388443c8fc34310b9f908fe847f077b).Notes1 Hence, whenever we report null findings, we cannot exclude that with a more powerful design the effects might be significant.2 Participants solved three training problems and read an explanation for each one. We focused on the problem solution times for the two problems after the first explanation (i.e., the third and fourth problem of the whole intervention).3 Given that all reading times were fixed at 8 seconds in the fast group (i.e., participants could not advance earlier), this analysis was not informative in this group.Additional informationFundingThis study was supported by the Idex Université Paris Cité ANR-18-IDEX-0001 and by a research grant (DIAGNOR, ANR-16-CE28-0010-01) from the Agence Nationale de la Recherche, France.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"25 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135816007","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Open-minded and reflective thinking predicts reasoning and meta-reasoning: evidence from a ratio-bias conflict task 开放性和反思性思维预测推理和元推理:来自比率偏见冲突任务的证据
3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-09-22 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2023.2259548
Henry W. Strudwicke, Glen E. Bodner, Paul Williamson, Michelle M. Arnold
AbstractDispositional measures of actively open-minded thinking and cognitive reflection both predict reasoning accuracy on conflict problems. Here we investigated their relative impact on meta-reasoning. To this end, we measured reasoning accuracy and two indices of meta-reasoning performance – conflict detection sensitivity and meta-reasoning discrimination – using a ratio-bias task. Our key predictors were actively open-minded thinking and cognitive reflection, and numeracy, cognitive ability, and mindware instantiation were controlled for. Actively open-minded thinking was a better predictor of reasoning accuracy and meta-reasoning discrimination than cognitive reflection, and was the only dispositional measure to significantly predict conflict detection sensitivity. Thus, susceptibility to biased reasoning and meta-reasoning may be better captured by a reasoner’s ability to engage in open-minded thinking than by their ability to engage in reflective thinking.Keywords: Meta-reasoningindividual differencesbias susceptibilitythinking dispositions AcknowledgmentsThis research was supported by an HDR Research Award from the College of Education, Psychology and Social Work at Flinders University. The authors would like to thank Matthew Christian and Olivia Burton for their feedback on an earlier draft of this manuscript.Author contributionsH.W.S and M.M.A conceived the study. H.W.S programmed the experiment and collected the data. H.W.S and P.W analyzed the data. H.W.S and G.E.B co-wrote the manuscript and P.W made critical edits.Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.Data availability statementThe data are publicly available at Open Sciences Framework: https://osf.io/jm63r/?view_only=15070473806541548adc224496c86660
【摘要】主动开放思维和认知反思的性格指标都能预测冲突问题的推理准确性。在这里,我们调查了它们对元推理的相对影响。为此,我们使用比率偏差任务测量推理准确性和元推理性能的两个指标-冲突检测灵敏度和元推理辨别力。我们的主要预测因素是积极开放的思维和认知反思,计算能力、认知能力和意识实例化是控制因素。与认知反思相比,积极开放思维能更好地预测推理准确性和元推理辨析,是唯一能显著预测冲突检测敏感性的性格指标。因此,对偏见推理和元推理的敏感性可能更好地体现在推理者进行开放思维的能力上,而不是他们进行反思性思维的能力上。本研究获得了弗林德斯大学教育、心理与社会工作学院HDR研究奖的支持。作者要感谢Matthew Christian和Olivia Burton对本文早期草稿的反馈。sh.w.s.和m.m.a.构思了这项研究。h.w.s.编写了实验程序并收集了数据。h.w.s.和p.w.分析了数据。h.w.s.和G.E.B共同撰写了手稿,p.w.进行了重要的编辑。披露声明作者未报告潜在的利益冲突。数据可用性声明数据可在Open Sciences Framework: https://osf.io/jm63r/?view_only=15070473806541548adc224496c86660公开获取
{"title":"Open-minded and reflective thinking predicts reasoning and meta-reasoning: evidence from a ratio-bias conflict task","authors":"Henry W. Strudwicke, Glen E. Bodner, Paul Williamson, Michelle M. Arnold","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2023.2259548","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2023.2259548","url":null,"abstract":"AbstractDispositional measures of actively open-minded thinking and cognitive reflection both predict reasoning accuracy on conflict problems. Here we investigated their relative impact on meta-reasoning. To this end, we measured reasoning accuracy and two indices of meta-reasoning performance – conflict detection sensitivity and meta-reasoning discrimination – using a ratio-bias task. Our key predictors were actively open-minded thinking and cognitive reflection, and numeracy, cognitive ability, and mindware instantiation were controlled for. Actively open-minded thinking was a better predictor of reasoning accuracy and meta-reasoning discrimination than cognitive reflection, and was the only dispositional measure to significantly predict conflict detection sensitivity. Thus, susceptibility to biased reasoning and meta-reasoning may be better captured by a reasoner’s ability to engage in open-minded thinking than by their ability to engage in reflective thinking.Keywords: Meta-reasoningindividual differencesbias susceptibilitythinking dispositions AcknowledgmentsThis research was supported by an HDR Research Award from the College of Education, Psychology and Social Work at Flinders University. The authors would like to thank Matthew Christian and Olivia Burton for their feedback on an earlier draft of this manuscript.Author contributionsH.W.S and M.M.A conceived the study. H.W.S programmed the experiment and collected the data. H.W.S and P.W analyzed the data. H.W.S and G.E.B co-wrote the manuscript and P.W made critical edits.Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.Data availability statementThe data are publicly available at Open Sciences Framework: https://osf.io/jm63r/?view_only=15070473806541548adc224496c86660","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"44 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136060934","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Gain-loss domain and social value orientation as determinants of risk allocation decisions 损益域和社会价值取向是风险分配决策的决定因素
3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-09-21 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2023.2259543
Ming-Hong Tsai, Verlin B. Hinsz
AbstractPeople often make less risky decisions for themselves than others. We examined how people allocated risks (i.e., determining the ratio of uncertain outcomes to certain outcomes) between themselves and others. We also investigated gain (vs. loss) domain and social value orientation as predictors of risk allocations. The results of three experiments demonstrated that participants were more likely to share their risks equally between themselves and others than distribute risk unequally. In the gain (vs. loss) domain, participants allocated fewer risks to themselves and more risks to the other person for unequal risk allocations. Compared to proselfs, prosocials were more likely to allocate risks equally. We also found stronger domain effects on unequal risk allocations for proselfs than for prosocials. Therefore, our findings clarify the effects of risk distribution, domain, and social value orientation on interpersonal allocation decisions and highlight equal risk distribution between oneself and others.Keywords: Allocation decisionrisk distributiongain-loss domainsocial value orientationself-other Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Notes1 For the options of risk allocation, a percentage refers to the proportion of an uncertain outcome to a total outcome allocated to oneself or another individual.2 We declare that there is no conflict of interest. We also confirm that the manuscript adheres to ethical guidelines specified in the APA Code of Conduct and our national ethics guidelines. Please see the data and analysis codes at https://osf.io/zmwfc/?view_only=95de83989f50460c97477aa6cb5239b0.3 We conducted power analyses to demonstrate that our sample sizes have sufficient power in Experiments 1-3. We also ran additional analyses to demonstrate the even distribution of sample sizes across different conditions before and after filtering the data in Experiments 1 and 2. These are presented in the supplemental materials.4 We followed the incentive instructions to pair participants, calculate each participant’s final points, and awarded the three participants accordingly in each condition with the incentive instructions (in Experiments 1 and 2).5 We also ran additional analyses without the controls for the differences in the items and choices, and the results demonstrated consistent patterns between the results with and without controls, which suggests that the differences in the first-stage allocations did not significantly influence our results. Please see the relevant results in the section titled “Results without Controls for the Allocation Outcomes During the Initial Decisions in Experiments 1 and 2” in the supplemental materials.6 The percentage refers to the average difference of the selection likelihood of a specific risk-distribution option between the gain and loss conditions.
人们往往比别人更少为自己做冒险的决定。我们研究了人们如何在自己和他人之间分配风险(即确定不确定结果与确定结果的比例)。我们还研究了收益(vs.损失)领域和社会价值取向作为风险分配的预测因素。三个实验的结果表明,参与者更有可能在自己和他人之间平均分担风险,而不是不平等地分配风险。在收益(相对于损失)领域,参与者为自己分配的风险更少,而为他人分配的风险更多,风险分配不平等。与皈依者相比,亲社会者更倾向于平均分配风险。我们还发现,与亲社会者相比,信仰者在不平等风险分配上的领域效应更强。因此,我们的研究结果阐明了风险分配、领域和社会价值取向对人际分配决策的影响,并强调了自己与他人之间的风险平等分配。关键词:分配决策风险分配损失领域社会价值取向自我他人披露声明作者未发现潜在利益冲突。注1在风险分配选项中,百分比是指不确定的结果占分配给自己或他人的总结果的比例我们声明不存在利益冲突。我们也确认稿件符合APA行为准则和我们国家伦理准则中规定的伦理准则。请参阅https://osf.io/zmwfc/?view_only=95de83989f50460c97477aa6cb5239b0.3上的数据和分析代码。我们在实验1-3中进行了功率分析,以证明我们的样本量具有足够的功率。我们还进行了额外的分析,以证明在实验1和2中过滤数据之前和之后,样本大小在不同条件下的均匀分布。这些都在补充材料中提出我们按照激励指示将参与者配对,计算每个参与者的最终得分,并根据激励指示在每种情况下相应地奖励三名参与者(实验1和2)我们还在没有控制的情况下对项目和选择的差异进行了额外的分析,结果显示了有控制和没有控制的结果之间的一致模式,这表明第一阶段分配的差异对我们的结果没有显著影响。相关结果见补充材料“实验1和实验2初始决策中分配结果不加控制的结果”部分百分比是指某一特定风险分配选项在收益和损失条件下的选择可能性的平均差值。
{"title":"Gain-loss domain and social value orientation as determinants of risk allocation decisions","authors":"Ming-Hong Tsai, Verlin B. Hinsz","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2023.2259543","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2023.2259543","url":null,"abstract":"AbstractPeople often make less risky decisions for themselves than others. We examined how people allocated risks (i.e., determining the ratio of uncertain outcomes to certain outcomes) between themselves and others. We also investigated gain (vs. loss) domain and social value orientation as predictors of risk allocations. The results of three experiments demonstrated that participants were more likely to share their risks equally between themselves and others than distribute risk unequally. In the gain (vs. loss) domain, participants allocated fewer risks to themselves and more risks to the other person for unequal risk allocations. Compared to proselfs, prosocials were more likely to allocate risks equally. We also found stronger domain effects on unequal risk allocations for proselfs than for prosocials. Therefore, our findings clarify the effects of risk distribution, domain, and social value orientation on interpersonal allocation decisions and highlight equal risk distribution between oneself and others.Keywords: Allocation decisionrisk distributiongain-loss domainsocial value orientationself-other Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Notes1 For the options of risk allocation, a percentage refers to the proportion of an uncertain outcome to a total outcome allocated to oneself or another individual.2 We declare that there is no conflict of interest. We also confirm that the manuscript adheres to ethical guidelines specified in the APA Code of Conduct and our national ethics guidelines. Please see the data and analysis codes at https://osf.io/zmwfc/?view_only=95de83989f50460c97477aa6cb5239b0.3 We conducted power analyses to demonstrate that our sample sizes have sufficient power in Experiments 1-3. We also ran additional analyses to demonstrate the even distribution of sample sizes across different conditions before and after filtering the data in Experiments 1 and 2. These are presented in the supplemental materials.4 We followed the incentive instructions to pair participants, calculate each participant’s final points, and awarded the three participants accordingly in each condition with the incentive instructions (in Experiments 1 and 2).5 We also ran additional analyses without the controls for the differences in the items and choices, and the results demonstrated consistent patterns between the results with and without controls, which suggests that the differences in the first-stage allocations did not significantly influence our results. Please see the relevant results in the section titled “Results without Controls for the Allocation Outcomes During the Initial Decisions in Experiments 1 and 2” in the supplemental materials.6 The percentage refers to the average difference of the selection likelihood of a specific risk-distribution option between the gain and loss conditions.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"46 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136237559","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Argument evaluation and production in the correction of political innumeracy 政治数学盲纠偏中的论证评价与生成
3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-09-21 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2023.2259537
Martin Dockendorff, Hugo Mercier
AbstractThe public is largely innumerate, making systematic mistakes in estimating some politically relevant facts, such as the share of foreign-born citizens. In two-step or multistep flow models, such mistakes could be corrected if better-informed citizens were able to convince their peers, in particular by using good arguments citing reliable sources. In six experiments, we find two issues that dampen the potential power of this two-step flow process. First, even though participants were more convinced by good than by poor arguments, many did not change their minds, even when confronted with good arguments. Second, participants are not inclined to spontaneously generate arguments that cite reliable sources, even when they have just been influenced by such arguments. Both issues should put a significant brake in the spread of political numeracy through the two-step flow process, in particular in non-dialogic contexts.Keywords: Political numeracytwo-step flowargument evaluationargument productionargument transmission AcknowledgementsThis work was supported by the French National Research Agency (ANR) under grants ANR-17-EURE-0017 FrontCog and ANR-10-IDEX-0001-02 PSL. We thank John Sides for providing constructive comments on earlier versions of this paper.Disclosure statementThe authors report there are no competing interests to declare.Notes1 All Experiments were conducted between Fall 2016 and Summer 2017.2 The distribution is thus far from being normal. However, since t-tests are very robust against non-normality, we have relied on them rather than on Wilcoxon tests (Rasch & Guiard, Citation2004). The same analyses using Wilcoxon signed-rank test can be found in the ESM.Additional informationFundingThis work was supported by the French National Research Agency (ANR) under grants ANR-17-EURE-0017 FrontCog and ANR-10-IDEX-0001-02 PSL.
摘要公众在很大程度上是不懂数学的,在估计一些政治相关事实(如外国出生公民的比例)时犯了系统性错误。在两步或多步流动模型中,如果消息灵通的公民能够说服他们的同伴,特别是通过引用可靠来源的有力论据,这些错误就可以得到纠正。在六个实验中,我们发现两个问题抑制了这两步流程的潜在力量。首先,尽管参与者更相信好的论点而不是糟糕的论点,但即使面对好的论点,许多人也不会改变主意。其次,参与者不倾向于自发地提出引用可靠来源的论点,即使他们刚刚受到这些论点的影响。这两个问题都应该通过两步流动进程,特别是在非对话的情况下,对政治计算能力的传播起到重要的抑制作用。本文由法国国家研究机构(ANR)资助,项目编号为ANR-17- eure -0017 FrontCog和ANR-10- idex -0001-02 PSL。我们感谢John Sides对本文早期版本提供的建设性意见。作者报告无利益竞争需要申报。注1所有实验都是在2016年秋季到2017年夏季之间进行的。7.2因此,分布远非正态分布。然而,由于t检验对非正态性非常稳健,我们依赖于它们而不是Wilcoxon检验(Rasch & guard, Citation2004)。在ESM中可以找到使用Wilcoxon符号秩检验的相同分析。本研究由法国国家研究机构(ANR)资助,ANR-17- eure -0017 FrontCog和ANR-10- idex -0001-02 PSL。
{"title":"Argument evaluation and production in the correction of political innumeracy","authors":"Martin Dockendorff, Hugo Mercier","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2023.2259537","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2023.2259537","url":null,"abstract":"AbstractThe public is largely innumerate, making systematic mistakes in estimating some politically relevant facts, such as the share of foreign-born citizens. In two-step or multistep flow models, such mistakes could be corrected if better-informed citizens were able to convince their peers, in particular by using good arguments citing reliable sources. In six experiments, we find two issues that dampen the potential power of this two-step flow process. First, even though participants were more convinced by good than by poor arguments, many did not change their minds, even when confronted with good arguments. Second, participants are not inclined to spontaneously generate arguments that cite reliable sources, even when they have just been influenced by such arguments. Both issues should put a significant brake in the spread of political numeracy through the two-step flow process, in particular in non-dialogic contexts.Keywords: Political numeracytwo-step flowargument evaluationargument productionargument transmission AcknowledgementsThis work was supported by the French National Research Agency (ANR) under grants ANR-17-EURE-0017 FrontCog and ANR-10-IDEX-0001-02 PSL. We thank John Sides for providing constructive comments on earlier versions of this paper.Disclosure statementThe authors report there are no competing interests to declare.Notes1 All Experiments were conducted between Fall 2016 and Summer 2017.2 The distribution is thus far from being normal. However, since t-tests are very robust against non-normality, we have relied on them rather than on Wilcoxon tests (Rasch & Guiard, Citation2004). The same analyses using Wilcoxon signed-rank test can be found in the ESM.Additional informationFundingThis work was supported by the French National Research Agency (ANR) under grants ANR-17-EURE-0017 FrontCog and ANR-10-IDEX-0001-02 PSL.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"19 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136102051","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Thinking & Reasoning
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1