首页 > 最新文献

Journal of General Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Multiple sources of unconscious-information processing affect a single response: independent unconscious priming effects. 无意识信息处理的多种来源影响单一反应:独立的无意识引物效应。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-26 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2321536
Dingding Wang, Jiuhong Yan, Jun Li, Jerwen Jou, Jun Jiang, Jiang Qiu, Shen Tu

At present there is little knowledge on whether and how multiple pieces of unconscious information can simultaneously affect a single conscious response. In the present study, we manipulated the congruency relation between a masked prime arrow and the target arrow, as well as that between masked flankers and the target arrow. The results demonstrated that the masked prime and flankers produced independent unconscious priming effects on the response to the target. In the process of studying the above phenomenon, two secondary findings were made. First, although the prime congruency effect was obtained, the flanker congruency effect was smaller when the flankers were displayed simultaneously with the target than when they were displayed sequentially before the target. This suggested that priming stimulation required enough time to be processed to a sufficient extent to produce an unconscious priming effect. Second, when the prime stimulus was removed, leaving only the flankers, the flanker priming effect increased, suggesting that the attention attracted to the prime and its conscious mask could also reduce the flanker congruency effect. These results observed across several experiments were replicated in one within-subjects experiment. We proposed an "independent unconscious influence hypothesis" for the phenomenon. This hypothesis was further integrated into a more comprehensive unconscious information processing model. The possible causes of the observed phenomena were discussed.

目前,人们对多个无意识信息是否以及如何同时影响一个有意识反应知之甚少。在本研究中,我们操纵了被遮蔽的主箭头和目标箭头之间的一致性关系,以及被遮蔽的侧翼箭头和目标箭头之间的一致性关系。结果表明,被遮蔽的主箭头和侧翼箭头对目标的反应产生了独立的无意识引物效应。在研究上述现象的过程中,还得出了两个次要发现。首先,虽然获得了质点一致效应,但当侧翼物与目标物同时显示时,侧翼物一致效应要小于在目标物之前顺序显示时。这表明,引物刺激需要足够的时间才能被处理到足以产生无意识引物效应的程度。其次,当移除首要刺激,只留下侧翼刺激时,侧翼引物效应会增加,这表明首要刺激及其有意识遮蔽所吸引的注意力也会降低侧翼同位效应。这些在多个实验中观察到的结果在一个主体内实验中得到了重复。我们为这一现象提出了 "独立无意识影响假说"。这一假说被进一步整合到一个更全面的无意识信息处理模型中。我们还讨论了观察到的现象的可能原因。
{"title":"Multiple sources of unconscious-information processing affect a single response: independent unconscious priming effects.","authors":"Dingding Wang, Jiuhong Yan, Jun Li, Jerwen Jou, Jun Jiang, Jiang Qiu, Shen Tu","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2321536","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2321536","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>At present there is little knowledge on whether and how multiple pieces of unconscious information can simultaneously affect a single conscious response. In the present study, we manipulated the congruency relation between a masked prime arrow and the target arrow, as well as that between masked flankers and the target arrow. The results demonstrated that the masked prime and flankers produced independent unconscious priming effects on the response to the target. In the process of studying the above phenomenon, two secondary findings were made. First, although the prime congruency effect was obtained, the flanker congruency effect was smaller when the flankers were displayed simultaneously with the target than when they were displayed sequentially before the target. This suggested that priming stimulation required enough time to be processed to a sufficient extent to produce an unconscious priming effect. Second, when the prime stimulus was removed, leaving only the flankers, the flanker priming effect increased, suggesting that the attention attracted to the prime and its conscious mask could also reduce the flanker congruency effect. These results observed across several experiments were replicated in one within-subjects experiment. We proposed an \"independent unconscious influence hypothesis\" for the phenomenon. This hypothesis was further integrated into a more comprehensive unconscious information processing model. The possible causes of the observed phenomena were discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139974015","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Judgments of learning reactively affect memory by inducing covert retrieval. 对学习的判断通过诱导隐蔽性检索对记忆产生反应性影响。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-28 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2409785
Xiaojing Zhang, Yanlin Guo, Yingjie Jiang, Yuan Yuan

Making judgments of learning (JOLs) can directly influence memory, a phenomenon termed the reactivity effect of JOLs. However, controversy surrounds the mechanism behind JOL reactivity. This study employs related and unrelated word pairs as learning materials to compare memory outcomes across different JOL conditions. We contrasted the traditional JOL reactivity paradigm with a covert retrieval induction paradigm to explore whether JOLs impact memory through covert retrieval. In Experiment 1, data from 40 participants (18 females, 22 males) were analyzed, revealed distinct patterns between the two paradigms. When word pairs are presented entirely, the JOL group outperforms the no-JOL group in memorizing related pairs, aligning with traditional JOL reactivity. Conversely, when target words are omitted, the JOL group's memory resembles that of the no-JOL group. This comparison suggested that JOLs may prompt covert retrieval. In Experiment 2, which involved manipulating the retrieval strength, data from 52 participants (46 females, 6 males) were analyzed, yielded results consistent with those of Experiment 1. We conclude that covert retrieval significantly contributes to the JOL reactivity effect, enhancing memory through JOL-induced covert retrieval.

学习判断(JOL)可以直接影响记忆,这种现象被称为 JOL 的反应性效应。然而,JOL反应性背后的机制还存在争议。本研究采用相关和不相关的词对作为学习材料,比较不同JOL条件下的记忆结果。我们将传统的 JOL 反应性范式与隐蔽检索诱导范式进行了对比,以探讨 JOL 是否会通过隐蔽检索影响记忆。在实验 1 中,我们对 40 名参与者(18 名女性,22 名男性)的数据进行了分析,结果发现两种范式之间存在明显的差异。当词对完全呈现时,JOL 组在记忆相关词对方面优于无 JOL 组,这与传统的 JOL 反应性一致。相反,当目标词被省略时,JOL 组的记忆与无 JOL 组相似。这一对比表明,JOL 可能会促使隐蔽检索。在实验 2 中,我们对 52 名参与者(46 名女性,6 名男性)的数据进行了分析,结果与实验 1 一致。我们的结论是,隐蔽检索对 JOL 反应性效应有显著的促进作用,通过 JOL 诱导的隐蔽检索增强记忆。
{"title":"Judgments of learning reactively affect memory by inducing covert retrieval.","authors":"Xiaojing Zhang, Yanlin Guo, Yingjie Jiang, Yuan Yuan","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2409785","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2024.2409785","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Making judgments of learning (JOLs) can directly influence memory, a phenomenon termed the reactivity effect of JOLs. However, controversy surrounds the mechanism behind JOL reactivity. This study employs related and unrelated word pairs as learning materials to compare memory outcomes across different JOL conditions. We contrasted the traditional JOL reactivity paradigm with a covert retrieval induction paradigm to explore whether JOLs impact memory through covert retrieval. In Experiment 1, data from 40 participants (18 females, 22 males) were analyzed, revealed distinct patterns between the two paradigms. When word pairs are presented entirely, the JOL group outperforms the no-JOL group in memorizing related pairs, aligning with traditional JOL reactivity. Conversely, when target words are omitted, the JOL group's memory resembles that of the no-JOL group. This comparison suggested that JOLs may prompt covert retrieval. In Experiment 2, which involved manipulating the retrieval strength, data from 52 participants (46 females, 6 males) were analyzed, yielded results consistent with those of Experiment 1. We conclude that covert retrieval significantly contributes to the JOL reactivity effect, enhancing memory through JOL-induced covert retrieval.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-09-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142336968","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mindfulness matters: Unveiling the relationship between trait mindfulness and procrastination - A three level meta-analysis. 正念很重要:揭示正念特质与拖延症之间的关系--三层荟萃分析。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-28 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2407426
Yidi Mao, Juan Zhang, Yijun Liu, Yihui Wang

Procrastination is a personality tendency with several deleterious consequences. Trait mindfulness has been found to be associated with procrastination. Nevertheless, the relationship between trait mindfulness and procrastination has yielded inconsistent results in previous studies. Therefore, by utilizing a three-level random-effects meta-analysic model, the present study synthesized previous studies to investigate the relationship between trait mindfulness and procrastination and explore the potential moderators that may affect this relationship. A significant and negative relationship between trait mindfulness and procrastination (r = -0.365; p < 0.0001) was found by analyzing 36 effect sizes from 19 studies with 14,094 participants. The subsequent moderator analysis did not reveal any significant moderators. Overall, the current meta-analysis shed insightful light on information for future research on reducing procrastination by considering utilizing mindfulness-based interventions that make it easier for individuals to maintain a mindfulness state and enhance trait mindfulness, thereby reducing procrastination.

拖延是一种具有多种有害后果的人格倾向。有研究发现,特质正念与拖延症有关。然而,在以往的研究中,特质正念与拖延症之间关系的研究结果并不一致。因此,本研究利用三级随机效应荟萃分析模型,综合了以往的研究,探讨了特质正念与拖延之间的关系,并探索了可能影响这种关系的潜在调节因素。特质正念与拖延之间存在明显的负相关(r = -0.365; p
{"title":"Mindfulness matters: Unveiling the relationship between trait mindfulness and procrastination - A three level meta-analysis.","authors":"Yidi Mao, Juan Zhang, Yijun Liu, Yihui Wang","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2407426","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2407426","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Procrastination is a personality tendency with several deleterious consequences. Trait mindfulness has been found to be associated with procrastination. Nevertheless, the relationship between trait mindfulness and procrastination has yielded inconsistent results in previous studies. Therefore, by utilizing a three-level random-effects meta-analysic model, the present study synthesized previous studies to investigate the relationship between trait mindfulness and procrastination and explore the potential moderators that may affect this relationship. A significant and negative relationship between trait mindfulness and procrastination (<i>r</i> = -0.365; <i>p</i> < 0.0001) was found by analyzing 36 effect sizes from 19 studies with 14,094 participants. The subsequent moderator analysis did not reveal any significant moderators. Overall, the current meta-analysis shed insightful light on information for future research on reducing procrastination by considering utilizing mindfulness-based interventions that make it easier for individuals to maintain a mindfulness state and enhance trait mindfulness, thereby reducing procrastination.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-09-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142336969","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reality Cheque: Different Methods of Eliciting Fear-Related Beliefs Reveal Multiple Representations of Threat. 现实支票:激发恐惧相关信念的不同方法揭示了威胁的多重表征。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-22 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2405876
James W B Elsey, Vivian N Metselaar, Elias Geiser, Donna F Knoop, Suraya M Gangadien, Nella A Schrijver, Lena M van den Nieuwenhof, Véra M Spiekman, Marta J Jakschik, Casper M Enkelaar, Esperanza S J Visbeek, Marieke Effting, Merel Kindt

Influential models of fears and phobias suggest that irrational threat beliefs underpin excessive fear. Yet, many fearful individuals recognize their fear is not justified. Drawing on memory competition/multiple representations theory, we developed a novel, fear-relevant procedure, which reveals conflicting representations of threat. In three experiments (Experiment 1, N = 49, Experiment 2, N = 47, Experiment 3, N = 75), fearful and non-fearful participants not only provided Probability Ratings for fear-related outcomes in a fear-relevant exposure task, but placed Bets, with payoffs depending on what happened in reality. Fearful participants displayed much higher Probability Ratings than Low fear participants. However, Bets revealed far less consistent group differences, even when proximal to threat (Experiments 1 and 2), and differences between High and Low fear participants' Bets disappeared when they could not be anchored to previous Probability Ratings (Experiment 3). A Neutral Betting task also showed that general betting strategies were comparable between groups. We suggest that these findings may reflect the multi-representational nature of belief, in which both adaptive and maladaptive representations of a feared object may exist in parallel, with personal and contextual factors determining which form of representation is retrieved or expressed. This perspective can provide insights into the complex interplay of adaptive and maladaptive beliefs that is a central focus of currently dominant therapies.

具有影响力的恐惧和恐惧症模型表明,非理性的威胁信念是过度恐惧的基础。然而,许多恐惧者认识到他们的恐惧是不合理的。借鉴记忆竞争/多重表征理论,我们开发了一种新颖的、与恐惧相关的程序,它能揭示相互冲突的威胁表征。在三项实验中(实验 1,49 人;实验 2,47 人;实验 3,75 人),恐惧和非恐惧参与者不仅为恐惧相关暴露任务中的恐惧相关结果提供了概率评级,而且还下了赌注,赌注的回报取决于现实中发生的事情。恐惧参与者的概率评级远高于低恐惧参与者。然而,即使是在接近威胁的情况下,投注所显示的群体差异也不那么一致(实验 1 和 2),而且当高恐惧参与者和低恐惧参与者的投注不能锚定于之前的概率评级时,他们之间的差异就消失了(实验 3)。中性下注任务也表明,不同组之间的一般下注策略具有可比性。我们认为,这些发现可能反映了信念的多重表征性质,其中恐惧对象的适应性和不适应性表征可能同时存在,个人和环境因素决定了哪种表征形式被检索或表达。这种观点可以让我们深入了解适应性信念和适应不良信念之间复杂的相互作用,而这正是目前主流疗法的核心重点。
{"title":"Reality Cheque: Different Methods of Eliciting Fear-Related Beliefs Reveal Multiple Representations of Threat.","authors":"James W B Elsey, Vivian N Metselaar, Elias Geiser, Donna F Knoop, Suraya M Gangadien, Nella A Schrijver, Lena M van den Nieuwenhof, Véra M Spiekman, Marta J Jakschik, Casper M Enkelaar, Esperanza S J Visbeek, Marieke Effting, Merel Kindt","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2405876","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2024.2405876","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Influential models of fears and phobias suggest that irrational threat beliefs underpin excessive fear. Yet, many fearful individuals recognize their fear is not justified. Drawing on memory competition/multiple representations theory, we developed a novel, fear-relevant procedure, which reveals conflicting representations of threat. In three experiments (Experiment 1, <i>N</i> = 49, Experiment 2, <i>N</i> = 47, Experiment 3, <i>N</i> = 75), fearful and non-fearful participants not only provided Probability Ratings for fear-related outcomes in a fear-relevant exposure task, but placed Bets, with payoffs depending on what happened in reality. Fearful participants displayed much higher Probability Ratings than Low fear participants. However, Bets revealed far less consistent group differences, even when proximal to threat (Experiments 1 and 2), and differences between High and Low fear participants' Bets disappeared when they could not be anchored to previous Probability Ratings (Experiment 3). A Neutral Betting task also showed that general betting strategies were comparable between groups. We suggest that these findings may reflect the multi-representational nature of belief, in which both adaptive and maladaptive representations of a feared object may exist in parallel, with personal and contextual factors determining which form of representation is retrieved or expressed. This perspective can provide insights into the complex interplay of adaptive and maladaptive beliefs that is a central focus of currently dominant therapies.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-09-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298627","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The two-way relationship of interpersonal curiosity and daily envy. 人际好奇心与日常羡慕的双向关系。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-09-20 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2405042
Jiali He, Liu Hanshu

Objective: Envy and interpersonal curiosity are common emotional and psychological phenomena in relationships that collectively influence our social behavior and experiences. However, there is a research gap regarding the interaction between interpersonal curiosity and envy. Therefore, this study will primarily utilize daily diary methods to investigate the relationship between interpersonal curiosity and envy.

Method: Using the Benign and Malicious Envy Scale and the Chinese Adolescents' Interpersonal Curiosity Questionnaire, Study 1 conducted a preliminary study on 392 middle school students using cross-sectional data. Based on the data from Study 1, we selected the ten items (2 items per variable) with the highest factor load for the daily measurement of Study 2. Study 2 utilized the diary method and recruited 81 adolescents as participants for a 14-day measurement period.

Results: The results of Study 1indicate a significant correlation between malicious envy and interpersonal curiosity, as well as a significant correlation between benign envy and interpersonal curiosity (excluding general interpersonal curiosity). The findings from Study 2 indicate that daily malicious envy can enhance private interpersonal curiosity and general interpersonal curiosity, daily benign envy can enhance curiosity exploration, and daily curiosity exploration can also enhance benign envy.

Discussion: This study enhances comprehension of the two-way relationship between adolescent envy and interpersonal curiosity within the framework of social comparison theory. The findings underscore the significance of adolescents recognizing and respecting others' boundaries to mitigate the adverse effects linked to malicious envy. Furthermore, adolescents are encouraged to channel benign envy into a source of motivation, fostering personal growth and fostering positive relationships through curiosity exploration.

目的嫉妒和人际好奇心是人际关系中常见的情绪和心理现象,它们共同影响着我们的社会行为和体验。然而,关于人际好奇心与羡慕之间的互动关系的研究尚属空白。因此,本研究将主要采用每日日记法来研究人际好奇心与嫉妒之间的关系:研究 1 采用良性和恶意嫉妒量表和中国青少年人际好奇心问卷,对 392 名中学生进行了横断面初步研究。根据研究 1 的数据,我们选取了因子负荷最高的 10 个项目(每个变量 2 个项目)进行研究 2 的日常测量。研究 2 采用日记法,招募了 81 名青少年作为参与者,进行为期 14 天的测量:研究 1 的结果表明,恶意嫉妒与人际好奇心之间存在显著相关,良性嫉妒与人际好奇心(不包括一般人际好奇心)之间也存在显著相关。研究 2 的结果表明,日常恶意嫉妒能增强私人人际好奇心和一般人际好奇心,日常良性嫉妒能增强好奇心探索,日常好奇心探索也能增强良性嫉妒:本研究在社会比较理论的框架下,加深了对青少年嫉妒与人际好奇心之间双向关系的理解。研究结果强调了青少年认识和尊重他人界限的重要性,以减轻与恶意嫉妒相关的不良影响。此外,研究还鼓励青少年将善意的嫉妒转化为动力,通过好奇心的探索促进个人成长并建立积极的人际关系。
{"title":"The two-way relationship of interpersonal curiosity and daily envy.","authors":"Jiali He, Liu Hanshu","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2405042","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2024.2405042","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Envy and interpersonal curiosity are common emotional and psychological phenomena in relationships that collectively influence our social behavior and experiences. However, there is a research gap regarding the interaction between interpersonal curiosity and envy. Therefore, this study will primarily utilize daily diary methods to investigate the relationship between interpersonal curiosity and envy.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Using the Benign and Malicious Envy Scale and the Chinese Adolescents' Interpersonal Curiosity Questionnaire, Study 1 conducted a preliminary study on 392 middle school students using cross-sectional data. Based on the data from Study 1, we selected the ten items (2 items per variable) with the highest factor load for the daily measurement of Study 2. Study 2 utilized the diary method and recruited 81 adolescents as participants for a 14-day measurement period.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The results of Study 1indicate a significant correlation between malicious envy and interpersonal curiosity, as well as a significant correlation between benign envy and interpersonal curiosity (excluding general interpersonal curiosity). The findings from Study 2 indicate that daily malicious envy can enhance private interpersonal curiosity and general interpersonal curiosity, daily benign envy can enhance curiosity exploration, and daily curiosity exploration can also enhance benign envy.</p><p><strong>Discussion: </strong>This study enhances comprehension of the two-way relationship between adolescent envy and interpersonal curiosity within the framework of social comparison theory. The findings underscore the significance of adolescents recognizing and respecting others' boundaries to mitigate the adverse effects linked to malicious envy. Furthermore, adolescents are encouraged to channel benign envy into a source of motivation, fostering personal growth and fostering positive relationships through curiosity exploration.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298628","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of executive functions in long-term memory: case report. 执行功能在长期记忆中的作用:病例报告。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-08-06 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2385106
M P Salguero-Alcañiz, M M González-Ramírez, C Gómez-Heredia, J R Alameda-Bailén

The role of executive functions in long-term memory has been studied. We describe a single-case study, consisting of a 45-year-old male patient, hospitalized for right frontal stroke. After the stroke, the patient had memory alterations in everyday activities. However, performance in short-term memory tests was not significantly altered. Long-term memory assessments included pre- and post-stroke episodic, semantic, and procedural memories. Specific skills involved in the acquisition of new learning (auditory-verbal and visual reproduction) were also evaluated, as well as executive functions. The results evidence that short-term memory was not affected. Regarding long-term memory, significant differences were observed between pre- and post-stroke knowledge, the former being better preserved, which reveals anterograde amnesia. Pre-stroke long-term memory was also affected, but only with respect to episodic knowledge, with semantic and procedural memories preserved (episodic retrograde amnesia). Executive functions were altered as well, which could have been a factor affecting the acquisition and consolidation of new learning, despite the fact that short-term memory was not significantly altered. Therefore, executive functions might be a determinant factor in the acquisition of new learning, regardless of short-term memory processes, at least partially. According to the results of the present study, alterations in these functions might lead to anterograde amnesia. This entails the need to evaluate executive functions as an intrinsic part of memory evaluation.

人们一直在研究执行功能在长期记忆中的作用。我们描述了一项单例研究,研究对象是一名因右额叶中风住院的 45 岁男性患者。中风后,患者在日常活动中出现记忆改变。然而,短期记忆测试的表现并无明显改变。长期记忆评估包括中风前后的情节记忆、语义记忆和程序记忆。此外,还对获取新知识的特定技能(听觉-语言和视觉再现)以及执行功能进行了评估。结果表明,短期记忆没有受到影响。在长期记忆方面,卒中前和卒中后的知识之间存在显著差异,前者保存得更好,这表明存在逆行性遗忘。脑卒中前的长期记忆也受到了影响,但仅限于情节性知识,语义和程序性记忆得以保留(情节性逆行性遗忘)。执行功能也发生了改变,这可能是影响获取和巩固新知识的一个因素,尽管短期记忆并没有明显改变。因此,不管短期记忆过程如何,执行功能可能是获取新知识的决定性因素,至少是部分决定性因素。根据本研究的结果,这些功能的改变可能会导致逆行性遗忘症。因此,有必要将执行功能作为记忆评估的内在组成部分进行评估。
{"title":"The role of executive functions in long-term memory: case report.","authors":"M P Salguero-Alcañiz, M M González-Ramírez, C Gómez-Heredia, J R Alameda-Bailén","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2385106","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2024.2385106","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The role of executive functions in long-term memory has been studied. We describe a single-case study, consisting of a 45-year-old male patient, hospitalized for right frontal stroke. After the stroke, the patient had memory alterations in everyday activities. However, performance in short-term memory tests was not significantly altered. Long-term memory assessments included pre- and post-stroke episodic, semantic, and procedural memories. Specific skills involved in the acquisition of new learning (auditory-verbal and visual reproduction) were also evaluated, as well as executive functions. The results evidence that short-term memory was not affected. Regarding long-term memory, significant differences were observed between pre- and post-stroke knowledge, the former being better preserved, which reveals anterograde amnesia. Pre-stroke long-term memory was also affected, but only with respect to episodic knowledge, with semantic and procedural memories preserved (episodic retrograde amnesia). Executive functions were altered as well, which could have been a factor affecting the acquisition and consolidation of new learning, despite the fact that short-term memory was not significantly altered. Therefore, executive functions might be a determinant factor in the acquisition of new learning, regardless of short-term memory processes, at least partially. According to the results of the present study, alterations in these functions might lead to anterograde amnesia. This entails the need to evaluate executive functions as an intrinsic part of memory evaluation.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-08-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141898649","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Positive and negative factors of the influence of new media and the digital environment on ideal and belief education among university students in China. 新媒体和数字环境对中国大学生理想信念教育影响的积极因素和消极因素。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2385105
Dandan Xu, Shiqi Feng, Zijin Huang

The authors identity the relationship between the positive and negative aspects of social media and the ideal belief learning and behavior of university students. The cluster sampling method was adopted in the paper, including Guangdong, Shandong, Henan, Sichuan, and Jiangsu provinces. A total of 1014 questionnaires were distributed to a purposive sample of university students between the ages of 16 and 35. The authors applied the uses and gratifications theory to study students' social media behavior. This study identified 18 positive and negative effects of social media. Noteworthy positive outcomes attributed to social media in fostering ideals and beliefs encompass heightened awareness, enhanced communication facilitation, convenient connectivity, reduced expenses on educational materials, improved social and communication proficiencies, as well as diminished stress levels. The negative effects of new media and the Internet include a lack of critical thinking, a waste of time, dysgraphia, disrupted connection to learning, students' laziness, and health risks.

作者对社交媒体的正反两方面与大学生理想信念学习和行为之间的关系进行了辨析。本文采用聚类抽样法,包括广东、山东、河南、四川和江苏等省。对 16 至 35 岁的大学生进行了有目的的抽样调查,共发放问卷 1014 份。作者运用使用和满足理论研究了学生的社交媒体行为。这项研究确定了社交媒体的 18 种积极和消极影响。值得注意的是,社交媒体在培养理想和信念方面的积极影响包括提高意识、促进沟通、方便连接、减少教材费用、提高社交和沟通能力以及减轻压力。新媒体和互联网的负面影响包括缺乏批判性思维、浪费时间、书写障碍、学习联系中断、学生懒惰以及健康风险。
{"title":"Positive and negative factors of the influence of new media and the digital environment on ideal and belief education among university students in China.","authors":"Dandan Xu, Shiqi Feng, Zijin Huang","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2385105","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2024.2385105","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The authors identity the relationship between the positive and negative aspects of social media and the ideal belief learning and behavior of university students. The cluster sampling method was adopted in the paper, including Guangdong, Shandong, Henan, Sichuan, and Jiangsu provinces. A total of 1014 questionnaires were distributed to a purposive sample of university students between the ages of 16 and 35. The authors applied the uses and gratifications theory to study students' social media behavior. This study identified 18 positive and negative effects of social media. Noteworthy positive outcomes attributed to social media in fostering ideals and beliefs encompass heightened awareness, enhanced communication facilitation, convenient connectivity, reduced expenses on educational materials, improved social and communication proficiencies, as well as diminished stress levels. The negative effects of new media and the Internet include a lack of critical thinking, a waste of time, dysgraphia, disrupted connection to learning, students' laziness, and health risks.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141861253","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The detrimental and beneficial effects of collaboration are sensitive to both collaborative frequency and collaborative order but not to the encoding task. 协作的不利和有利效应对协作频率和协作顺序都很敏感,但对编码任务却不敏感。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-29 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2385098
Aiqing Nie, Si Liu

Extensive attention has been dedicated to studying the influence of others on genuine or false memory during ongoing and post-collaboration. These studies have revealed both detrimental and beneficial effects on episodic memory. Although ongoing effects such as collaborative inhibition have been examined in the semantic situation, the post-collaboration effects have not received the same level of scrutiny To address this gap, the current study instructed participants to either generate or remember idioms during the study phase, which encompassed semantic and episodic encoding. There were three recall sessions, during which four groups were designated: individual (III), preceding collaboration (CII), following collaboration (ICI), and multiple collaboration (CCI). The main results and implications of the study are outlined below. (a) The detrimental effect of collaborative inhibition was found to be sensitive to collaborative frequency, indicating that the contribution of retrieval strategy disruption proposed by the Retrieval Strategy Disruption Hypothesis (RSDH) is conditional. (b) We observed a reliable beneficial effect of error pruning, as evidenced by smaller errors in collaborators compared to individual participants. Furthermore, this beneficial effect was consistently evident in both ongoing and post-collaboration scenarios for the two encoding tasks. (c) The post-collaborative memory benefit was observed in both Recall 2 and Recall 3. This suggests that mechanisms such as relearning, cross-cueing, re-exposure, and pruning errors may have contributed to this effect. (d) The observation of the beneficial effects of picked-up and shared memory indicates the contribution of similar mechanisms as to post-collaborative memory benefit. (e) These effects were observed regardless of the encoding task, but they were influenced by both collaborative frequency and collaborative order. The results are discussed in terms of the RSDH and other relevant theories. Additionally, future research directions are provided.

人们广泛关注研究他人对正在进行的和合作后的真实或虚假记忆的影响。这些研究揭示了对外显记忆的有害和有利影响。为了弥补这一不足,本研究要求参与者在研究阶段生成或记住成语,研究阶段包括语义编码和外显编码。共进行了三次回忆,在此期间指定了四组:个人(III)、合作前(CII)、合作后(ICI)和多重合作(CCI)。研究的主要结果和意义概述如下。(a) 研究发现,合作抑制的不利影响对合作频率很敏感,这表明检索策略中断假说(RSDH)提出的检索策略中断的贡献是有条件的。(b) 我们观察到了错误修剪的可靠有益效应,合作者与个体参与者相比错误更少就是证明。此外,在两个编码任务中,这种有利效应在进行中和合作后的情景中都持续明显。(c) 在回忆 2 和回忆 3 中都观察到了合作后记忆的益处。这表明,重新学习、交叉提示、重新暴露和修剪错误等机制可能促成了这一效果。(d) 对拾取记忆和共享记忆的有利影响的观察表明,类似的机制对协作后记忆的益处有所贡献。(e) 无论编码任务是什么,都能观察到这些效应,但它们受到协作频率和协作顺序的影响。本文从 RSDH 和其他相关理论的角度讨论了这些结果。此外,还提供了未来的研究方向。
{"title":"The detrimental and beneficial effects of collaboration are sensitive to both collaborative frequency and collaborative order but not to the encoding task.","authors":"Aiqing Nie, Si Liu","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2385098","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2024.2385098","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Extensive attention has been dedicated to studying the influence of others on genuine or false memory during ongoing and post-collaboration. These studies have revealed both detrimental and beneficial effects on episodic memory. Although ongoing effects such as collaborative inhibition have been examined in the semantic situation, the post-collaboration effects have not received the same level of scrutiny To address this gap, the current study instructed participants to either generate or remember idioms during the study phase, which encompassed semantic and episodic encoding. There were three recall sessions, during which four groups were designated: individual (III), preceding collaboration (CII), following collaboration (ICI), and multiple collaboration (CCI). The main results and implications of the study are outlined below. (a) The detrimental effect of collaborative inhibition was found to be sensitive to collaborative frequency, indicating that the contribution of retrieval strategy disruption proposed by the Retrieval Strategy Disruption Hypothesis (RSDH) is conditional. (b) We observed a reliable beneficial effect of error pruning, as evidenced by smaller errors in collaborators compared to individual participants. Furthermore, this beneficial effect was consistently evident in both ongoing and post-collaboration scenarios for the two encoding tasks. (c) The post-collaborative memory benefit was observed in both Recall 2 and Recall 3. This suggests that mechanisms such as relearning, cross-cueing, re-exposure, and pruning errors may have contributed to this effect. (d) The observation of the beneficial effects of picked-up and shared memory indicates the contribution of similar mechanisms as to post-collaborative memory benefit. (e) These effects were observed regardless of the encoding task, but they were influenced by both collaborative frequency and collaborative order. The results are discussed in terms of the RSDH and other relevant theories. Additionally, future research directions are provided.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141793770","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The MoodWheel app: validation and factor structure of a new tool for the assessment of mood based on the experience sampling method. MoodWheel 应用程序:基于经验取样法的情绪评估新工具的验证和因子结构。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-19 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2378284
Oana A David, Cristina Tomoiagă, Liviu A Fodor

Background: The MoodWheel app is a newly developed tool that uses experience sampling method (ESM) for assessing negative and positive emotions, based on the circumplex model of emotions and the binary model of distress, and including behavioral and heart rate (HR) measurement via photoplethysmography and the possibility to personalize the application with additional measures. Aims: This study was designed to assess the factorial structure, reliability and validity of the MoodWheel (MW) application for evaluating emotions in children, adolescents.

Methods: A sample of 490 children and adolescents were recruited from the schools. Internal consistency was assessed via Cronbach's alpha test. Concurrent validity was assessed by evaluating the correlations between MW and Profile of Emotional Distress scale (PED) scores, in terms of functional/dysfunctional negative and positive emotions.

Results: Results obtained show that MW has good to excellent internal consistency and test-retest reliability, while the convergent validity was also adequate. Moreover, we found support for the organization of the MW based on the binary model of distress, given the predictive value found for the irrational and rational beliefs.

Conclusions: The MW application is a useful and easy to use tool that can be used for the accurate measurement of emotions, which will be complimented in future with additional behavioral parameters to provide a comprehensive and dynamic assessment.

背景介绍情绪轮(MoodWheel)应用程序是一种新开发的工具,它采用经验取样法(ESM)评估消极和积极情绪,以情绪的环状模型和二元痛苦模型为基础,包括行为和心率(HR)测量(通过光电血压计),并可通过其他测量方法对应用程序进行个性化设置。目的:本研究旨在评估用于评估儿童和青少年情绪的 MoodWheel(MW)应用程序的因子结构、可靠性和有效性:方法:从学校招募了 490 名儿童和青少年样本。通过 Cronbach's alpha 检验评估内部一致性。通过评估 MW 与情绪困扰量表(PED)分数之间在功能性/功能性消极和积极情绪方面的相关性来评估并发有效性:结果:研究结果表明,MW 具有良好至卓越的内部一致性和测试-再测可靠性,同时具有适当的收敛效度。此外,考虑到非理性和理性信念的预测价值,我们发现基于二元痛苦模型的 MW 组织方式也得到了支持:MW 应用程序是一种有用且易于使用的工具,可用于准确测量情绪,今后还将增加其他行为参数,以提供全面、动态的评估。
{"title":"The MoodWheel app: validation and factor structure of a new tool for the assessment of mood based on the experience sampling method.","authors":"Oana A David, Cristina Tomoiagă, Liviu A Fodor","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2378284","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2024.2378284","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>The MoodWheel app is a newly developed tool that uses experience sampling method (ESM) for assessing negative and positive emotions, based on the circumplex model of emotions and the binary model of distress, and including behavioral and heart rate (HR) measurement via photoplethysmography and the possibility to personalize the application with additional measures. Aims: This study was designed to assess the factorial structure, reliability and validity of the MoodWheel (MW) application for evaluating emotions in children, adolescents.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>A sample of 490 children and adolescents were recruited from the schools. Internal consistency was assessed via Cronbach's alpha test. Concurrent validity was assessed by evaluating the correlations between MW and Profile of Emotional Distress scale (PED) scores, in terms of functional/dysfunctional negative and positive emotions.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Results obtained show that MW has good to excellent internal consistency and test-retest reliability, while the convergent validity was also adequate. Moreover, we found support for the organization of the MW based on the binary model of distress, given the predictive value found for the irrational and rational beliefs.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The MW application is a useful and easy to use tool that can be used for the accurate measurement of emotions, which will be complimented in future with additional behavioral parameters to provide a comprehensive and dynamic assessment.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141724749","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotional contextual effects of face perception: a test of the affective realism hypothesis. 人脸感知的情感语境效应:对情感真实性假说的检验。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2378326
Yuan Gao, Wuji Lin, Jiaxi Liu, Yujie Chen, Chunqian Xiao, Jiexin Chen, Lei Mo

Affective feelings naturally infuse individuals' perceptions, serving as valid windows onto the real world. The affective realism hypothesis further explains how these feelings work: as properties of individuals' perceptual experiences, these feelings influence perception. Notably, this hypothesis based on affective feelings with different valences has been substantiated, whereas the existing evidence is not compelling enough. Moreover, whether specific affective feelings can be experienced as properties of target perception remains unclear. Addressing these two issues deepens our understanding of the nature of emotional representation. Hence, we investigated the affective realism hypothesis based on affective feelings with different valences and specific emotions, comparing it with the affective misattribution hypothesis. In Experiment 1, we examined the effects of affective feelings with various valences on targets' perception through the AM (1a) and CFS paradigms (1b). In Experiment 2, we investigated the effects of affective feelings with anger, sadness, and disgust using similar methods. Results from Experiments 1a and 1b consistently indicated significant differences in valence ratings of neutral faces under emotional contexts with varying valences. Experiment 2a revealed significant differences in specific emotion ratings of neutral faces under different specific emotional contexts in the AM paradigm, whereas such differences were not observed in the CFS paradigm in Experiment 2b. We concluded that affective feelings with different valences, rather than specific emotions, can be experienced as inherent properties of target perception, validating the affective realism hypothesis. These findings supported the view that the nature of emotional representation should be described as affective dimensions.

情感自然而然地渗透到个体的感知中,成为观察真实世界的有效窗口。情感现实主义假说进一步解释了这些情感是如何起作用的:作为个体知觉经验的属性,这些情感影响着知觉。值得注意的是,这一假说基于不同价值的情感感受得到了证实,而现有的证据还不够有力。此外,特定的情感感受是否能作为目标知觉的属性而被体验到,这一点仍不清楚。解决这两个问题将加深我们对情感表征本质的理解。因此,我们研究了基于不同价值和特定情绪的情感真实假说,并将其与情感错误归因假说进行了比较。在实验 1 中,我们通过 AM 范式(1a)和 CFS 范式(1b)考察了不同情绪价值的情感对目标知觉的影响。在实验 2 中,我们使用类似的方法研究了愤怒、悲伤和厌恶等情绪情感对目标感知的影响。实验 1a 和 1b 的结果一致表明,在不同的情绪情境下,对中性面孔的情绪评价存在显著差异。实验 2a 显示,在 AM 范式中,不同特定情绪情境下中性面孔的特定情绪评分存在显著差异,而在实验 2b 的 CFS 范式中则没有观察到这种差异。我们的结论是,不同情绪值的情感感受,而不是特定情绪,可以作为目标感知的固有属性被体验到,从而验证了情感现实主义假说。这些发现支持了情感表征的性质应被描述为情感维度的观点。
{"title":"Emotional contextual effects of face perception: a test of the affective realism hypothesis.","authors":"Yuan Gao, Wuji Lin, Jiaxi Liu, Yujie Chen, Chunqian Xiao, Jiexin Chen, Lei Mo","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2378326","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2024.2378326","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Affective feelings naturally infuse individuals' perceptions, serving as valid windows onto the real world. The <i>affective realism hypothesis</i> further explains how these feelings work: as properties of individuals' perceptual experiences, these feelings influence perception. Notably, this hypothesis based on affective feelings with different valences has been substantiated, whereas the existing evidence is not compelling enough. Moreover, whether specific affective feelings can be experienced as properties of target perception remains unclear. Addressing these two issues deepens our understanding of the nature of emotional representation. Hence, we investigated the affective realism hypothesis based on affective feelings with different valences and specific emotions, comparing it with the affective misattribution hypothesis. In Experiment 1, we examined the effects of affective feelings with various valences on targets' perception through the AM (1a) and CFS paradigms (1b). In Experiment 2, we investigated the effects of affective feelings with anger, sadness, and disgust using similar methods. Results from Experiments 1a and 1b consistently indicated significant differences in valence ratings of neutral faces under emotional contexts with varying valences. Experiment 2a revealed significant differences in specific emotion ratings of neutral faces under different specific emotional contexts in the AM paradigm, whereas such differences were not observed in the CFS paradigm in Experiment 2b. We concluded that affective feelings with different valences, rather than specific emotions, can be experienced as inherent properties of target perception, validating the affective realism hypothesis. These findings supported the view that the nature of emotional representation should be described as affective dimensions.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141634991","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of General Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1