首页 > 最新文献

Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Learning to control orientation and force in a hammering task: The initial stage. 学习在锤击任务中控制方向和力量:初级阶段。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2012-01-01 DOI: 10.1027/2151-2604/A000088
C. Vernooij, L. Mouton, R. Bongers
The ability to create stone tools is considered an important step in the emergence of human cognition. To further our understanding of these evolutionary processes we focused on the initial learning processes with which this percussive skill may be acquired. We studied a hammering task in which participants had to create a ground force vector by hitting a target on a force plate with a hammerstone. The produced ground force vector was presented as an arrow on a computer screen and had to end in a displayed target. The target could vary in its angle of azimuth and inclination. Over 5 days, three of the five participants adapted a wrist joint angle and two of these three participants adapted a shoulder joint angle that affected only angle of inclination of the ground force vector. Length and angle of azimuth of the ground force vector were not affected. In learning to control a hammering task, the first parameter to be manipulated seems to be the angle of inclination by adjusting the wrist and shoulder join...
制造石器的能力被认为是人类认知出现的重要一步。为了进一步了解这些进化过程,我们将重点放在了获得这种敲击技能的初始学习过程上。我们研究了一个锤击任务,在这个任务中,参与者必须通过用锤击石击打力板上的目标来创建一个地面力向量。生成的地面力矢量在计算机屏幕上以箭头的形式呈现,并以显示的目标结束。目标可以改变其方位角和倾斜角。在5天的时间里,5名参与者中的3名调整了手腕关节的角度,这3名参与者中的2名调整了仅影响地面力矢量倾斜角的肩关节角度。地面力矢量的长度和方位角不受影响。在学习控制锤击任务时,要操纵的第一个参数似乎是通过调整手腕和肩膀连接来控制倾斜角度。
{"title":"Learning to control orientation and force in a hammering task: The initial stage.","authors":"C. Vernooij, L. Mouton, R. Bongers","doi":"10.1027/2151-2604/A000088","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/A000088","url":null,"abstract":"The ability to create stone tools is considered an important step in the emergence of human cognition. To further our understanding of these evolutionary processes we focused on the initial learning processes with which this percussive skill may be acquired. We studied a hammering task in which participants had to create a ground force vector by hitting a target on a force plate with a hammerstone. The produced ground force vector was presented as an arrow on a computer screen and had to end in a displayed target. The target could vary in its angle of azimuth and inclination. Over 5 days, three of the five participants adapted a wrist joint angle and two of these three participants adapted a shoulder joint angle that affected only angle of inclination of the ground force vector. Length and angle of azimuth of the ground force vector were not affected. In learning to control a hammering task, the first parameter to be manipulated seems to be the angle of inclination by adjusting the wrist and shoulder join...","PeriodicalId":47289,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology","volume":"36 1","pages":"29-36"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2012-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"74218854","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
The proactive control of stereotype activation: Implicit goals to not stereotype 刻板印象激活的主动控制:内隐目标到非刻板印象
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2012-01-01 DOI: 10.1027/2151-2604/A000110
G. Moskowitz, Jeff Stone
Stereotypes are typically conceived of as controlled through conscious willing. We propose that goals can lead to stereotype control even when the goals are not consciously noted. This is called proactive control since goal pursuit occurs not as a reaction to a stereotype having been activated and having exerted influence, but as an act of goal shielding that inhibits stereotypes instead of activating them. In two experiments proactive control over stereotypes toward African Americans was illustrated using a lexical decision task. In Experiment 1, participants with egalitarian goals showed slower responses to stereotypic words when following an African American male face (relative to following a White face). Experiment 2 illustrated African American faces facilitated responses to stimuli relevant to egalitarian goals; White faces did not. Together, these studies indicate that, without consciously trying, participants with egalitarian goals’ implicit reaction to African Americans included triggering fairne...
刻板印象通常被认为是通过有意识的意愿来控制的。我们认为目标可以导致刻板印象控制,即使目标没有被有意识地注意到。这被称为主动控制,因为目标追求不是对刻板印象被激活和施加影响的反应,而是作为一种目标屏蔽行为,抑制刻板印象,而不是激活它们。在两个实验中,使用词汇决策任务说明了对非裔美国人刻板印象的主动控制。在实验1中,具有平等主义目标的参与者在跟随非裔美国男性面孔时(相对于跟随白人面孔)对刻板印象词汇的反应较慢。实验2显示非裔美国人面孔对平等主义目标相关刺激的反应更容易;而白人则不然。总之,这些研究表明,在没有有意识尝试的情况下,具有平等主义目标的参与者对非裔美国人的隐性反应包括触发公平……
{"title":"The proactive control of stereotype activation: Implicit goals to not stereotype","authors":"G. Moskowitz, Jeff Stone","doi":"10.1027/2151-2604/A000110","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/A000110","url":null,"abstract":"Stereotypes are typically conceived of as controlled through conscious willing. We propose that goals can lead to stereotype control even when the goals are not consciously noted. This is called proactive control since goal pursuit occurs not as a reaction to a stereotype having been activated and having exerted influence, but as an act of goal shielding that inhibits stereotypes instead of activating them. In two experiments proactive control over stereotypes toward African Americans was illustrated using a lexical decision task. In Experiment 1, participants with egalitarian goals showed slower responses to stereotypic words when following an African American male face (relative to following a White face). Experiment 2 illustrated African American faces facilitated responses to stimuli relevant to egalitarian goals; White faces did not. Together, these studies indicate that, without consciously trying, participants with egalitarian goals’ implicit reaction to African Americans included triggering fairne...","PeriodicalId":47289,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology","volume":"108 1","pages":"172-179"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2012-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"76933932","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
Where to Grasp a Tool? Task-Dependent Adjustments of Tool Transformations by Tool Users 如何掌握工具?工具用户对工具转换的任务相关调整
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2012-01-01 DOI: 10.1027/2151-2604/A000089
O. Herbort
Biomechanical and environmental constraints limit body movements and tool use actions. However, in the case of tool use, such constraints can often be overcome by adjusting a tool's tool transformation to the requirements of the intended tool use action. The research presented here examined whether participants grasped a lever at different positions, thus modifying the lever's tool transformation, to accommodate speed and accuracy requirements of different tasks. Participants were asked to quickly track a sequence of targets with the lever. If accuracy requirements were high, participants compensated for limits in the accuracy of hand movements by grasping the lever at a position that enabled precise control of the lever. If accuracy requirements were low, participants compensated for limits in hand speed by grasping the lever at a position that enabled fast lever movements with comparatively slow hand movements. This task-dependent grasp selection was only present after participants had practiced the tasks. The data show that in addition to adapting to fixed tool transformations, participants also actively controlled tool transformations to facilitate tool use actions.
生物力学和环境约束限制了身体运动和工具使用行为。然而,在工具使用的情况下,这种约束通常可以通过调整工具的工具转换以满足预期的工具使用动作的要求来克服。本文的研究考察了参与者是否在不同的位置抓住杠杆,从而改变杠杆的工具转换,以适应不同任务的速度和精度要求。参与者被要求用杠杆快速追踪一系列目标。如果精度要求很高,参与者通过在一个能够精确控制杠杆的位置抓住杠杆来补偿手部运动精度的限制。如果精度要求较低,参与者通过在一个位置抓住杠杆来补偿手的速度限制,这个位置可以用相对较慢的手运动来实现快速的杠杆运动。这种与任务相关的把握选择只在参与者练习了任务后才出现。数据表明,除了适应固定的工具转换外,参与者还积极控制工具转换以促进工具使用行为。
{"title":"Where to Grasp a Tool? Task-Dependent Adjustments of Tool Transformations by Tool Users","authors":"O. Herbort","doi":"10.1027/2151-2604/A000089","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/A000089","url":null,"abstract":"Biomechanical and environmental constraints limit body movements and tool use actions. However, in the case of tool use, such constraints can often be overcome by adjusting a tool's tool transformation to the requirements of the intended tool use action. The research presented here examined whether participants grasped a lever at different positions, thus modifying the lever's tool transformation, to accommodate speed and accuracy requirements of different tasks. Participants were asked to quickly track a sequence of targets with the lever. If accuracy requirements were high, participants compensated for limits in the accuracy of hand movements by grasping the lever at a position that enabled precise control of the lever. If accuracy requirements were low, participants compensated for limits in hand speed by grasping the lever at a position that enabled fast lever movements with comparatively slow hand movements. This task-dependent grasp selection was only present after participants had practiced the tasks. The data show that in addition to adapting to fixed tool transformations, participants also actively controlled tool transformations to facilitate tool use actions.","PeriodicalId":47289,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology","volume":"69 1","pages":"37-43"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2012-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"78711253","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Analyzing response time distributions: Methodological and theoretical suggestions for prospective memory researchers 分析反应时间分布:对前瞻性记忆研究者的方法和理论建议
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2011-12-01 DOI: 10.1027/2151-2604/A000056
Gene A. Brewer
The analysis of response times from prospective memory experiments has resulted in multiple theoretical propositions about the role of attention in prospective memory. Extant theories of prospective memory are in good agreement that attention is necessary for detecting intention-related cues. However, these theories were primarily formulated to describe differences in mean reaction times across experimental conditions. While this approach has been fruitful for establishing a fundamental relation between attention and prospective memory, reaction time modeling techniques can be applied to prospective memory data to better constrain theorizing. In the current work, the ex-Gaussian distribution is fit to data from a prospective memory task. The results from this analysis suggest that modeling reaction time data has the potential for clarifying our understanding of the role of attention in prospective memory.
对前瞻记忆实验反应时间的分析得出了关于注意在前瞻记忆中的作用的多种理论命题。现存的前瞻记忆理论一致认为,注意对于发现意图相关线索是必要的。然而,这些理论主要是用来描述不同实验条件下平均反应时间的差异。虽然这种方法在建立注意和前瞻记忆之间的基本关系方面取得了丰硕成果,但反应时间建模技术可以应用于前瞻记忆数据,以更好地约束理论化。在目前的工作中,前高斯分布适合于前瞻性记忆任务的数据。这一分析的结果表明,模拟反应时间数据有可能澄清我们对注意力在前瞻记忆中的作用的理解。
{"title":"Analyzing response time distributions: Methodological and theoretical suggestions for prospective memory researchers","authors":"Gene A. Brewer","doi":"10.1027/2151-2604/A000056","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/A000056","url":null,"abstract":"The analysis of response times from prospective memory experiments has resulted in multiple theoretical propositions about the role of attention in prospective memory. Extant theories of prospective memory are in good agreement that attention is necessary for detecting intention-related cues. However, these theories were primarily formulated to describe differences in mean reaction times across experimental conditions. While this approach has been fruitful for establishing a fundamental relation between attention and prospective memory, reaction time modeling techniques can be applied to prospective memory data to better constrain theorizing. In the current work, the ex-Gaussian distribution is fit to data from a prospective memory task. The results from this analysis suggest that modeling reaction time data has the potential for clarifying our understanding of the role of attention in prospective memory.","PeriodicalId":47289,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":"117-124"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2011-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"75049269","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 39
Varying the Complexity of the Prospective Memory Decision Process in an Air Traffic Control Simulation 在空中交通管制模拟中改变前瞻记忆决策过程的复杂性
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2011-04-01 DOI: 10.1027/2151-2604/A000051
S. Loft, B. Pearcy, R. Remington
Operators that monitor and control dynamic displays (air traffic control [ATC], naval radar tracking) sometimes fail to remember to perform deferred tasks. Such memory failures have been studied in the laboratory, but only recently applied to tasks such as ATC (Loft & Remington, 2010; Loft, Finnerty, & Remington, 2011; Loft, Smith, & Bhaskara, 2011). In work domains such as ATC, operators must often consider multiple display features before determining an action. The current study examined the effect of varying the number of aircraft display features that individuals need to process for the prospective memory (PM) task. Participants made more PM errors, and were slower to make aircraft acceptance decisions and to detect conflicts, when the PM task required that target aircraft satisfied one of the two possible conditions, compared to only one possible condition. Directions for research are discussed that should continue to bridge the gap between PM in basic and applied settings.
监控和控制动态显示(空中交通管制[ATC],海军雷达跟踪)的操作员有时会忘记执行延迟的任务。这种记忆故障已经在实验室中进行了研究,但直到最近才应用于ATC等任务(Loft & Remington, 2010;Loft, Finnerty, & Remington, 2011;Loft, Smith, & Bhaskara, 2011)。在空管等工作领域,操作员在决定操作之前必须经常考虑多个显示功能。目前的研究考察了个体在前瞻记忆(PM)任务中需要处理的不同飞机显示特征数量的影响。与只有一个可能条件相比,当PM任务要求目标飞机满足两个可能条件中的一个时,参与者犯了更多的PM错误,并且做出飞机接受决策和检测冲突的速度较慢。研究方向进行了讨论,应继续弥合差距之间的PM在基础和应用设置。
{"title":"Varying the Complexity of the Prospective Memory Decision Process in an Air Traffic Control Simulation","authors":"S. Loft, B. Pearcy, R. Remington","doi":"10.1027/2151-2604/A000051","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/A000051","url":null,"abstract":"Operators that monitor and control dynamic displays (air traffic control [ATC], naval radar tracking) sometimes fail to remember to perform deferred tasks. Such memory failures have been studied in the laboratory, but only recently applied to tasks such as ATC (Loft & Remington, 2010; Loft, Finnerty, & Remington, 2011; Loft, Smith, & Bhaskara, 2011). In work domains such as ATC, operators must often consider multiple display features before determining an action. The current study examined the effect of varying the number of aircraft display features that individuals need to process for the prospective memory (PM) task. Participants made more PM errors, and were slower to make aircraft acceptance decisions and to detect conflicts, when the PM task required that target aircraft satisfied one of the two possible conditions, compared to only one possible condition. Directions for research are discussed that should continue to bridge the gap between PM in basic and applied settings.","PeriodicalId":47289,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology","volume":"20 1","pages":"77-84"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2011-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72899951","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Torture as Negative Excessive Behavior of Revenge and Punishment 酷刑作为报复和惩罚的消极过度行为
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2011-01-01 DOI: 10.1027/2151-2604/A000067
A. Maercker, Tobias M. Glück
Several successful research traditions in the psychology of revenge and punishment have developed in German-speaking psychology over the last two decades. They have provided insights into topics such as retributive justice and social discrimination or social punishment. In the following, recent studies will be summarized, followed by a research agenda on revenge phenomena and implications for future research.
在过去的二十年中,在德语心理学中发展了一些成功的报复和惩罚心理学研究传统。他们提供了诸如报复性正义和社会歧视或社会惩罚等主题的见解。在下面,将总结最近的研究,然后是报复现象的研究议程和对未来研究的启示。
{"title":"Torture as Negative Excessive Behavior of Revenge and Punishment","authors":"A. Maercker, Tobias M. Glück","doi":"10.1027/2151-2604/A000067","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/A000067","url":null,"abstract":"Several successful research traditions in the psychology of revenge and punishment have developed in German-speaking psychology over the last two decades. They have provided insights into topics such as retributive justice and social discrimination or social punishment. In the following, recent studies will be summarized, followed by a research agenda on revenge phenomena and implications for future research.","PeriodicalId":47289,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology","volume":"190 1","pages":"184-186"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2011-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"74169787","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Trauma of Torture and the Rehabilitation of Torture Survivors 酷刑的创伤和酷刑幸存者的康复
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2011-01-01 DOI: 10.1027/2151-2604/A000060
L. Hárdi, A. Kroó
The aim of torture is to cause severe pain and suffering in order to destroy the structure of the personality and the identity of the victim. Torture is applied in over a 100 countries worldwide, and its consequences affect millions of survivors. The rehabilitation of those who have experienced torture is a lengthy and complex process; treatment centers all over the world are constantly developing their methods to assist the mental and physical healing of torture survivors. The present article offers insights into the nature of torture, applied torture techniques, the psychological sequelae of torture, and diagnostic developments. Furthermore, current issues of rehabilitation and reparation are discussed, including the debate on evidence-based practice in treatment. The aim of the authors is to offer a brief but comprehensive review on torture and rehabilitation for professionals of mental health care and other relevant fields.
酷刑的目的是造成严重的痛苦和折磨,以破坏受害者的人格结构和身份。全世界有100多个国家实行酷刑,其后果影响到数百万幸存者。那些经历过酷刑的人的康复是一个漫长而复杂的过程;世界各地的治疗中心都在不断发展他们的方法来帮助酷刑幸存者的精神和身体康复。这篇文章提供了对酷刑的本质、酷刑技术的应用、酷刑的心理后遗症和诊断发展的见解。此外,还讨论了当前的康复和修复问题,包括关于治疗中循证实践的辩论。作者的目的是为精神卫生保健和其他相关领域的专业人员提供关于酷刑和康复的简短而全面的综述。
{"title":"The Trauma of Torture and the Rehabilitation of Torture Survivors","authors":"L. Hárdi, A. Kroó","doi":"10.1027/2151-2604/A000060","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/A000060","url":null,"abstract":"The aim of torture is to cause severe pain and suffering in order to destroy the structure of the personality and the identity of the victim. Torture is applied in over a 100 countries worldwide, and its consequences affect millions of survivors. The rehabilitation of those who have experienced torture is a lengthy and complex process; treatment centers all over the world are constantly developing their methods to assist the mental and physical healing of torture survivors. The present article offers insights into the nature of torture, applied torture techniques, the psychological sequelae of torture, and diagnostic developments. Furthermore, current issues of rehabilitation and reparation are discussed, including the debate on evidence-based practice in treatment. The aim of the authors is to offer a brief but comprehensive review on torture and rehabilitation for professionals of mental health care and other relevant fields.","PeriodicalId":47289,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology","volume":"24 1","pages":"133-142"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2011-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86172250","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 18
Digit Repetition Effect in Two-Digit Number Comparison 两位数比较中的数字重复效应
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2011-01-01 DOI: 10.1027/2151-2604/A000043
S. Gazzellini, A. Laudanna
Previous data from two-digit number naming show that when target and prime numbers share one digit at different positions (e.g., target 28 and prime 86 or 72) an inhibition effect may be observed (Ratinckx, Brysbaert, & Fias, 2005). Such an effect has been ascribed to the mechanism of morpho-phonological transcoding from Arabic to verbal format. We evaluate the alternative hypothesis of an inhibition effect arising during the Arabic form processing when two different syntactic values have to be assigned to the same single digit. In Experiments 1 and 3, a digit repetition effect was observed in number comparison tasks, even when phonological transcoding is blocked by an articulatory suppression task. Conversely, Experiment 2 showed that no digit repetition effect can be found with number comparison of verbal written numbers. Results are compatible with an Arabic-syntactic processing stage, where units and decades are decomposed and where each single digit is activated and recognized.
先前的两位数命名数据表明,当目标数和素数在不同位置共享一个数字时(例如目标28和素数86或72),可能会观察到抑制效应(Ratinckx, Brysbaert, & Fias, 2005)。这种效应被归因于从阿拉伯语到口头格式的词形音位转码机制。我们评估了另一种假设,即在阿拉伯语形式处理过程中,当两个不同的句法值必须分配给同一个个位数时,会产生抑制效应。在实验1和3中,即使语音转码被发音抑制任务阻断,在数字比较任务中也观察到数字重复效应。相反,实验2表明,口头书写数字的数字比较不存在数字重复效应。结果与阿拉伯语句法处理阶段兼容,在这个阶段,单位和几十被分解,每个单个数字被激活和识别。
{"title":"Digit Repetition Effect in Two-Digit Number Comparison","authors":"S. Gazzellini, A. Laudanna","doi":"10.1027/2151-2604/A000043","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/A000043","url":null,"abstract":"Previous data from two-digit number naming show that when target and prime numbers share one digit at different positions (e.g., target 28 and prime 86 or 72) an inhibition effect may be observed (Ratinckx, Brysbaert, & Fias, 2005). Such an effect has been ascribed to the mechanism of morpho-phonological transcoding from Arabic to verbal format. We evaluate the alternative hypothesis of an inhibition effect arising during the Arabic form processing when two different syntactic values have to be assigned to the same single digit. In Experiments 1 and 3, a digit repetition effect was observed in number comparison tasks, even when phonological transcoding is blocked by an articulatory suppression task. Conversely, Experiment 2 showed that no digit repetition effect can be found with number comparison of verbal written numbers. Results are compatible with an Arabic-syntactic processing stage, where units and decades are decomposed and where each single digit is activated and recognized.","PeriodicalId":47289,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology","volume":"3 1","pages":"30-36"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2011-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"73553893","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Prospective memory: Beyond the cost debate 前瞻性记忆:超越成本争论
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2011-01-01 DOI: 10.1027/2151-2604/A000050
Rebekah E. Smith
{"title":"Prospective memory: Beyond the cost debate","authors":"Rebekah E. Smith","doi":"10.1027/2151-2604/A000050","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/A000050","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47289,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology","volume":"60 1","pages":"75-76"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2011-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86377173","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Time-Based Prospective Memory in Schoolchildren 学童基于时间的前瞻记忆
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2011-01-01 DOI: 10.1027/2151-2604/A000053
Babett Voigt, Ingo Aberle, Judith Schönfeld, M. Kliegel
The present study examined age differences in time-based prospective memory (TBPM) in primary school age children and tested the role of self-initiated memory retrieval and strategic time monitoring (TM) as possible developmental mechanisms. Fifty-four children were recruited from local primary schools (27 younger children, mean age = 7.2 ± 0.55 years, and 27 older children, mean age = 9.61 ± 0.71 years). The task was a driving game scenario in which children had to drive a vehicle (ongoing task) and to remember to refuel before the vehicle runs out of gas (TBPM task, i.e., the fuel gauge served as child-appropriate time equivalent). Fuel gauge was either displayed permanently (low level of self-initiation) or could only be viewed on demand by hitting a button (high level of self-initiation). The results revealed age-dependent TBPM differences with better performance in older children. In contrast, level of self-initiated memory retrieval did not affect TBPM performance. However, strategies of TM influenc...
本研究考察了小学学龄儿童基于时间的前瞻记忆(TBPM)的年龄差异,并验证了自我启动记忆检索和策略时间监控(TM)作为可能的发展机制的作用。从当地小学招募54名儿童,其中年龄较小的27名,平均年龄= 7.2±0.55岁;年龄较大的27名,平均年龄= 9.61±0.71岁。这个任务是一个驾驶游戏场景,在这个场景中,孩子们必须驾驶一辆车(正在进行的任务),并记得在汽车用完汽油之前加油(TBPM任务,也就是说,燃料计相当于孩子们适当的时间)。燃油表要么是永久显示的(低水平的自启动),要么只能在需要时通过点击按钮查看(高水平的自启动)。结果显示,TBPM存在年龄依赖性差异,年龄较大的儿童表现更好。相比之下,自我启动记忆检索水平对TBPM性能没有影响。然而,TM的策略影响…
{"title":"Time-Based Prospective Memory in Schoolchildren","authors":"Babett Voigt, Ingo Aberle, Judith Schönfeld, M. Kliegel","doi":"10.1027/2151-2604/A000053","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/A000053","url":null,"abstract":"The present study examined age differences in time-based prospective memory (TBPM) in primary school age children and tested the role of self-initiated memory retrieval and strategic time monitoring (TM) as possible developmental mechanisms. Fifty-four children were recruited from local primary schools (27 younger children, mean age = 7.2 ± 0.55 years, and 27 older children, mean age = 9.61 ± 0.71 years). The task was a driving game scenario in which children had to drive a vehicle (ongoing task) and to remember to refuel before the vehicle runs out of gas (TBPM task, i.e., the fuel gauge served as child-appropriate time equivalent). Fuel gauge was either displayed permanently (low level of self-initiation) or could only be viewed on demand by hitting a button (high level of self-initiation). The results revealed age-dependent TBPM differences with better performance in older children. In contrast, level of self-initiated memory retrieval did not affect TBPM performance. However, strategies of TM influenc...","PeriodicalId":47289,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology","volume":"3 1","pages":"92-99"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2011-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87755849","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 28
期刊
Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1