首页 > 最新文献

Journal of General Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
The MoodWheel app: validation and factor structure of a new tool for the assessment of mood based on the experience sampling method. MoodWheel 应用程序:基于经验取样法的情绪评估新工具的验证和因子结构。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-19 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2378284
Oana A David, Cristina Tomoiagă, Liviu A Fodor

Background: The MoodWheel app is a newly developed tool that uses experience sampling method (ESM) for assessing negative and positive emotions, based on the circumplex model of emotions and the binary model of distress, and including behavioral and heart rate (HR) measurement via photoplethysmography and the possibility to personalize the application with additional measures. Aims: This study was designed to assess the factorial structure, reliability and validity of the MoodWheel (MW) application for evaluating emotions in children, adolescents.

Methods: A sample of 490 children and adolescents were recruited from the schools. Internal consistency was assessed via Cronbach's alpha test. Concurrent validity was assessed by evaluating the correlations between MW and Profile of Emotional Distress scale (PED) scores, in terms of functional/dysfunctional negative and positive emotions.

Results: Results obtained show that MW has good to excellent internal consistency and test-retest reliability, while the convergent validity was also adequate. Moreover, we found support for the organization of the MW based on the binary model of distress, given the predictive value found for the irrational and rational beliefs.

Conclusions: The MW application is a useful and easy to use tool that can be used for the accurate measurement of emotions, which will be complimented in future with additional behavioral parameters to provide a comprehensive and dynamic assessment.

背景介绍情绪轮(MoodWheel)应用程序是一种新开发的工具,它采用经验取样法(ESM)评估消极和积极情绪,以情绪的环状模型和二元痛苦模型为基础,包括行为和心率(HR)测量(通过光电血压计),并可通过其他测量方法对应用程序进行个性化设置。目的:本研究旨在评估用于评估儿童和青少年情绪的 MoodWheel(MW)应用程序的因子结构、可靠性和有效性:方法:从学校招募了 490 名儿童和青少年样本。通过 Cronbach's alpha 检验评估内部一致性。通过评估 MW 与情绪困扰量表(PED)分数之间在功能性/功能性消极和积极情绪方面的相关性来评估并发有效性:结果:研究结果表明,MW 具有良好至卓越的内部一致性和测试-再测可靠性,同时具有适当的收敛效度。此外,考虑到非理性和理性信念的预测价值,我们发现基于二元痛苦模型的 MW 组织方式也得到了支持:MW 应用程序是一种有用且易于使用的工具,可用于准确测量情绪,今后还将增加其他行为参数,以提供全面、动态的评估。
{"title":"The MoodWheel app: validation and factor structure of a new tool for the assessment of mood based on the experience sampling method.","authors":"Oana A David, Cristina Tomoiagă, Liviu A Fodor","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2378284","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2024.2378284","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>The MoodWheel app is a newly developed tool that uses experience sampling method (ESM) for assessing negative and positive emotions, based on the circumplex model of emotions and the binary model of distress, and including behavioral and heart rate (HR) measurement via photoplethysmography and the possibility to personalize the application with additional measures. Aims: This study was designed to assess the factorial structure, reliability and validity of the MoodWheel (MW) application for evaluating emotions in children, adolescents.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>A sample of 490 children and adolescents were recruited from the schools. Internal consistency was assessed via Cronbach's alpha test. Concurrent validity was assessed by evaluating the correlations between MW and Profile of Emotional Distress scale (PED) scores, in terms of functional/dysfunctional negative and positive emotions.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Results obtained show that MW has good to excellent internal consistency and test-retest reliability, while the convergent validity was also adequate. Moreover, we found support for the organization of the MW based on the binary model of distress, given the predictive value found for the irrational and rational beliefs.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The MW application is a useful and easy to use tool that can be used for the accurate measurement of emotions, which will be complimented in future with additional behavioral parameters to provide a comprehensive and dynamic assessment.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-17"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141724749","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotional contextual effects of face perception: a test of the affective realism hypothesis. 人脸感知的情感语境效应:对情感真实性假说的检验。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2378326
Yuan Gao, Wuji Lin, Jiaxi Liu, Yujie Chen, Chunqian Xiao, Jiexin Chen, Lei Mo

Affective feelings naturally infuse individuals' perceptions, serving as valid windows onto the real world. The affective realism hypothesis further explains how these feelings work: as properties of individuals' perceptual experiences, these feelings influence perception. Notably, this hypothesis based on affective feelings with different valences has been substantiated, whereas the existing evidence is not compelling enough. Moreover, whether specific affective feelings can be experienced as properties of target perception remains unclear. Addressing these two issues deepens our understanding of the nature of emotional representation. Hence, we investigated the affective realism hypothesis based on affective feelings with different valences and specific emotions, comparing it with the affective misattribution hypothesis. In Experiment 1, we examined the effects of affective feelings with various valences on targets' perception through the AM (1a) and CFS paradigms (1b). In Experiment 2, we investigated the effects of affective feelings with anger, sadness, and disgust using similar methods. Results from Experiments 1a and 1b consistently indicated significant differences in valence ratings of neutral faces under emotional contexts with varying valences. Experiment 2a revealed significant differences in specific emotion ratings of neutral faces under different specific emotional contexts in the AM paradigm, whereas such differences were not observed in the CFS paradigm in Experiment 2b. We concluded that affective feelings with different valences, rather than specific emotions, can be experienced as inherent properties of target perception, validating the affective realism hypothesis. These findings supported the view that the nature of emotional representation should be described as affective dimensions.

情感自然而然地渗透到个体的感知中,成为观察真实世界的有效窗口。情感现实主义假说进一步解释了这些情感是如何起作用的:作为个体知觉经验的属性,这些情感影响着知觉。值得注意的是,这一假说基于不同价值的情感感受得到了证实,而现有的证据还不够有力。此外,特定的情感感受是否能作为目标知觉的属性而被体验到,这一点仍不清楚。解决这两个问题将加深我们对情感表征本质的理解。因此,我们研究了基于不同价值和特定情绪的情感真实假说,并将其与情感错误归因假说进行了比较。在实验 1 中,我们通过 AM 范式(1a)和 CFS 范式(1b)考察了不同情绪价值的情感对目标知觉的影响。在实验 2 中,我们使用类似的方法研究了愤怒、悲伤和厌恶等情绪情感对目标感知的影响。实验 1a 和 1b 的结果一致表明,在不同的情绪情境下,对中性面孔的情绪评价存在显著差异。实验 2a 显示,在 AM 范式中,不同特定情绪情境下中性面孔的特定情绪评分存在显著差异,而在实验 2b 的 CFS 范式中则没有观察到这种差异。我们的结论是,不同情绪值的情感感受,而不是特定情绪,可以作为目标感知的固有属性被体验到,从而验证了情感现实主义假说。这些发现支持了情感表征的性质应被描述为情感维度的观点。
{"title":"Emotional contextual effects of face perception: a test of the affective realism hypothesis.","authors":"Yuan Gao, Wuji Lin, Jiaxi Liu, Yujie Chen, Chunqian Xiao, Jiexin Chen, Lei Mo","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2378326","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2024.2378326","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Affective feelings naturally infuse individuals' perceptions, serving as valid windows onto the real world. The <i>affective realism hypothesis</i> further explains how these feelings work: as properties of individuals' perceptual experiences, these feelings influence perception. Notably, this hypothesis based on affective feelings with different valences has been substantiated, whereas the existing evidence is not compelling enough. Moreover, whether specific affective feelings can be experienced as properties of target perception remains unclear. Addressing these two issues deepens our understanding of the nature of emotional representation. Hence, we investigated the affective realism hypothesis based on affective feelings with different valences and specific emotions, comparing it with the affective misattribution hypothesis. In Experiment 1, we examined the effects of affective feelings with various valences on targets' perception through the AM (1a) and CFS paradigms (1b). In Experiment 2, we investigated the effects of affective feelings with anger, sadness, and disgust using similar methods. Results from Experiments 1a and 1b consistently indicated significant differences in valence ratings of neutral faces under emotional contexts with varying valences. Experiment 2a revealed significant differences in specific emotion ratings of neutral faces under different specific emotional contexts in the AM paradigm, whereas such differences were not observed in the CFS paradigm in Experiment 2b. We concluded that affective feelings with different valences, rather than specific emotions, can be experienced as inherent properties of target perception, validating the affective realism hypothesis. These findings supported the view that the nature of emotional representation should be described as affective dimensions.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-28"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141634991","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of self-healing training on perfectionism and frustration tolerance in mothers of single-parent students. 自我修复训练对单亲学生母亲完美主义和挫折容忍度的影响。
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-30 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2023.2275305
Marzie Vahhab, Zohreh Latifi, Mehdi Marvi, Mohammad Soltanizadeh, Alexander Loyd

It is essential to focus on the variables that can empower mothers of single-parent students. The present study aimed to investigate the effects of self-healing training on perfectionism and frustration tolerance in mothers of single-parent students. The statistical population included all divorced mothers of elementary school students within the 2020-21 academic year. Sampling was performed in two stages, the first of which was multistage cluster sampling, whereas the second stage was purposive sampling based on inclusion and exclusion criteria. The study sample (n = 40) was divided into two groups of 20 (control and experimental). This quasi-experimental research adopted a pretest-posttest control group design with follow-up. The experimental group was trained in the self-healing protocol for fourteen weekly 90-minute sessions. The repeated measures ANOVA in SPSS-26 was then used for data analysis. The mean ± SD of perfectionism in the self-healing and control groups in the posttest was 67.75 ± 11.84 and 106.05 ± 22.01. Moreover, in the posttest stage, the mean ± SD of frustration tolerance in the intervention and control groups was 78.60 ± 7.93 and 99.45 ± 9.36, respectively. According to findings, self-healing training significantly affected the components of perfectionism including personal standards, concern over mistakes, and parental criticism. It also had significant effects on the components of frustration tolerance including discomfort intolerance, emotional intolerance, and achievement made by single mothers of students. The results were stable over time. However, self-healing training had no significant effects on perfectionism entitlement and subscales of frustration tolerance including discipline and organization, doubt about actions, and parental expectations.

关注能够赋予单亲学生母亲权力的变量至关重要。本研究旨在调查自我修复训练对单亲学生母亲完美主义和挫折容忍度的影响。统计人群包括2020-21学年内所有小学生的离异母亲。抽样分两个阶段进行,第一阶段是多阶段整群抽样,而第二阶段是基于纳入和排除标准的有目的抽样。研究样本(n = 40)分为两组,每组20人(对照组和实验组)。这项准实验研究采用了前测后测对照组设计,并进行了随访。实验组接受了为期14周90分钟的自我修复方案培训。然后使用SPSS-26中的重复测量ANOVA进行数据分析。平均值 ± 自我修复组和对照组在后测中完美主义的SD为67.75 ± 11.84和106.05 ± 22.01.此外,在后测阶段 ± 干预组和对照组的挫折耐受性SD为78.60 ± 7.93和99.45 ± 9.36。根据研究结果,自我修复训练显著影响完美主义的组成部分,包括个人标准、对错误的担忧和父母的批评。它还对挫折容忍的组成部分产生了重大影响,包括不适容忍、情绪容忍和学生单身母亲的成就。随着时间的推移,结果是稳定的。然而,自我修复训练对完美主义权利和挫折容忍的分量表(包括纪律和组织、对行动的怀疑和父母的期望)没有显著影响。
{"title":"Effects of self-healing training on perfectionism and frustration tolerance in mothers of single-parent students.","authors":"Marzie Vahhab, Zohreh Latifi, Mehdi Marvi, Mohammad Soltanizadeh, Alexander Loyd","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2275305","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2275305","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is essential to focus on the variables that can empower mothers of single-parent students. The present study aimed to investigate the effects of self-healing training on perfectionism and frustration tolerance in mothers of single-parent students. The statistical population included all divorced mothers of elementary school students within the 2020-21 academic year. Sampling was performed in two stages, the first of which was multistage cluster sampling, whereas the second stage was purposive sampling based on inclusion and exclusion criteria. The study sample (n = 40) was divided into two groups of 20 (control and experimental). This quasi-experimental research adopted a pretest-posttest control group design with follow-up. The experimental group was trained in the self-healing protocol for fourteen weekly 90-minute sessions. The repeated measures ANOVA in SPSS-26 was then used for data analysis. The mean ± SD of perfectionism in the self-healing and control groups in the posttest was 67.75 ± 11.84 and 106.05 ± 22.01. Moreover, in the posttest stage, the mean ± SD of frustration tolerance in the intervention and control groups was 78.60 ± 7.93 and 99.45 ± 9.36, respectively. According to findings, self-healing training significantly affected the components of perfectionism including personal standards, concern over mistakes, and parental criticism. It also had significant effects on the components of frustration tolerance including discomfort intolerance, emotional intolerance, and achievement made by single mothers of students. The results were stable over time. However, self-healing training had no significant effects on perfectionism entitlement and subscales of frustration tolerance including discipline and organization, doubt about actions, and parental expectations.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"374-389"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71414718","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Getting it right takes time: response time and performance in secondary school students. 做好这件事需要时间:中学生的反应时间和表现。
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-31 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2023.2275304
Antonio Rodán, Miriam Romero, Cristina Casadevante, José Santacreu, Pedro R Montoro, María José Contreras

The relation between response time and performance in cognitive tasks is increasingly evident. In the present study, we analyzed the effect of participants' spontaneous speed when responding to a mental rotation task. We carried out a data reanalysis from a previous study where a training of 3 practice sessions of 100 trials each was applied. The procedure was applied to a sample of 21 high school students (11 boys, 10 girls). The relation between response time and performance (hits) across the training trials was analyzed. In addition, we carried out a regression analysis of performance on the learning task as a function of response time on that same task, as well as with the score on two previously applied tests of spatial intelligence and fluid intelligence. Results showed, (a) a significant relationship (r = 0.624) between response time and hits, (b) that the group of participants with longer response times performed better; (c) that participants' response time explained most of the variance of their score on the training task in the regression analysis, although spatial and fluid intelligence scores improved the prediction of performance. Our results suggest that the reflective style achieves greater performance in solving spatial tasks, which could have important practical implications to promote a slower and more reflective style when solving school tasks with spatial components.

在认知任务中,反应时间与表现之间的关系越来越明显。在本研究中,我们分析了参与者在应对心理旋转任务时的自发速度的影响。我们对之前的一项研究进行了数据再分析,其中应用了3次练习,每次100次试验。该程序适用于21名高中生(11名男生,10名女生)的样本。分析了整个训练试验中反应时间与表现(命中率)之间的关系。此外,我们对学习任务的表现进行了回归分析,将其作为同一任务的反应时间的函数,并对之前应用的两项空间智能和流体智能测试的得分进行了回归。结果表明:(a) = 0.624),(b)反应时间较长的参与者组表现更好;(c) 在回归分析中,参与者的反应时间解释了他们在训练任务中得分的大部分差异,尽管空间和流动智力得分提高了对表现的预测。我们的研究结果表明,反思风格在解决空间任务时表现更出色,这对于在解决具有空间成分的学校任务时促进更慢、更具反思性的风格可能具有重要的现实意义。
{"title":"Getting it right takes time: response time and performance in secondary school students.","authors":"Antonio Rodán, Miriam Romero, Cristina Casadevante, José Santacreu, Pedro R Montoro, María José Contreras","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2275304","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2275304","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The relation between response time and performance in cognitive tasks is increasingly evident. In the present study, we analyzed the effect of participants' spontaneous speed when responding to a mental rotation task. We carried out a data reanalysis from a previous study where a training of 3 practice sessions of 100 trials each was applied. The procedure was applied to a sample of 21 high school students (11 boys, 10 girls). The relation between response time and performance (hits) across the training trials was analyzed. In addition, we carried out a regression analysis of performance on the learning task as a function of response time on that same task, as well as with the score on two previously applied tests of spatial intelligence and fluid intelligence. Results showed, (a) a significant relationship (<i>r</i> = 0.624) between response time and hits, (b) that the group of participants with longer response times performed better; (c) that participants' response time explained most of the variance of their score on the training task in the regression analysis, although spatial and fluid intelligence scores improved the prediction of performance. Our results suggest that the reflective style achieves greater performance in solving spatial tasks, which could have important practical implications to promote a slower and more reflective style when solving school tasks with spatial components.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"357-373"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71414719","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bidirectional relations between gratitude and depression/anxiety: based on three follow-up data. 感恩与抑郁/焦虑的双向关系:基于三个随访数据。
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-19 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2023.2275315
Zhongyuan Liang, Yanhui Xiang

In the study, 512 high school students from China were followed three times over a two-year period using a follow-up study design. Based on the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions, a cross-lagged model was developed to investigate the bidirectional relationship between gratitude and depression/anxiety. The results showed that gratitude was significantly negatively correlated with depression and anxiety. However, gratitude did not significantly negatively predict depression and anxiety in the cross-lag analysis, while depression and anxiety did significantly negatively predict gratitude. Based on the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions, this study breaks the direct promoting effect of gratitude on promoting mental health in traditional cognition, and reveals the one-way predicting relationship between depression and anxiety, two typical adverse psychological emotions, on gratitude, which has important theoretical and practical significance for understanding the development of social emotions in adolescents from the perspective of mental health.

在这项研究中,512名来自中国的高中生在两年的时间里接受了三次随访研究设计。本研究基于积极情绪的拓展-构建理论,建立了感恩与抑郁/焦虑之间的双向关系。结果表明,感恩与抑郁、焦虑呈显著负相关。然而,在交叉滞后分析中,感恩对抑郁和焦虑没有显著的负向预测,而抑郁和焦虑对感恩有显著的负向预测。本研究基于积极情绪的扩宽构建理论,打破了传统认知中感恩对心理健康的直接促进作用,揭示了抑郁和焦虑这两种典型不良心理情绪对感恩的单向预测关系,对于从心理健康的角度理解青少年社会情绪的发展具有重要的理论和现实意义。
{"title":"Bidirectional relations between gratitude and depression/anxiety: based on three follow-up data.","authors":"Zhongyuan Liang, Yanhui Xiang","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2275315","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2275315","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the study, 512 high school students from China were followed three times over a two-year period using a follow-up study design. Based on the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions, a cross-lagged model was developed to investigate the bidirectional relationship between gratitude and depression/anxiety. The results showed that gratitude was significantly negatively correlated with depression and anxiety. However, gratitude did not significantly negatively predict depression and anxiety in the cross-lag analysis, while depression and anxiety did significantly negatively predict gratitude. Based on the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions, this study breaks the direct promoting effect of gratitude on promoting mental health in traditional cognition, and reveals the one-way predicting relationship between depression and anxiety, two typical adverse psychological emotions, on gratitude, which has important theoretical and practical significance for understanding the development of social emotions in adolescents from the perspective of mental health.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"390-405"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138048192","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychological endurance: how grit, resilience, and related factors contribute to sustained effort despite adversity. 心理耐力:勇气、复原力和相关因素如何有助于在逆境中持续努力。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-12 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2023.2253955
Adam T Biggs, Todd R Seech, Scott L Johnston, Dale W Russell

Many concepts describe how individuals sustain effort despite challenging circumstances. For example, scholars and practitioners may incorporate discussions of grit, hardiness, self-control, and resilience into their ideas of performance under adversity. Although there are nuanced points underlying each construct capable of generating empirically sound propositions, the shared attributes make them difficult to differentiate. As a result, substantial confusion arises when debating how these related factors concomitantly contribute to success, especially when practitioners attempt to communicate these ideas in applied settings. The model proposed here-psychological endurance-is a unified theory to explore how multiple concepts contribute to sustained goal-directed behaviors and individual success. Central to this model is the metaphor of a psychological battery, which potentiates and sustains optimal performance despite adversity. Grit and hardiness are associated with the maximum charge of the psychological battery, or how long an individual could sustain effort. Self-control modulates energy management that augments effort required to sustain endurance, whereas resilience represents the ability to recharge. These factors are constrained by both psychological and physiological stressors in the environment that drain the psychology battery. Taken together, these ideas form a novel framework to discuss related psychological concepts, and ideally, optimize intervention to enhance psychological endurance.

许多概念描述了个人如何在具有挑战性的环境中保持努力。例如,学者和从业人员可能会将勇气、坚韧、自控力和复原力等方面的讨论纳入他们关于逆境表现的观点中。尽管每个概念背后都有一些细微差别,能够产生符合经验的命题,但它们的共同属性使其难以区分。因此,在讨论这些相关因素如何共同促进成功时,尤其是当实践者试图在应用环境中传达这些观点时,就会产生很大的困惑。本文提出的模型--心理耐力--是一个统一的理论,用于探讨多种概念如何促进持续的目标导向行为和个人成功。这一模型的核心是心理电池的隐喻,它能在逆境中激发和维持最佳表现。勇气和毅力与心理电池的最大电量有关,或者说与个人能够持续努力的时间有关。自控力能调节能量管理,从而增强维持耐力所需的努力,而恢复力则代表充电能力。这些因素都受到环境中耗尽心理电池的心理和生理压力的制约。综合来看,这些观点形成了一个新颖的框架,可用于讨论相关的心理学概念,并在理想情况下优化干预措施,以增强心理耐力。
{"title":"Psychological endurance: how grit, resilience, and related factors contribute to sustained effort despite adversity.","authors":"Adam T Biggs, Todd R Seech, Scott L Johnston, Dale W Russell","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2253955","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2253955","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many concepts describe how individuals sustain effort despite challenging circumstances. For example, scholars and practitioners may incorporate discussions of grit, hardiness, self-control, and resilience into their ideas of performance under adversity. Although there are nuanced points underlying each construct capable of generating empirically sound propositions, the shared attributes make them difficult to differentiate. As a result, substantial confusion arises when debating how these related factors concomitantly contribute to success, especially when practitioners attempt to communicate these ideas in applied settings. The model proposed here-psychological endurance-is a unified theory to explore how multiple concepts contribute to sustained goal-directed behaviors and individual success. Central to this model is the metaphor of a <i>psychological battery</i>, which potentiates and sustains optimal performance despite adversity. Grit and hardiness are associated with the maximum charge of the psychological battery, or how long an individual could sustain effort. Self-control modulates energy management that augments effort required to sustain endurance, whereas resilience represents the ability to recharge. These factors are constrained by both psychological and physiological stressors in the environment that drain the psychology battery. Taken together, these ideas form a novel framework to discuss related psychological concepts, and ideally, optimize intervention to enhance psychological endurance.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"271-313"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10212602","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social power may be associated with health through positive emotion. 社会力量可能通过积极的情绪与健康联系在一起。
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-25 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2023.2261135
Tyler N Livingston, Caroline Cummings, Jonathan Singer

Increased social power-defined as one's influence on another's behavior-guides activation of one's behavioral activation system which, in turn, elicits greater positive emotion. Positive emotion has also been linked to greater health. The current research assessed whether power and positive emotion are related to health. In Study 1, participants (N = 403; Mage = 48.33 years) wrote a narrative about a time in which they felt powerful or powerless. Greater self-reported feelings of power, concurrent with more frequent use of positive emotional words within the narrative, was associated with fewer references to health within the narrative. In Study 2, participants (N = 401; Mage = 33.05 years) primed with the concept of power (vs. powerlessness) reported greater health competency through enhanced positive emotion. Findings provided preliminary data supporting the continued study of power to better understand the link between positive emotion and health. Future research should elucidate the long-term relationships between these variables to examine whether increased power can produce downstream positive effects on health and health behavior.

社会力量的增加被定义为一个人对另一个人行为的影响,引导一个人行为激活系统的激活,进而引发更积极的情绪。积极的情绪也与更健康有关。目前的研究评估了力量和积极情绪是否与健康有关。在研究1中,参与者(N = 403;Mage=48.33 年)写了一篇关于他们感到强大或无力的时代的故事。自我报告的权力感更强,同时在叙事中更频繁地使用积极的情感词汇,与叙事中较少提及健康有关。在研究2中,参与者(N = 401;Mage=33.05 年)通过增强积极情绪,报告了更高的健康能力。研究结果为继续研究权力提供了初步数据,以更好地理解积极情绪与健康之间的联系。未来的研究应该阐明这些变量之间的长期关系,以检验功率的增加是否会对健康和健康行为产生下游的积极影响。
{"title":"Social power may be associated with health through positive emotion.","authors":"Tyler N Livingston, Caroline Cummings, Jonathan Singer","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2261135","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2261135","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Increased social power-defined as one's influence on <i>another's</i> behavior-guides activation of one's behavioral activation system which, in turn, elicits greater positive emotion. Positive emotion has also been linked to greater health. The current research assessed whether power and positive emotion are related to health. In Study 1, participants (<i>N</i> = 403; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 48.33 years) wrote a narrative about a time in which they felt powerful or powerless. Greater self-reported feelings of power, concurrent with more frequent use of positive emotional words within the narrative, was associated with fewer references to health within the narrative. In Study 2, participants (<i>N</i> = 401; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 33.05 years) primed with the concept of power (vs. powerlessness) reported greater health competency through enhanced positive emotion. Findings provided preliminary data supporting the continued study of power to better understand the link between positive emotion and health. Future research should elucidate the long-term relationships between these variables to examine whether increased power can produce downstream positive effects on health and health behavior.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"314-334"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41177270","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differentiating the DF effect in episodic memory: evaluating the contribution of the procedures of collaborative memory. 区分外显记忆中的 DF 效应:评估协作记忆程序的贡献。
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-06 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2023.2252133
Aiqing Nie, Bingyan Guo

Existing research has demonstrated a significant directed forgetting (DF) effect in memory. However, it remains unclear whether this phenomenon would occur in the context of interpersonal collaboration. Additionally, the contribution of emotional valence to the DF effect in item memory and source memory (which are subtypes of episodic memory) also needs to be explored. To address these issues, we conducted two experiments that combined the collaborative memory paradigm with the item-method procedure of DF. In both experiments, positive, neutral, or negative words were presented as stimuli, each followed by an R/F cue during encoding. We conducted two recalls, labeled Recall 1 and Recall 2, which consisted of both memory tasks. Recall 1 was performed either individually or collaboratively, whereas Recall 2 was done individually. Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 adopted the free-flowing procedure and the turn-taking procedure of collaborative memory, respectively. We obtained three implications from our current findings. (a) The occurrence of the DF effect in item memory was found regardless of the procedure of collaborative memory, and it was insensitive to the emotional valence of words or to whether participants had collaborated or not. These patterns demonstrate that both the mechanisms of elaborative rehearsal and active suppression/encoding blocking were engaged across words of different emotional valences and in nominal and collaborative circumstances. (b) In source memory, the DF effect showed different patterns in ongoing and post-collaborative memory, which underpins the dual-process models. (c) The amplitude of the DF effect was sensitive to the interaction of emotional valence by the status of collaboration, and the impact of collaboration differed between the two experiments, offering telling evidence of different aspects of the retrieval strategy disruption hypothesis (RSDH). Directions for identifying more influential factors are put forward.

现有研究表明,记忆中存在显著的定向遗忘(DF)效应。然而,这种现象是否会在人际协作中出现仍不清楚。此外,情绪情感对项目记忆和源记忆(属于外显记忆的子类型)中定向遗忘效应的贡献也有待探讨。为了解决这些问题,我们进行了两项实验,将协作记忆范式与 DF 的项目-方法程序相结合。在这两个实验中,正面、中性或负面词语作为刺激出现,在编码过程中,每个词语后面都有一个R/F提示。我们进行了两次回忆,分别称为 "回忆 1 "和 "回忆 2",其中包括两项记忆任务。回忆 1 可以单独完成,也可以合作完成,而回忆 2 则单独完成。实验 1 和实验 2 分别采用了协作记忆的自由流动程序和轮流记忆程序。我们从目前的研究结果中获得了三点启示。(a) 无论协作记忆的程序如何,在项目记忆中都会出现 DF 效应,而且这种效应对词语的情绪价值或参与者是否协作并不敏感。这些模式表明,精心排练和主动抑制/编码阻断这两种机制在不同情绪价值的词语中、在名义和合作的情况下都会发生作用。(b) 在源记忆中,DF 效应在进行中记忆和合作后记忆中表现出不同的模式,这也是双过程模型的基础。(c) DF 效应的幅度对协作状态与情绪情感的交互作用很敏感,而且协作的影响在两个实验中有所不同,这为检索策略中断假说(RSDH)的不同方面提供了有力的证据。我们还提出了确定更多影响因素的方向。
{"title":"Differentiating the DF effect in episodic memory: evaluating the contribution of the procedures of collaborative memory.","authors":"Aiqing Nie, Bingyan Guo","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2252133","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2252133","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Existing research has demonstrated a significant directed forgetting (DF) effect in memory. However, it remains unclear whether this phenomenon would occur in the context of interpersonal collaboration. Additionally, the contribution of emotional valence to the DF effect in item memory and source memory (which are subtypes of episodic memory) also needs to be explored. To address these issues, we conducted two experiments that combined the collaborative memory paradigm with the item-method procedure of DF. In both experiments, positive, neutral, or negative words were presented as stimuli, each followed by an R/F cue during encoding. We conducted two recalls, labeled Recall 1 and Recall 2, which consisted of both memory tasks. Recall 1 was performed either individually or collaboratively, whereas Recall 2 was done individually. Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 adopted the free-flowing procedure and the turn-taking procedure of collaborative memory, respectively. We obtained three implications from our current findings. (a) The occurrence of the DF effect in item memory was found regardless of the procedure of collaborative memory, and it was insensitive to the emotional valence of words or to whether participants had collaborated or not. These patterns demonstrate that both the mechanisms of elaborative rehearsal and active suppression/encoding blocking were engaged across words of different emotional valences and in nominal and collaborative circumstances. (b) In source memory, the DF effect showed different patterns in ongoing and post-collaborative memory, which underpins the dual-process models. (c) The amplitude of the DF effect was sensitive to the interaction of emotional valence by the status of collaboration, and the impact of collaboration differed between the two experiments, offering telling evidence of different aspects of the retrieval strategy disruption hypothesis (RSDH). Directions for identifying more influential factors are put forward.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"223-270"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10218367","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is it time to replace the Big Five personality model? Factorial structure of the NEO PI-R in a community sample of Spanish adults. 是时候取代五大人格模式了吗?西班牙成年人社区样本中近地天体PI-R的因子结构。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-26 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2023.2261136
Ana Sanz-García, María Paz García-Vera, Jesús Sanz

Recent studies have revived the issue of whether the five-factor personality model or Big Five is the most valid to summarize the most relevant personality traits or whether, on the contrary, the basic structure of personality traits would better fit a six-factor model such as the HEXACO model: Honesty-Humility (H), Emotionality (E), Extraversion (X), Agreeableness (A), Conscientiousness (C), and Openness to Experience (O). In a Spanish community sample of 682 adults, the factorial structure of the 30 facets of the NEO-Revised Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and its 16 facets common to the HEXACO model was analyzed. In two subsamples of participants, the internal structure of the NEO PI-R, of 30 and 16 facets, fit the five-factor Big Five model better than the six-factor HEXACO model. In addition, the internal 30-facet structure of the NEO-PI-R replicated that obtained in the original US validation and those previously obtained in Spain, although the latter used different participant samples (people evaluated in personnel selection processes, university students). These results suggest that, at least in Spain, the five-factor personality model or Big Five is still the most valid taxonomy of personality traits.

最近的研究重新提出了一个问题,即五因素人格模型或五大人格模型是否是总结最相关人格特征的最有效模型,或者相反,人格特征的基本结构是否更适合六因素模型,如HEXACO模型:诚实-谦逊(H)、情感(E)、外向(X)、随和(a)、认真(C),以及对经验的开放(O)。在西班牙社区682名成年人的样本中,分析了NEO修订人格量表(NEO PI-R)的30个方面及其HEXACO模型共有的16个方面的因子结构。在参与者的两个子样本中,近地天体PI-R的内部结构,包括30个和16个方面,比六因子HEXACO模型更适合五因子大五模型。此外,NEO-PI-R的内部30个方面的结构复制了最初在美国验证中获得的结构和之前在西班牙获得的结构,尽管后者使用了不同的参与者样本(在人员选拔过程中评估的人、大学生)。这些结果表明,至少在西班牙,五因素人格模型或五大人格模型仍然是最有效的人格特征分类法。
{"title":"Is it time to replace the Big Five personality model? Factorial structure of the NEO PI-R in a community sample of Spanish adults.","authors":"Ana Sanz-García, María Paz García-Vera, Jesús Sanz","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2261136","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2261136","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent studies have revived the issue of whether the five-factor personality model or Big Five is the most valid to summarize the most relevant personality traits or whether, on the contrary, the basic structure of personality traits would better fit a six-factor model such as the HEXACO model: Honesty-Humility (H), Emotionality (E), Extraversion (X), Agreeableness (A), Conscientiousness (C), and Openness to Experience (O). In a Spanish community sample of 682 adults, the factorial structure of the 30 facets of the NEO-Revised Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and its 16 facets common to the HEXACO model was analyzed. In two subsamples of participants, the internal structure of the NEO PI-R, of 30 and 16 facets, fit the five-factor Big Five model better than the six-factor HEXACO model. In addition, the internal 30-facet structure of the NEO-PI-R replicated that obtained in the original US validation and those previously obtained in Spain, although the latter used different participant samples (people evaluated in personnel selection processes, university students). These results suggest that, at least in Spain, the five-factor personality model or Big Five is still the most valid taxonomy of personality traits.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"335-356"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41137790","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The association of internet searches and actual suicide in Spain. 互联网搜索与西班牙实际自杀的关联。
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-30 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2023.2276803
Luis Alejandro Lopez-Agudo, Oscar David Marcenaro-Gutierrez

High suicide rates are a major issue in Spain, to the extent that they are the main non-natural cause of death in this country. The present study analyses the relationship between Internet searches and actual suicide rates in Spain. For this purpose, we employ data from actual suicide rates and Google® searches for Spain, differencing by the means used to commit suicide. Our results show that suicide ("suicidio") search term seems to be positively associated with higher total suicide rates, in addition to suicides using poison, suffocation and jumping. The suicide ("suicidio") topic presents similar results to the suicide search term, and suicide by car crash also seems to be associated with Internet searches of this topic.

高自杀率是西班牙的一个主要问题,因为自杀率是该国主要的非自然死亡原因。本研究分析了互联网搜索与西班牙实际自杀率之间的关系。为此,我们使用了来自西班牙实际自杀率和谷歌®搜索的数据,通过自杀手段进行区分。我们的研究结果表明,除了使用毒药、窒息和跳跃自杀外,自杀(“suicidio”)搜索词似乎与更高的总自杀率呈正相关。自杀(“suicidio”)主题呈现出与自杀搜索词相似的结果,车祸自杀似乎也与该主题的互联网搜索有关。
{"title":"The association of internet searches and actual suicide in Spain.","authors":"Luis Alejandro Lopez-Agudo, Oscar David Marcenaro-Gutierrez","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2276803","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2276803","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>High suicide rates are a major issue in Spain, to the extent that they are the main non-natural cause of death in this country. The present study analyses the relationship between Internet searches and actual suicide rates in Spain. For this purpose, we employ data from actual suicide rates and Google<sup>®</sup> searches for Spain, differencing by the means used to commit suicide. Our results show that suicide (\"<i>suicidio</i>\") search term seems to be positively associated with higher total suicide rates, in addition to suicides using poison, suffocation and jumping. The suicide (\"<i>suicidio</i>\") topic presents similar results to the suicide search term, and suicide by car crash also seems to be associated with Internet searches of this topic.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"406-424"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71414720","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of General Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1