首页 > 最新文献

Journal of General Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Psychological endurance: how grit, resilience, and related factors contribute to sustained effort despite adversity. 心理耐力:勇气、复原力和相关因素如何有助于在逆境中持续努力。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-12 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2023.2253955
Adam T Biggs, Todd R Seech, Scott L Johnston, Dale W Russell

Many concepts describe how individuals sustain effort despite challenging circumstances. For example, scholars and practitioners may incorporate discussions of grit, hardiness, self-control, and resilience into their ideas of performance under adversity. Although there are nuanced points underlying each construct capable of generating empirically sound propositions, the shared attributes make them difficult to differentiate. As a result, substantial confusion arises when debating how these related factors concomitantly contribute to success, especially when practitioners attempt to communicate these ideas in applied settings. The model proposed here-psychological endurance-is a unified theory to explore how multiple concepts contribute to sustained goal-directed behaviors and individual success. Central to this model is the metaphor of a psychological battery, which potentiates and sustains optimal performance despite adversity. Grit and hardiness are associated with the maximum charge of the psychological battery, or how long an individual could sustain effort. Self-control modulates energy management that augments effort required to sustain endurance, whereas resilience represents the ability to recharge. These factors are constrained by both psychological and physiological stressors in the environment that drain the psychology battery. Taken together, these ideas form a novel framework to discuss related psychological concepts, and ideally, optimize intervention to enhance psychological endurance.

许多概念描述了个人如何在具有挑战性的环境中保持努力。例如,学者和从业人员可能会将勇气、坚韧、自控力和复原力等方面的讨论纳入他们关于逆境表现的观点中。尽管每个概念背后都有一些细微差别,能够产生符合经验的命题,但它们的共同属性使其难以区分。因此,在讨论这些相关因素如何共同促进成功时,尤其是当实践者试图在应用环境中传达这些观点时,就会产生很大的困惑。本文提出的模型--心理耐力--是一个统一的理论,用于探讨多种概念如何促进持续的目标导向行为和个人成功。这一模型的核心是心理电池的隐喻,它能在逆境中激发和维持最佳表现。勇气和毅力与心理电池的最大电量有关,或者说与个人能够持续努力的时间有关。自控力能调节能量管理,从而增强维持耐力所需的努力,而恢复力则代表充电能力。这些因素都受到环境中耗尽心理电池的心理和生理压力的制约。综合来看,这些观点形成了一个新颖的框架,可用于讨论相关的心理学概念,并在理想情况下优化干预措施,以增强心理耐力。
{"title":"Psychological endurance: how grit, resilience, and related factors contribute to sustained effort despite adversity.","authors":"Adam T Biggs, Todd R Seech, Scott L Johnston, Dale W Russell","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2253955","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2253955","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many concepts describe how individuals sustain effort despite challenging circumstances. For example, scholars and practitioners may incorporate discussions of grit, hardiness, self-control, and resilience into their ideas of performance under adversity. Although there are nuanced points underlying each construct capable of generating empirically sound propositions, the shared attributes make them difficult to differentiate. As a result, substantial confusion arises when debating how these related factors concomitantly contribute to success, especially when practitioners attempt to communicate these ideas in applied settings. The model proposed here-psychological endurance-is a unified theory to explore how multiple concepts contribute to sustained goal-directed behaviors and individual success. Central to this model is the metaphor of a <i>psychological battery</i>, which potentiates and sustains optimal performance despite adversity. Grit and hardiness are associated with the maximum charge of the psychological battery, or how long an individual could sustain effort. Self-control modulates energy management that augments effort required to sustain endurance, whereas resilience represents the ability to recharge. These factors are constrained by both psychological and physiological stressors in the environment that drain the psychology battery. Taken together, these ideas form a novel framework to discuss related psychological concepts, and ideally, optimize intervention to enhance psychological endurance.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10212602","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social power may be associated with health through positive emotion. 社会力量可能通过积极的情绪与健康联系在一起。
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-25 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2023.2261135
Tyler N Livingston, Caroline Cummings, Jonathan Singer

Increased social power-defined as one's influence on another's behavior-guides activation of one's behavioral activation system which, in turn, elicits greater positive emotion. Positive emotion has also been linked to greater health. The current research assessed whether power and positive emotion are related to health. In Study 1, participants (N = 403; Mage = 48.33 years) wrote a narrative about a time in which they felt powerful or powerless. Greater self-reported feelings of power, concurrent with more frequent use of positive emotional words within the narrative, was associated with fewer references to health within the narrative. In Study 2, participants (N = 401; Mage = 33.05 years) primed with the concept of power (vs. powerlessness) reported greater health competency through enhanced positive emotion. Findings provided preliminary data supporting the continued study of power to better understand the link between positive emotion and health. Future research should elucidate the long-term relationships between these variables to examine whether increased power can produce downstream positive effects on health and health behavior.

社会力量的增加被定义为一个人对另一个人行为的影响,引导一个人行为激活系统的激活,进而引发更积极的情绪。积极的情绪也与更健康有关。目前的研究评估了力量和积极情绪是否与健康有关。在研究1中,参与者(N = 403;Mage=48.33 年)写了一篇关于他们感到强大或无力的时代的故事。自我报告的权力感更强,同时在叙事中更频繁地使用积极的情感词汇,与叙事中较少提及健康有关。在研究2中,参与者(N = 401;Mage=33.05 年)通过增强积极情绪,报告了更高的健康能力。研究结果为继续研究权力提供了初步数据,以更好地理解积极情绪与健康之间的联系。未来的研究应该阐明这些变量之间的长期关系,以检验功率的增加是否会对健康和健康行为产生下游的积极影响。
{"title":"Social power may be associated with health through positive emotion.","authors":"Tyler N Livingston, Caroline Cummings, Jonathan Singer","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2261135","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2261135","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Increased social power-defined as one's influence on <i>another's</i> behavior-guides activation of one's behavioral activation system which, in turn, elicits greater positive emotion. Positive emotion has also been linked to greater health. The current research assessed whether power and positive emotion are related to health. In Study 1, participants (<i>N</i> = 403; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 48.33 years) wrote a narrative about a time in which they felt powerful or powerless. Greater self-reported feelings of power, concurrent with more frequent use of positive emotional words within the narrative, was associated with fewer references to health within the narrative. In Study 2, participants (<i>N</i> = 401; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 33.05 years) primed with the concept of power (vs. powerlessness) reported greater health competency through enhanced positive emotion. Findings provided preliminary data supporting the continued study of power to better understand the link between positive emotion and health. Future research should elucidate the long-term relationships between these variables to examine whether increased power can produce downstream positive effects on health and health behavior.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41177270","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differentiating the DF effect in episodic memory: evaluating the contribution of the procedures of collaborative memory. 区分外显记忆中的 DF 效应:评估协作记忆程序的贡献。
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-06 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2023.2252133
Aiqing Nie, Bingyan Guo

Existing research has demonstrated a significant directed forgetting (DF) effect in memory. However, it remains unclear whether this phenomenon would occur in the context of interpersonal collaboration. Additionally, the contribution of emotional valence to the DF effect in item memory and source memory (which are subtypes of episodic memory) also needs to be explored. To address these issues, we conducted two experiments that combined the collaborative memory paradigm with the item-method procedure of DF. In both experiments, positive, neutral, or negative words were presented as stimuli, each followed by an R/F cue during encoding. We conducted two recalls, labeled Recall 1 and Recall 2, which consisted of both memory tasks. Recall 1 was performed either individually or collaboratively, whereas Recall 2 was done individually. Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 adopted the free-flowing procedure and the turn-taking procedure of collaborative memory, respectively. We obtained three implications from our current findings. (a) The occurrence of the DF effect in item memory was found regardless of the procedure of collaborative memory, and it was insensitive to the emotional valence of words or to whether participants had collaborated or not. These patterns demonstrate that both the mechanisms of elaborative rehearsal and active suppression/encoding blocking were engaged across words of different emotional valences and in nominal and collaborative circumstances. (b) In source memory, the DF effect showed different patterns in ongoing and post-collaborative memory, which underpins the dual-process models. (c) The amplitude of the DF effect was sensitive to the interaction of emotional valence by the status of collaboration, and the impact of collaboration differed between the two experiments, offering telling evidence of different aspects of the retrieval strategy disruption hypothesis (RSDH). Directions for identifying more influential factors are put forward.

现有研究表明,记忆中存在显著的定向遗忘(DF)效应。然而,这种现象是否会在人际协作中出现仍不清楚。此外,情绪情感对项目记忆和源记忆(属于外显记忆的子类型)中定向遗忘效应的贡献也有待探讨。为了解决这些问题,我们进行了两项实验,将协作记忆范式与 DF 的项目-方法程序相结合。在这两个实验中,正面、中性或负面词语作为刺激出现,在编码过程中,每个词语后面都有一个R/F提示。我们进行了两次回忆,分别称为 "回忆 1 "和 "回忆 2",其中包括两项记忆任务。回忆 1 可以单独完成,也可以合作完成,而回忆 2 则单独完成。实验 1 和实验 2 分别采用了协作记忆的自由流动程序和轮流记忆程序。我们从目前的研究结果中获得了三点启示。(a) 无论协作记忆的程序如何,在项目记忆中都会出现 DF 效应,而且这种效应对词语的情绪价值或参与者是否协作并不敏感。这些模式表明,精心排练和主动抑制/编码阻断这两种机制在不同情绪价值的词语中、在名义和合作的情况下都会发生作用。(b) 在源记忆中,DF 效应在进行中记忆和合作后记忆中表现出不同的模式,这也是双过程模型的基础。(c) DF 效应的幅度对协作状态与情绪情感的交互作用很敏感,而且协作的影响在两个实验中有所不同,这为检索策略中断假说(RSDH)的不同方面提供了有力的证据。我们还提出了确定更多影响因素的方向。
{"title":"Differentiating the DF effect in episodic memory: evaluating the contribution of the procedures of collaborative memory.","authors":"Aiqing Nie, Bingyan Guo","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2252133","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2252133","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Existing research has demonstrated a significant directed forgetting (DF) effect in memory. However, it remains unclear whether this phenomenon would occur in the context of interpersonal collaboration. Additionally, the contribution of emotional valence to the DF effect in item memory and source memory (which are subtypes of episodic memory) also needs to be explored. To address these issues, we conducted two experiments that combined the collaborative memory paradigm with the item-method procedure of DF. In both experiments, positive, neutral, or negative words were presented as stimuli, each followed by an R/F cue during encoding. We conducted two recalls, labeled Recall 1 and Recall 2, which consisted of both memory tasks. Recall 1 was performed either individually or collaboratively, whereas Recall 2 was done individually. Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 adopted the free-flowing procedure and the turn-taking procedure of collaborative memory, respectively. We obtained three implications from our current findings. (a) The occurrence of the DF effect in item memory was found regardless of the procedure of collaborative memory, and it was insensitive to the emotional valence of words or to whether participants had collaborated or not. These patterns demonstrate that both the mechanisms of elaborative rehearsal and active suppression/encoding blocking were engaged across words of different emotional valences and in nominal and collaborative circumstances. (b) In source memory, the DF effect showed different patterns in ongoing and post-collaborative memory, which underpins the dual-process models. (c) The amplitude of the DF effect was sensitive to the interaction of emotional valence by the status of collaboration, and the impact of collaboration differed between the two experiments, offering telling evidence of different aspects of the retrieval strategy disruption hypothesis (RSDH). Directions for identifying more influential factors are put forward.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10218367","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is it time to replace the Big Five personality model? Factorial structure of the NEO PI-R in a community sample of Spanish adults. 是时候取代五大人格模式了吗?西班牙成年人社区样本中近地天体PI-R的因子结构。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-26 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2023.2261136
Ana Sanz-García, María Paz García-Vera, Jesús Sanz

Recent studies have revived the issue of whether the five-factor personality model or Big Five is the most valid to summarize the most relevant personality traits or whether, on the contrary, the basic structure of personality traits would better fit a six-factor model such as the HEXACO model: Honesty-Humility (H), Emotionality (E), Extraversion (X), Agreeableness (A), Conscientiousness (C), and Openness to Experience (O). In a Spanish community sample of 682 adults, the factorial structure of the 30 facets of the NEO-Revised Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and its 16 facets common to the HEXACO model was analyzed. In two subsamples of participants, the internal structure of the NEO PI-R, of 30 and 16 facets, fit the five-factor Big Five model better than the six-factor HEXACO model. In addition, the internal 30-facet structure of the NEO-PI-R replicated that obtained in the original US validation and those previously obtained in Spain, although the latter used different participant samples (people evaluated in personnel selection processes, university students). These results suggest that, at least in Spain, the five-factor personality model or Big Five is still the most valid taxonomy of personality traits.

最近的研究重新提出了一个问题,即五因素人格模型或五大人格模型是否是总结最相关人格特征的最有效模型,或者相反,人格特征的基本结构是否更适合六因素模型,如HEXACO模型:诚实-谦逊(H)、情感(E)、外向(X)、随和(a)、认真(C),以及对经验的开放(O)。在西班牙社区682名成年人的样本中,分析了NEO修订人格量表(NEO PI-R)的30个方面及其HEXACO模型共有的16个方面的因子结构。在参与者的两个子样本中,近地天体PI-R的内部结构,包括30个和16个方面,比六因子HEXACO模型更适合五因子大五模型。此外,NEO-PI-R的内部30个方面的结构复制了最初在美国验证中获得的结构和之前在西班牙获得的结构,尽管后者使用了不同的参与者样本(在人员选拔过程中评估的人、大学生)。这些结果表明,至少在西班牙,五因素人格模型或五大人格模型仍然是最有效的人格特征分类法。
{"title":"Is it time to replace the Big Five personality model? Factorial structure of the NEO PI-R in a community sample of Spanish adults.","authors":"Ana Sanz-García, María Paz García-Vera, Jesús Sanz","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2261136","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2261136","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent studies have revived the issue of whether the five-factor personality model or Big Five is the most valid to summarize the most relevant personality traits or whether, on the contrary, the basic structure of personality traits would better fit a six-factor model such as the HEXACO model: Honesty-Humility (H), Emotionality (E), Extraversion (X), Agreeableness (A), Conscientiousness (C), and Openness to Experience (O). In a Spanish community sample of 682 adults, the factorial structure of the 30 facets of the NEO-Revised Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and its 16 facets common to the HEXACO model was analyzed. In two subsamples of participants, the internal structure of the NEO PI-R, of 30 and 16 facets, fit the five-factor Big Five model better than the six-factor HEXACO model. In addition, the internal 30-facet structure of the NEO-PI-R replicated that obtained in the original US validation and those previously obtained in Spain, although the latter used different participant samples (people evaluated in personnel selection processes, university students). These results suggest that, at least in Spain, the five-factor personality model or Big Five is still the most valid taxonomy of personality traits.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41137790","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The association of internet searches and actual suicide in Spain. 互联网搜索与西班牙实际自杀的关联。
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-30 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2023.2276803
Luis Alejandro Lopez-Agudo, Oscar David Marcenaro-Gutierrez

High suicide rates are a major issue in Spain, to the extent that they are the main non-natural cause of death in this country. The present study analyses the relationship between Internet searches and actual suicide rates in Spain. For this purpose, we employ data from actual suicide rates and Google® searches for Spain, differencing by the means used to commit suicide. Our results show that suicide ("suicidio") search term seems to be positively associated with higher total suicide rates, in addition to suicides using poison, suffocation and jumping. The suicide ("suicidio") topic presents similar results to the suicide search term, and suicide by car crash also seems to be associated with Internet searches of this topic.

高自杀率是西班牙的一个主要问题,因为自杀率是该国主要的非自然死亡原因。本研究分析了互联网搜索与西班牙实际自杀率之间的关系。为此,我们使用了来自西班牙实际自杀率和谷歌®搜索的数据,通过自杀手段进行区分。我们的研究结果表明,除了使用毒药、窒息和跳跃自杀外,自杀(“suicidio”)搜索词似乎与更高的总自杀率呈正相关。自杀(“suicidio”)主题呈现出与自杀搜索词相似的结果,车祸自杀似乎也与该主题的互联网搜索有关。
{"title":"The association of internet searches and actual suicide in Spain.","authors":"Luis Alejandro Lopez-Agudo, Oscar David Marcenaro-Gutierrez","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2276803","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00221309.2023.2276803","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>High suicide rates are a major issue in Spain, to the extent that they are the main non-natural cause of death in this country. The present study analyses the relationship between Internet searches and actual suicide rates in Spain. For this purpose, we employ data from actual suicide rates and Google<sup>®</sup> searches for Spain, differencing by the means used to commit suicide. Our results show that suicide (\"<i>suicidio</i>\") search term seems to be positively associated with higher total suicide rates, in addition to suicides using poison, suffocation and jumping. The suicide (\"<i>suicidio</i>\") topic presents similar results to the suicide search term, and suicide by car crash also seems to be associated with Internet searches of this topic.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71414720","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining gullibility with sentence verification judgments. 用句子验证判断来检验轻信。
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-08 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2360401
Yasuhiro Ozuru, Masoumeh Heidari

Three experiments were conducted to examine gullibility as measured by people's bias to respond with a True response when performing sentence verification judgment task. The experiments manipulated the location of unfamiliar concepts such that some sentences contained unfamiliar concepts in the subject while other sentences contained unfamiliar concepts in the predicate, hence measuring the bias to judge an idea to be true when one cannot make the decision relying on background knowledge. The results indicated: 1) a higher frequency of True response when an unfamiliar concept is located in the subject compared to when it is in the predicate; and 2) the frequency of True response was lower than chance level even when unfamiliar information is located in the subject. The results were discussed in relation to gullibility and how the verification judgment is processed as a plausibility judgment.

我们进行了三项实验来研究人们在完成句子验证判断任务时的 "轻信 "倾向。这些实验操纵了陌生概念的位置,使一些句子的主语中包含陌生概念,而另一些句子的谓语中包含陌生概念,从而测量了当一个人无法依靠背景知识做出判断时,判断一个观点为真的偏差。结果表明1)与谓语中的陌生概念相比,当陌生概念位于主语中时,"真 "的反应频率更高;2)即使陌生信息位于主语中,"真 "的反应频率也低于偶然水平。这些结果与轻信以及如何将验证判断作为可信度判断来处理进行了讨论。
{"title":"Examining gullibility with sentence verification judgments.","authors":"Yasuhiro Ozuru, Masoumeh Heidari","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2360401","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2024.2360401","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Three experiments were conducted to examine gullibility as measured by people's bias to respond with a True response when performing sentence verification judgment task. The experiments manipulated the location of unfamiliar concepts such that some sentences contained unfamiliar concepts in the subject while other sentences contained unfamiliar concepts in the predicate, hence measuring the bias to judge an idea to be true when one cannot make the decision relying on background knowledge. The results indicated: 1) a higher frequency of True response when an unfamiliar concept is located in the subject compared to when it is in the predicate; and 2) the frequency of True response was lower than chance level even when unfamiliar information is located in the subject. The results were discussed in relation to gullibility and how the verification judgment is processed as a plausibility judgment.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-06-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141288775","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No evidence for the influence of head-heart conceptual metaphor on moral decision making and personality. 没有证据表明头心概念隐喻对道德决策和人格有影响。
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-05 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2360405
Yanyun Zhou, Chi-Shing Tse

In English, head is associated with rationality and logic, whereas heart is related to feeling and emotionality. In Chinese, these head- and heart-related metaphors also exist. Could these head- and heart-related conceptual metaphors influence people's moral decision-making and personality? This seems so based on the previous findings that (a) simply pointing an index finger to heart (versus head) position caused participants to produce more emotional responses in a moral decision task, and (b) participants who believed themselves to be heart locators, relative to those who regarded themselves as head locators, scored higher in affect intensity, femininity, and intimacy related activities. The current study attempted to replicate these findings, following the same design and procedure of previous work, with Chinese participants from Hong Kong and Chinese mainland. In Experiments 1a and 1b, 203 participants performed the moral decision task on dilemmas with their index fingers pointing to head or heart. In Experiments 2a and 2b, 304 participants completed the scales of self-location, affective intensity, femininity, and intimacy related activities. In these high-powered experiments, we failed to replicate the findings of previous work. Bayesian analyses further showed that no head- and heart-related conceptual metaphor effect was likely to occur. Potential reasons for our inconsistent results with those of previous studies and the implications of our current findings were discussed.

在英语中,head 与理性和逻辑有关,而 heart 则与感觉和情感有关。在中文中,也存在这些与头和心相关的隐喻。这些与头和心相关的概念隐喻是否会影响人们的道德决策和人格?根据之前的研究结果,似乎是这样的:(a)在道德决策任务中,简单地将食指指向心脏(相对于头部)位置会使参与者产生更多的情绪反应;(b)认为自己是心脏定位者的参与者,相对于认为自己是头部定位者的参与者,在情感强度、女性特质和亲密关系相关活动中得分更高。本研究试图在香港和中国大陆的中国被试身上复制这些发现,实验设计和程序与之前的研究相同。在实验 1a 和 1b 中,203 名参与者在进行道德决策任务时,用食指指向头部或心脏。在实验 2a 和 2b 中,304 名参与者完成了自我定位、情感强度、女性特质和亲密关系相关活动的量表。在这些高功率实验中,我们未能重复之前的研究结果。贝叶斯分析进一步表明,与头部和心脏相关的概念隐喻效应不可能发生。我们讨论了与之前研究结果不一致的潜在原因,以及目前研究结果的意义。
{"title":"No evidence for the influence of head-heart conceptual metaphor on moral decision making and personality.","authors":"Yanyun Zhou, Chi-Shing Tse","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2360405","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2024.2360405","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In English, head is associated with rationality and logic, whereas heart is related to feeling and emotionality. In Chinese, these head- and heart-related metaphors also exist. Could these head- and heart-related conceptual metaphors influence people's moral decision-making and personality? This seems so based on the previous findings that (a) simply pointing an index finger to heart (versus head) position caused participants to produce more emotional responses in a moral decision task, and (b) participants who believed themselves to be heart locators, relative to those who regarded themselves as head locators, scored higher in affect intensity, femininity, and intimacy related activities. The current study attempted to replicate these findings, following the same design and procedure of previous work, with Chinese participants from Hong Kong and Chinese mainland. In Experiments 1a and 1b, 203 participants performed the moral decision task on dilemmas with their index fingers pointing to head or heart. In Experiments 2a and 2b, 304 participants completed the scales of self-location, affective intensity, femininity, and intimacy related activities. In these high-powered experiments, we failed to replicate the findings of previous work. Bayesian analyses further showed that no head- and heart-related conceptual metaphor effect was likely to occur. Potential reasons for our inconsistent results with those of previous studies and the implications of our current findings were discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-06-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141263031","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Anxiety on the internet: Describing person, provider, and organization online posts. 互联网上的焦虑:描述个人、提供者和组织的网上帖子。
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-27 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2349765
Kara Kaelber, Lauren S Seifert, Anh Thu Huynh Nguyen, Katelyn McWhirter

Anxiety is a pervasive phenomenon in contemporary society. With increased internet use in recent years, more people in the general population are seeking and providing help and participating in community online. The goal of our study was to evaluate the content of internet narratives among those who post about anxiety and determine what stakeholder groups are saying online. We used the bifurcated method; it is a multi-method (qualitative) approach with inductive, thematic analyses, and with quantification of content-related words via a computer program that crawls websites and counts the occurrences of specified terms (for cross-checking purposes). Themes of posts and webpages about anxiety were: using/reporting treatment strategies (83.3% saturation), providing help (77.8% saturation), telling personal stories (72.2% saturation), seeking help (61.1% saturation), and illustrating interpersonal impact (50% saturation). We argue that anxiety stakeholders may take part in health co-inquiry online (i.e., cooperating with others) in many of the same ways that they might collaborate in person. We recommend that clinicians query their clients about use of the internet in ways related to their anxiety (e.g., seeking information/treatment strategies, offering help to others, telling their personal stories, etc.) so that they might help them process what they experience online.

焦虑是当代社会的一种普遍现象。近年来,随着互联网使用率的提高,越来越多的普通人开始在网上寻求帮助、提供帮助和参与社区活动。我们的研究目的是评估那些发布焦虑信息的人在网络上的叙述内容,并确定利益相关群体在网上都说了些什么。我们采用了分叉法;这是一种多方法(定性)方法,包括归纳式主题分析,以及通过计算机程序对内容相关词汇进行量化,该程序会抓取网站并统计特定词汇的出现次数(用于交叉检查)。有关焦虑的帖子和网页的主题是:使用/报告治疗策略(83.3%的饱和度)、提供帮助(77.8%的饱和度)、讲述个人故事(72.2%的饱和度)、寻求帮助(61.1%的饱和度)和说明人际影响(50%的饱和度)。我们认为,焦虑的利益相关者可以通过许多与面对面合作相同的方式参与在线健康共同查询(即与他人合作)。我们建议临床医生询问他们的客户是否以与焦虑相关的方式使用互联网(例如,寻求信息/治疗策略、向他人提供帮助、讲述个人经历等),以便帮助他们处理他们在网上的经历。
{"title":"Anxiety on the internet: Describing person, provider, and organization online posts.","authors":"Kara Kaelber, Lauren S Seifert, Anh Thu Huynh Nguyen, Katelyn McWhirter","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2349765","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2024.2349765","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Anxiety is a pervasive phenomenon in contemporary society. With increased internet use in recent years, more people in the general population are seeking and providing help and participating in community online. The goal of our study was to evaluate the content of internet narratives among those who post about anxiety and determine what stakeholder groups are saying online. We used the bifurcated method; it is a multi-method (qualitative) approach with inductive, thematic analyses, and with quantification of content-related words via a computer program that crawls websites and counts the occurrences of specified terms (for cross-checking purposes). Themes of posts and webpages about anxiety were: using/reporting treatment strategies (83.3% saturation), providing help (77.8% saturation), telling personal stories (72.2% saturation), seeking help (61.1% saturation), and illustrating interpersonal impact (50% saturation). We argue that anxiety stakeholders may take part in health co-inquiry online (i.e., cooperating with others) in many of the same ways that they might collaborate in person. We recommend that clinicians query their clients about use of the internet in ways related to their anxiety (e.g., seeking information/treatment strategies, offering help to others, telling their personal stories, etc.) so that they might help them process what they experience online.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141155765","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The distinct effects of fearful and disgusting scenes on self-relevant face recognition. 恐惧和恶心场景对自我相关人脸识别的不同影响
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-20 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2349764
Yuan Yuan, Lili Guan, Yifei Cao, Yang Xu

Self-face recognition denotes the process by which a person can recognize their own face by distinguishing it from another's face. Although many research studies have explored the inhibition effect of negative information on self-relevant face processing, few researchers have examined whether negative scenes influence self-relevant face processing. Fearful and disgusting scenes are typical negative scenes, but little research to data has examined their discriminative effects on self-relevant face recognition. To investigate these issues, the current study explored the effect of negative scenes on self-relevant face recognition. In Study 1, 44 participants (20 men, 24 women) were asked to judge the orientation of a target face (self-face or friend-face) pictured in a negative or neutral scene, whereas 40 participants (19 men, 21 women) were asked to complete the same task in a fearful, disgusting, or neutral scene in Study 2. The results showed that negative scenes inhibited the speed of recognizing self-faces. Furthermore, the above effect of negative scenes on self-relevant face recognition occurred with fearful rather than disgusting scenes. Our findings suggest the distinct effects of fearful scenes and disgusting scenes on self-relevant face processing, which may be associated with the automatic attentional capture to negative scenes (especially fearful scenes) and the tendency to escape self-awareness.

自我人脸识别是指一个人通过将自己的脸与他人的脸区分开来来识别自己的过程。尽管许多研究都探讨了负面信息对自我相关人脸加工的抑制作用,但很少有研究人员探讨负面场景是否会影响自我相关人脸加工。恐惧和恶心场景是典型的负面场景,但很少有研究数据考察它们对自我相关人脸识别的辨别效果。为了研究这些问题,本研究探讨了负面场景对自我相关人脸识别的影响。在研究 1 中,44 名参与者(20 名男性,24 名女性)被要求判断负面或中性场景中的目标人脸(自己的脸或朋友的脸)的方位;而在研究 2 中,40 名参与者(19 名男性,21 名女性)被要求在恐惧、恶心或中性场景中完成同样的任务。结果表明,负面场景会抑制识别自我面孔的速度。此外,上述负面场景对自我相关人脸识别的影响发生在恐惧场景而非恶心场景中。我们的研究结果表明,恐惧场景和恶心场景对自我相关人脸处理的影响是不同的,这可能与自动注意捕捉负面场景(尤其是恐惧场景)和逃避自我意识的倾向有关。
{"title":"The distinct effects of fearful and disgusting scenes on self-relevant face recognition.","authors":"Yuan Yuan, Lili Guan, Yifei Cao, Yang Xu","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2349764","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2024.2349764","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Self-face recognition denotes the process by which a person can recognize their own face by distinguishing it from another's face. Although many research studies have explored the inhibition effect of negative information on self-relevant face processing, few researchers have examined whether negative scenes influence self-relevant face processing. Fearful and disgusting scenes are typical negative scenes, but little research to data has examined their discriminative effects on self-relevant face recognition. To investigate these issues, the current study explored the effect of negative scenes on self-relevant face recognition. In Study 1, 44 participants (20 men, 24 women) were asked to judge the orientation of a target face (self-face or friend-face) pictured in a negative or neutral scene, whereas 40 participants (19 men, 21 women) were asked to complete the same task in a fearful, disgusting, or neutral scene in Study 2. The results showed that negative scenes inhibited the speed of recognizing self-faces. Furthermore, the above effect of negative scenes on self-relevant face recognition occurred with fearful rather than disgusting scenes. Our findings suggest the distinct effects of fearful scenes and disgusting scenes on self-relevant face processing, which may be associated with the automatic attentional capture to negative scenes (especially fearful scenes) and the tendency to escape self-awareness.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141066388","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Calling and job involvement: the role of prosocial motivation in the performance of mission-driven organization. 召唤与工作参与:亲社会动机在使命驱动型组织绩效中的作用。
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-15 DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2024.2349763
Hsien-Chun Chen, I-Heng Chen, Chin Tung Stewart Ng

Previous studies suggested that individuals with prosocial motivation have better job performance in mission-driven organizations. However, the mediating mechanisms underlying this link remain unclear. On the basis of person-environment theory, this research proposed that work as a calling and job involvement are two important mediators between employees' prosocial motivation and their job performance in mission-driven organizations. Through a multi-wave and muti-source approach, 420 independent subordinate-immediate supervisor dyads from 173 divisions or stations of the police department in Taiwan were obtained. Our results illustrated that the prosocial motivation-job performance relationship is sequentially mediated by work as a calling and job involvement. We further discuss implications for future research and practices in light of these findings.

以往的研究表明,在使命驱动型组织中,具有亲社会动机的个人会有更好的工作表现。然而,这种联系的中介机制仍不清楚。本研究以人-环境理论为基础,提出工作是一种召唤和工作参与是使命驱动型组织中员工亲社会动机与工作绩效之间的两个重要中介。本研究采用多波段、多来源的方法,从台湾警察局的 173 个部门或派出所获得了 420 个独立的下属-直属上司二元组。研究结果表明,亲社会动机与工作绩效之间的关系依次受到工作使命感和工作参与的中介作用。根据这些发现,我们进一步讨论了对未来研究和实践的启示。
{"title":"Calling and job involvement: the role of prosocial motivation in the performance of mission-driven organization.","authors":"Hsien-Chun Chen, I-Heng Chen, Chin Tung Stewart Ng","doi":"10.1080/00221309.2024.2349763","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2024.2349763","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous studies suggested that individuals with prosocial motivation have better job performance in mission-driven organizations. However, the mediating mechanisms underlying this link remain unclear. On the basis of person-environment theory, this research proposed that work as a calling and job involvement are two important mediators between employees' prosocial motivation and their job performance in mission-driven organizations. Through a multi-wave and muti-source approach, 420 independent subordinate-immediate supervisor dyads from 173 divisions or stations of the police department in Taiwan were obtained. Our results illustrated that the prosocial motivation-job performance relationship is sequentially mediated by work as a calling and job involvement. We further discuss implications for future research and practices in light of these findings.</p>","PeriodicalId":47581,"journal":{"name":"Journal of General Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140946184","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of General Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1