首页 > 最新文献

Thinking & Reasoning最新文献

英文 中文
Understanding the relationship between rationality and intelligence: a latent-variable approach 理解理性与智力之间的关系:一种潜在变量方法
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2021-02-08 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2021.2008003
Alexander P. Burgoyne, Cody A. Mashburn, Jason S. Tsukahara, Zach Hambrick, R. Engle
Abstract A hallmark of intelligent behavior is rationality – the disposition and ability to think analytically to make decisions that maximize expected utility or follow the laws of probability. However, the question remains as to whether rationality and intelligence are empirically distinct, as does the question of what cognitive mechanisms underlie individual differences in rationality. In a sample of 331 participants, we assessed the relationship between rationality and intelligence. There was a common ability underpinning performance on some, but not all, rationality tests. Latent factors representing rationality and general intelligence were strongly correlated (r = .54), but their correlation fell well short of unity. Rationality correlated significantly with fluid intelligence (r = .56), working memory capacity (r = .44), and attention control (r = .49). Attention control fully accounted for the relationship between working memory capacity and rationality, and partially accounted for the relationship between fluid intelligence and rationality. We conclude by speculating about factors rationality tests may tap that other cognitive ability tests miss, and outline directions for further research.
智能行为的一个标志是理性——分析思考的倾向和能力,以做出最大化预期效用或遵循概率规律的决策。然而,理性和智力是否在经验上是不同的问题仍然存在,理性的个体差异背后的认知机制也是如此。在331名参与者的样本中,我们评估了理性与智力之间的关系。在一些(但不是全部)理性测试中,有一种共同的能力支撑着他们的表现。代表理性和一般智力的潜在因素是强相关的(r = 0.54),但它们的相关性远远不够统一。理性与流体智力(r = 0.56)、工作记忆容量(r = 0.44)和注意力控制(r = 0.49)显著相关。注意控制完全解释了工作记忆容量与理性的关系,部分解释了流体智力与理性的关系。最后,我们推测了理性测试可能挖掘的其他认知能力测试遗漏的因素,并概述了进一步研究的方向。
{"title":"Understanding the relationship between rationality and intelligence: a latent-variable approach","authors":"Alexander P. Burgoyne, Cody A. Mashburn, Jason S. Tsukahara, Zach Hambrick, R. Engle","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2021.2008003","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2021.2008003","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract A hallmark of intelligent behavior is rationality – the disposition and ability to think analytically to make decisions that maximize expected utility or follow the laws of probability. However, the question remains as to whether rationality and intelligence are empirically distinct, as does the question of what cognitive mechanisms underlie individual differences in rationality. In a sample of 331 participants, we assessed the relationship between rationality and intelligence. There was a common ability underpinning performance on some, but not all, rationality tests. Latent factors representing rationality and general intelligence were strongly correlated (r = .54), but their correlation fell well short of unity. Rationality correlated significantly with fluid intelligence (r = .56), working memory capacity (r = .44), and attention control (r = .49). Attention control fully accounted for the relationship between working memory capacity and rationality, and partially accounted for the relationship between fluid intelligence and rationality. We conclude by speculating about factors rationality tests may tap that other cognitive ability tests miss, and outline directions for further research.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"42 1","pages":"1 - 42"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2021-02-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72649770","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Stumpers: an annotated compendium* 树桩:注释纲要*
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2021-02-03 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2020.1870247
M. Bar-Hillel
Abstract A stumper is a riddle whose solution is typically so elusive that it does not come to mind, at least initially – leaving the responder stumped. Stumpers work by eliciting a (typically visual) representation of the narrative, in which the solution is not to be found. In order to solve the stumper, the blocking representation must be changed, which does not happen to most respondents. I have collected all the riddles I know at this time that qualify, in my opinion, as stumpers. I have composed a few, and tested many. Whenever rates of correct solutions were available, they are included, giving a rough proxy for difficulty.
一个难题是一个谜题,它的答案通常是如此难以捉摸,以至于它不会出现在脑海中,至少在一开始-让回答者陷入困境。Stumpers的工作原理是引出一种(通常是视觉上的)叙述,在这种叙述中找不到解决方案。为了解决这个难题,必须改变阻塞表示,这在大多数被调查者中并没有发生。我收集了目前我所知道的所有的谜语,在我看来,这些谜语是有资格成为难题的。我编写了一些,并测试了许多。只要有正确解决方案的比率,就会将其包含在内,从而粗略地表示难度。
{"title":"Stumpers: an annotated compendium*","authors":"M. Bar-Hillel","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2020.1870247","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2020.1870247","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract A stumper is a riddle whose solution is typically so elusive that it does not come to mind, at least initially – leaving the responder stumped. Stumpers work by eliciting a (typically visual) representation of the narrative, in which the solution is not to be found. In order to solve the stumper, the blocking representation must be changed, which does not happen to most respondents. I have collected all the riddles I know at this time that qualify, in my opinion, as stumpers. I have composed a few, and tested many. Whenever rates of correct solutions were available, they are included, giving a rough proxy for difficulty.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"30 1","pages":"536 - 566"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2021-02-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"73577088","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Intuitions about the epistemic virtues of majority voting 关于多数投票的认知美德的直觉
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-12-11 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2020.1857306
H. Mercier, Martin Dockendorff, Y. Majima, Anne-Sophie Hacquin, Melissa Schwartzberg
Abstract The Condorcet Jury Theorem, along with empirical results, establishes the accuracy of majority voting in a broad range of conditions. Here we investigate whether naïve participants (in the U.S. and Japan) are aware of this accuracy. In four experiments, participants were provided with information about an assembly voting to decide on one of two options, one being better than the other. In Experiments 1 and 2, participants were provided with specific parameters and they vastly underestimated the probability that the majority would select the right option. In Experiment 3, participants were provided with less specific information, and still underestimated the probability that the majority would select the right option. In Experiment 4, participants were asked to compare majority rules and supermajority rules. Most participants failed to grasp the relative weakness of supermajority rules. Our results are compatible with participant relying on a simple model of the voting situation based either on the competence of an individual voter, or on the minimum proportion required for a majority to form, making them largely blind to the “miracle of aggregation.”
孔多塞陪审团定理与实证结果一起,在广泛的条件下建立了多数投票的准确性。在这里,我们调查naïve参与者(在美国和日本)是否意识到这种准确性。在四个实验中,研究人员向参与者提供了关于集体投票决定两种选择之一的信息,其中一种比另一种更好。在实验1和2中,参与者被提供了特定的参数,他们大大低估了大多数人选择正确选项的可能性。在实验3中,参与者被提供的具体信息较少,仍然低估了大多数人会选择正确选项的概率。在实验4中,参与者被要求比较多数规则和绝对多数规则。大多数与会者未能领会绝对多数规则的相对弱点。我们的结果与依赖简单投票情况模型的参与者是兼容的,该模型要么基于单个选民的能力,要么基于形成多数所需的最小比例,这使得他们在很大程度上对“聚合奇迹”视而不见。
{"title":"Intuitions about the epistemic virtues of majority voting","authors":"H. Mercier, Martin Dockendorff, Y. Majima, Anne-Sophie Hacquin, Melissa Schwartzberg","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2020.1857306","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2020.1857306","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The Condorcet Jury Theorem, along with empirical results, establishes the accuracy of majority voting in a broad range of conditions. Here we investigate whether naïve participants (in the U.S. and Japan) are aware of this accuracy. In four experiments, participants were provided with information about an assembly voting to decide on one of two options, one being better than the other. In Experiments 1 and 2, participants were provided with specific parameters and they vastly underestimated the probability that the majority would select the right option. In Experiment 3, participants were provided with less specific information, and still underestimated the probability that the majority would select the right option. In Experiment 4, participants were asked to compare majority rules and supermajority rules. Most participants failed to grasp the relative weakness of supermajority rules. Our results are compatible with participant relying on a simple model of the voting situation based either on the competence of an individual voter, or on the minimum proportion required for a majority to form, making them largely blind to the “miracle of aggregation.”","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"20 1","pages":"445 - 463"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-12-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"81902675","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Schoolchildren’s transitive reasoning with the spatial relation ‘is left/right of’ 学童对空间关系的传递性推理"左/右"
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-11-11 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2020.1843536
K. Demiddele, Tom Heyman, W. Schaeken
Abstract We examine schoolchildren’s reasoning with spatial relations, such as ‘is to the left of’. Our aims are to obtain a more precise account of the effect of working memory on reasoning, a more detailed understanding of the internal representation of mental models and a developmental perspective. We discuss two experiments in which 348 children, between eight and twelve years old, needed to verify conclusions for 24 reasoning problems describing the spatial relations between pieces of clothing. In both experiments, children in the experimental condition were allowed to take notes by means of paper and pencil. We find that the participants spontaneously draw iconic representations of the items’ spatial ordering, have a strong preference for only considering one possible state of affairs even when more are relevant, and that an explanation in terms of working memory capacity alone cannot fully explain the data.
我们用空间关系来研究小学生的推理,比如“在左边”。我们的目标是更精确地描述工作记忆对推理的影响,更详细地理解心理模型的内部表征,并从发展的角度来看。我们讨论了两个实验,其中348名8至12岁的儿童需要验证24个推理问题的结论,这些推理问题描述了衣服之间的空间关系。在这两个实验中,实验条件下的孩子被允许用纸和笔做笔记。我们发现,参与者自发地绘制项目空间顺序的图标表示,即使有更多相关的情况,也强烈倾向于只考虑一种可能的事件状态,并且仅从工作记忆容量方面解释并不能完全解释数据。
{"title":"Schoolchildren’s transitive reasoning with the spatial relation ‘is left/right of’","authors":"K. Demiddele, Tom Heyman, W. Schaeken","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2020.1843536","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2020.1843536","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract We examine schoolchildren’s reasoning with spatial relations, such as ‘is to the left of’. Our aims are to obtain a more precise account of the effect of working memory on reasoning, a more detailed understanding of the internal representation of mental models and a developmental perspective. We discuss two experiments in which 348 children, between eight and twelve years old, needed to verify conclusions for 24 reasoning problems describing the spatial relations between pieces of clothing. In both experiments, children in the experimental condition were allowed to take notes by means of paper and pencil. We find that the participants spontaneously draw iconic representations of the items’ spatial ordering, have a strong preference for only considering one possible state of affairs even when more are relevant, and that an explanation in terms of working memory capacity alone cannot fully explain the data.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"1 1","pages":"479 - 509"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-11-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"84643053","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dynamic overconfidence: a growth curve and cross lagged analysis of accuracy, confidence, overestimation and their relations 动态过度自信:准确度、自信、高估及其关系的增长曲线和交叉滞后分析
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-11-06 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2020.1837241
E. Kausel, F. Carrasco, T. Reyes, A. Hirmas, A. Rodríguez
Abstract Research has paid little attention to how overconfidence evolves over time. We examined how task experience (experience within a task using a sequence of items) and outcome feedback affected accuracy, confidence and overconfidence in experiments over several trials. We conducted five studies involving 614 participants and used growth curve modelling and cross-lagged analyses. Findings revealed that mere task experience (without feedback) reduced overestimation linearly. Task experience coupled with feedback reduced overconfidence quadratically; the decreasing rate was initially strong but faded away over time. The decrease in overestimation was explained due to accuracy increasing at a faster rate than confidence did. Accuracy had lagged effects on confidence; a correct estimate led to more confidence in a subsequent estimate. We also found some evidence indicating that confidence had a negative lagged influence on accuracy. This dynamic influence between accuracy and confidence is a unique finding in the overconfidence literature.
研究很少关注过度自信是如何随着时间的推移而演变的。在几个实验中,我们研究了任务体验(在使用一系列项目的任务中的体验)和结果反馈如何影响准确性、信心和过度自信。我们进行了五项研究,涉及614名参与者,并使用了生长曲线建模和交叉滞后分析。研究结果显示,单纯的任务体验(没有反馈)会线性地减少高估。任务体验与反馈相结合会二次降低过度自信;下降的速度最初是强劲的,但随着时间的推移逐渐消失。过高估计的减少是由于准确性比置信度增长得更快。准确性对信心有滞后影响;正确的估计会使后续的估计更有信心。我们还发现一些证据表明,信心对准确性有负滞后影响。这种准确性和信心之间的动态影响在过度自信的文献中是一个独特的发现。
{"title":"Dynamic overconfidence: a growth curve and cross lagged analysis of accuracy, confidence, overestimation and their relations","authors":"E. Kausel, F. Carrasco, T. Reyes, A. Hirmas, A. Rodríguez","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2020.1837241","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2020.1837241","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Research has paid little attention to how overconfidence evolves over time. We examined how task experience (experience within a task using a sequence of items) and outcome feedback affected accuracy, confidence and overconfidence in experiments over several trials. We conducted five studies involving 614 participants and used growth curve modelling and cross-lagged analyses. Findings revealed that mere task experience (without feedback) reduced overestimation linearly. Task experience coupled with feedback reduced overconfidence quadratically; the decreasing rate was initially strong but faded away over time. The decrease in overestimation was explained due to accuracy increasing at a faster rate than confidence did. Accuracy had lagged effects on confidence; a correct estimate led to more confidence in a subsequent estimate. We also found some evidence indicating that confidence had a negative lagged influence on accuracy. This dynamic influence between accuracy and confidence is a unique finding in the overconfidence literature.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"260 1","pages":"417 - 444"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-11-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"76768499","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Belief in fake news, responsiveness to cognitive conflict, and analytic reasoning engagement 对假新闻的信仰,对认知冲突的反应,以及分析推理的参与
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-11-03 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2020.1847190
Michaela Bronstein, Gordon Pennycook, L. Buonomano, Tyrone D. Cannon
Abstract Analytic and intuitive reasoning processes have been implicated as important determinants of belief in (or skepticism of) fake news. However, the underlying cognitive mechanisms that encourage endorsement of fake news remain unclear. The present study investigated cognitive decoupling/response inhibition and the potential role of conflict processing in the initiation of analytic thought about fake news as factors that may facilitate skepticism. A base-rate task was used to test the hypotheses that conflict processing deficits and inefficient response inhibition would be related to stronger endorsement of fake news. In support of these hypotheses, increased belief in fake (but not real) news was associated with a smaller decrease in response confidence in the presence (vs. absence) of conflict and with inefficient (in terms of response latency) inhibition of prepotent responses. Through its support for these hypotheses, the present study advances efforts to determine who will fall for fake news, and why.
分析和直觉推理过程被认为是假新闻信仰(或怀疑)的重要决定因素。然而,鼓励支持假新闻的潜在认知机制仍不清楚。本研究调查了认知脱钩/反应抑制和冲突加工在假新闻分析思维启动中的潜在作用,作为可能促进怀疑的因素。本研究采用基础率任务来检验冲突处理缺陷和低效率反应抑制与假新闻更强的支持相关的假设。为了支持这些假设,对虚假(但不是真实)新闻的信任增加与对存在(相对于不存在)冲突的反应信心的较小下降以及对强效反应的低效抑制(就反应延迟而言)有关。通过对这些假设的支持,本研究推动了确定谁会相信假新闻及其原因的努力。
{"title":"Belief in fake news, responsiveness to cognitive conflict, and analytic reasoning engagement","authors":"Michaela Bronstein, Gordon Pennycook, L. Buonomano, Tyrone D. Cannon","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2020.1847190","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2020.1847190","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Analytic and intuitive reasoning processes have been implicated as important determinants of belief in (or skepticism of) fake news. However, the underlying cognitive mechanisms that encourage endorsement of fake news remain unclear. The present study investigated cognitive decoupling/response inhibition and the potential role of conflict processing in the initiation of analytic thought about fake news as factors that may facilitate skepticism. A base-rate task was used to test the hypotheses that conflict processing deficits and inefficient response inhibition would be related to stronger endorsement of fake news. In support of these hypotheses, increased belief in fake (but not real) news was associated with a smaller decrease in response confidence in the presence (vs. absence) of conflict and with inefficient (in terms of response latency) inhibition of prepotent responses. Through its support for these hypotheses, the present study advances efforts to determine who will fall for fake news, and why.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"3 1","pages":"510 - 535"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-11-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"80958611","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
Affective forecasting: Why can’t people predict their emotions? 情感预测:为什么人们不能预测自己的情绪?
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-10-28 DOI: 10.4324/9781003059745-5
P. Ayton, Alice Pott, N. Elwakili
{"title":"Affective forecasting: Why can’t people predict their emotions?","authors":"P. Ayton, Alice Pott, N. Elwakili","doi":"10.4324/9781003059745-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003059745-5","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"4 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-10-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"75344764","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
When nomenclature matters: Is the “new paradigm” really a new paradigm for the psychology of reasoning? 当命名法起作用时:“新范式”真的是推理心理学的新范式吗?
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-10-22 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2021.1990126
M. Knauff, Lupita Estefania Gazzo Castañeda
Abstract For most of its history, the psychology of reasoning was dominated by binary extensional logic. The so-called “new paradigm” instead puts subjective degrees of belief center stage, often represented as probabilities. We argue that the “new paradigm” is too vaguely defined and therefore does not allow a clear decision about what falls within its scope and what does not. We also show that there was not one settled theoretical “old” paradigm, before the new developments emerged, and that the alleged new paradigm is less revolutionary as the term suggests. A more veridical view is that current progress is developing in continuities where rival research programs can thrive or fail in the face of new experimental findings. The article closes with some topics where more connections between competing research programs are likely to promote progress in our understanding of human reasoning.
在推理心理学的大部分历史中,它是由二元外延逻辑主导的。而所谓的“新范式”则把主观的信念程度放在中心位置,通常用概率来表示。我们认为,“新范式”的定义过于模糊,因此不允许明确决定哪些属于其范围,哪些不属于。我们还表明,在新的发展出现之前,没有一个固定的理论“旧”范式,并且所谓的新范式并不像术语所暗示的那样具有革命性。一个更可靠的观点是,当前的进展是不断发展的,在新的实验发现面前,竞争对手的研究项目可以繁荣或失败。文章以一些主题结束,在这些主题中,相互竞争的研究项目之间的更多联系可能会促进我们对人类推理的理解的进步。
{"title":"When nomenclature matters: Is the “new paradigm” really a new paradigm for the psychology of reasoning?","authors":"M. Knauff, Lupita Estefania Gazzo Castañeda","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2021.1990126","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2021.1990126","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract For most of its history, the psychology of reasoning was dominated by binary extensional logic. The so-called “new paradigm” instead puts subjective degrees of belief center stage, often represented as probabilities. We argue that the “new paradigm” is too vaguely defined and therefore does not allow a clear decision about what falls within its scope and what does not. We also show that there was not one settled theoretical “old” paradigm, before the new developments emerged, and that the alleged new paradigm is less revolutionary as the term suggests. A more veridical view is that current progress is developing in continuities where rival research programs can thrive or fail in the face of new experimental findings. The article closes with some topics where more connections between competing research programs are likely to promote progress in our understanding of human reasoning.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"16 1","pages":"341 - 370"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-10-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89058086","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Everyday reasoning with unfamiliar conditionals 用不熟悉的条件句进行日常推理
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-10-01 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2020.1823478
Lupita Estefania Gazzo Castañeda, M. Knauff
Abstract Probabilistic theories of reasoning assume that people use their prior knowledge to estimate the conditional probability of q given p and that this probability predicts the acceptance of modus ponens inferences. But how do people reason with unfamiliar conditionals for which they do not have prior knowledge? Reasoning without prior knowledge has been extensively investigated in experiments in which participants were instructed to reason deductively. But it is still not clear how people reason with unfamiliar conditionals when they are instructed to reason as in daily life. Can probabilities also predict reasoning with unfamiliar content? In two experiments we instructed participants to reason as in everyday life and to evaluate conclusions from familiar and unfamiliar conditionals. Results showed that reasoning with familiar conditionals can be predicted by the conditional probability. For unfamiliar conditionals, however, this was not the case. Conclusions from unfamiliar conditionals were accepted more strongly than what could be expected according to their conditional probability. Our findings challenge probabilistic theories of reasoning and suggest that other approaches, such as a dual-strategy model, describe our results more adequately.
推理的概率论假设人们使用他们的先验知识来估计给定p的q的条件概率,并且这个概率预测了对命题式推理的接受程度。但是人们是如何用他们没有先验知识的陌生条件推理的呢?在实验中,没有先验知识的推理被广泛研究,在实验中,参与者被指示进行演绎推理。但是,当人们在日常生活中被要求进行推理时,他们是如何用不熟悉的条件句进行推理的,目前还不清楚。概率也能预测不熟悉内容的推理吗?在两个实验中,我们指导参与者像在日常生活中一样进行推理,并从熟悉和不熟悉的条件中评估结论。结果表明,条件概率可以对熟悉条件的推理进行预测。然而,对于不熟悉的条件句,情况并非如此。从不熟悉的条件句中得出的结论比根据条件概率可以预期的结论更容易被接受。我们的研究结果挑战了推理的概率理论,并建议其他方法,如双策略模型,更充分地描述我们的结果。
{"title":"Everyday reasoning with unfamiliar conditionals","authors":"Lupita Estefania Gazzo Castañeda, M. Knauff","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2020.1823478","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2020.1823478","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Probabilistic theories of reasoning assume that people use their prior knowledge to estimate the conditional probability of q given p and that this probability predicts the acceptance of modus ponens inferences. But how do people reason with unfamiliar conditionals for which they do not have prior knowledge? Reasoning without prior knowledge has been extensively investigated in experiments in which participants were instructed to reason deductively. But it is still not clear how people reason with unfamiliar conditionals when they are instructed to reason as in daily life. Can probabilities also predict reasoning with unfamiliar content? In two experiments we instructed participants to reason as in everyday life and to evaluate conclusions from familiar and unfamiliar conditionals. Results showed that reasoning with familiar conditionals can be predicted by the conditional probability. For unfamiliar conditionals, however, this was not the case. Conclusions from unfamiliar conditionals were accepted more strongly than what could be expected according to their conditional probability. Our findings challenge probabilistic theories of reasoning and suggest that other approaches, such as a dual-strategy model, describe our results more adequately.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"42 1","pages":"389 - 416"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89232123","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Autism limits strategic thinking after all: A process tracing study of the beauty contest game 自闭症最终限制了战略思维:对选美比赛游戏的过程追踪研究
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2020-10-01 DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2019.1679256
M. Król, M. Król
Abstract The beauty contest game is widely used to study the determinants of strategic thinking. Here, we examine the role of theory of mind in strategic reasoning by comparing both performance and the reasoning process in participants with autism vs. typically developing controls. Pantelis and Kennedy (2017) reported a surprising lack of difference between answers of participant with autism vs. controls in the game. Here, we study the process rather than just the outcome of reasoning by using a ‘payoff calculator’ with which one can simulate the game’s outcome before answering. We find that control participants play best response to others’ hypothetical choices entered into the calculator, while participants with autism are less strategic, choosing larger answers relative to those attributed to others. However, this difference could not be explained by the differences in the theory of mind between the groups with and without autism.
摘要:选美比赛游戏被广泛用于研究战略思维的决定因素。在这里,我们通过比较自闭症参与者与正常发展对照组的表现和推理过程来研究心理理论在策略推理中的作用。Pantelis和Kennedy(2017)报告说,在游戏中,自闭症参与者的答案与对照组的答案之间缺乏差异。在这里,我们通过使用“收益计算器”来研究推理的过程,而不仅仅是结果,人们可以在回答之前模拟游戏的结果。我们发现控制组的参与者会对输入计算器的其他人的假设性选择做出最佳反应,而患有自闭症的参与者则不那么有策略,他们会选择相对于他人给出的答案更大的答案。然而,这种差异不能用自闭症患者和非自闭症患者在心智理论上的差异来解释。
{"title":"Autism limits strategic thinking after all: A process tracing study of the beauty contest game","authors":"M. Król, M. Król","doi":"10.1080/13546783.2019.1679256","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2019.1679256","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The beauty contest game is widely used to study the determinants of strategic thinking. Here, we examine the role of theory of mind in strategic reasoning by comparing both performance and the reasoning process in participants with autism vs. typically developing controls. Pantelis and Kennedy (2017) reported a surprising lack of difference between answers of participant with autism vs. controls in the game. Here, we study the process rather than just the outcome of reasoning by using a ‘payoff calculator’ with which one can simulate the game’s outcome before answering. We find that control participants play best response to others’ hypothetical choices entered into the calculator, while participants with autism are less strategic, choosing larger answers relative to those attributed to others. However, this difference could not be explained by the differences in the theory of mind between the groups with and without autism.","PeriodicalId":47270,"journal":{"name":"Thinking & Reasoning","volume":"37 1","pages":"615 - 626"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"81276896","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Thinking & Reasoning
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1