首页 > 最新文献

Topics in Cognitive Science最新文献

英文 中文
Dissociative Amnesia: Remembrances Under Cover. 失忆症:掩盖下的回忆
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12734
Angelica Staniloiu, Hans J Markowitsch

The existence or questionability of "repressed memories" can be discussed as being a matter of definition. It seems, however, far-fetched to consider all "lost" memories as caused by encoding problems, brain damage, forgetfulness, failure to disclose events, and so on. We argue that dissociative amnesia (DA) (or "psychogenic amnesia," or "functional amnesia," or, as we favor to call it, "mnestic block syndrome") is caused by psychic alterations, but ultimately they can be traced to changes in the physiology of the brain, as we are of the opinion that all memory processes-positive or negative-alter brain functions, sometimes more permanently, sometimes transiently. We have proven this idea using functional imaging techniques, in particular fluoro-deoxy-d-glucose positron emission tomography. Having investigated dozens of patients with severe and long-lasting DA conditions, we believe it to be disrespectful to many (but not to all) of the affected patients to question their disease condition, which can be proven to be not caused by feigning, malingering, or direct brain damage.

关于 "压抑记忆 "的存在与否,可以讨论定义问题。然而,将所有 "丢失 "的记忆都视为由编码问题、脑损伤、遗忘、未披露事件等造成的,似乎有些牵强。我们认为,解离性遗忘症(DA)(或 "精神性遗忘症",或 "功能性遗忘症",或我们喜欢称之为 "记忆阻滞综合症")是由精神改变引起的,但最终可以追溯到大脑生理的变化,因为我们认为,所有的记忆过程--无论是积极的还是消极的--都会改变大脑功能,有时是比较持久的,有时是短暂的。我们利用功能成像技术,特别是氟脱氧葡萄糖正电子发射断层扫描,证明了这一观点。在对数十名患有严重和持久性痴呆症的患者进行调查后,我们认为,质疑他们的病情是对许多(但不是所有)患者的不尊重,因为可以证明他们的病情不是由假装、装病或直接脑损伤造成的。
{"title":"Dissociative Amnesia: Remembrances Under Cover.","authors":"Angelica Staniloiu, Hans J Markowitsch","doi":"10.1111/tops.12734","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12734","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The existence or questionability of \"repressed memories\" can be discussed as being a matter of definition. It seems, however, far-fetched to consider all \"lost\" memories as caused by encoding problems, brain damage, forgetfulness, failure to disclose events, and so on. We argue that dissociative amnesia (DA) (or \"psychogenic amnesia,\" or \"functional amnesia,\" or, as we favor to call it, \"mnestic block syndrome\") is caused by psychic alterations, but ultimately they can be traced to changes in the physiology of the brain, as we are of the opinion that all memory processes-positive or negative-alter brain functions, sometimes more permanently, sometimes transiently. We have proven this idea using functional imaging techniques, in particular fluoro-deoxy-d-glucose positron emission tomography. Having investigated dozens of patients with severe and long-lasting DA conditions, we believe it to be disrespectful to many (but not to all) of the affected patients to question their disease condition, which can be proven to be not caused by feigning, malingering, or direct brain damage.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140904939","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neural Generative Models and the Parallel Architecture of Language: A Critical Review and Outlook 神经生成模型与语言的并行结构:批判性评论与展望
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-18 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12733
Giulia Rambelli, Emmanuele Chersoni, Davide Testa, Philippe Blache, Alessandro Lenci
According to the parallel architecture, syntactic and semantic information processing are two separate streams that interact selectively during language comprehension. While considerable effort is put into psycho‐ and neurolinguistics to understand the interchange of processing mechanisms in human comprehension, the nature of this interaction in recent neural Large Language Models remains elusive. In this article, we revisit influential linguistic and behavioral experiments and evaluate the ability of a large language model, GPT‐3, to perform these tasks. The model can solve semantic tasks autonomously from syntactic realization in a manner that resembles human behavior. However, the outcomes present a complex and variegated picture, leaving open the question of how Language Models could learn structured conceptual representations.
根据并行结构,句法和语义信息处理是两个独立的信息流,在语言理解过程中有选择地相互作用。虽然心理语言学和神经语言学投入了大量精力来理解人类理解过程中处理机制的相互影响,但在最近的神经大语言模型中,这种相互作用的性质仍然难以捉摸。在本文中,我们重温了有影响力的语言学和行为学实验,并评估了大型语言模型 GPT-3 执行这些任务的能力。该模型能以类似人类行为的方式自主解决语义任务和句法实现问题。然而,实验结果却呈现出复杂多变的局面,这也为语言模型如何学习结构化概念表征留下了悬念。
{"title":"Neural Generative Models and the Parallel Architecture of Language: A Critical Review and Outlook","authors":"Giulia Rambelli, Emmanuele Chersoni, Davide Testa, Philippe Blache, Alessandro Lenci","doi":"10.1111/tops.12733","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12733","url":null,"abstract":"According to the parallel architecture, syntactic and semantic information processing are two separate streams that interact selectively during language comprehension. While considerable effort is put into psycho‐ and neurolinguistics to understand the interchange of processing mechanisms in human comprehension, the nature of this interaction in recent neural Large Language Models remains elusive. In this article, we revisit influential linguistic and behavioral experiments and evaluate the ability of a large language model, GPT‐3, to perform these tasks. The model can solve semantic tasks autonomously from syntactic realization in a manner that resembles human behavior. However, the outcomes present a complex and variegated picture, leaving open the question of how Language Models could learn structured conceptual representations.","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140626375","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
One Size Does Not Fit All: Idiographic Computational Models Reveal Individual Differences in Learning and Meta‐Learning Strategies 一刀切:成语计算模型揭示学习和元学习策略的个体差异
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-03 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12730
Theodros M. Haile, Chantel S. Prat, Andrea Stocco
Complex skill learning depends on the joint contribution of multiple interacting systems: working memory (WM), declarative long‐term memory (LTM) and reinforcement learning (RL). The present study aims to understand individual differences in the relative contributions of these systems during learning. We built four idiographic, ACT‐R models of performance on the stimulus‐response learning, Reinforcement Learning Working Memory task. The task consisted of short 3‐image, and long 6‐image, feedback‐based learning blocks. A no‐feedback test phase was administered after learning, with an interfering task inserted between learning and test. Our four models included two single‐mechanism RL and LTM models, and two integrated RL‐LTM models: (a) RL‐based meta‐learning, which selects RL or LTM to learn based on recent success, and (b) a parameterized RL‐LTM selection model at fixed proportions independent of learning success. Each model was the best fit for some proportion of our learners (LTM: 68.7%, RL: 4.8%, Meta‐RL: 13.25%, bias‐RL:13.25% of participants), suggesting fundamental differences in the way individuals deploy basic learning mechanisms, even for a simple stimulus‐response task. Finally, long‐term declarative memory seems to be the preferred learning strategy for this task regardless of block length (3‐ vs 6‐image blocks), as determined by the large number of subjects whose learning characteristics were best captured by the LTM only model, and a preference for LTM over RL in both of our integrated‐models, owing to the strength of our idiographic approach.
复杂技能的学习取决于多个相互作用系统的共同贡献:工作记忆(WM)、陈述性长期记忆(LTM)和强化学习(RL)。本研究旨在了解学习过程中这些系统相对贡献的个体差异。我们针对刺激-反应学习、强化学习工作记忆任务的表现建立了四种ACT-R模型。该任务由短的 3 个图像和长的 6 个图像组成,以反馈为基础。学习后进行无反馈测试,在学习和测试之间插入干扰任务。我们的四个模型包括两个单一机制的 RL 和 LTM 模型,以及两个综合的 RL-LTM 模型:(a)基于 RL 的元学习,即根据最近的成功案例选择 RL 或 LTM 进行学习;(b)参数化的 RL-LTM 选择模型,其固定比例与学习成功与否无关。对于我们的学习者中的一部分人来说,每种模型都是最合适的(LTM:68.7%,RL:4.8%,Meta-RL:13.25%,bias-RL:13.25%),这表明个体部署基本学习机制的方式存在根本差异,即使对于简单的刺激-反应任务也是如此。最后,无论图块长度如何(3 个图块与 6 个图块),长期陈述性记忆似乎都是这项任务的首选学习策略,这一点可以从大量受试者的学习特征被仅有的 LTM 模型所最好地捕捉到,以及由于我们的成因分析方法的优势,在我们的两个综合模型中,LTM 比 RL 更受青睐这两点得到证实。
{"title":"One Size Does Not Fit All: Idiographic Computational Models Reveal Individual Differences in Learning and Meta‐Learning Strategies","authors":"Theodros M. Haile, Chantel S. Prat, Andrea Stocco","doi":"10.1111/tops.12730","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12730","url":null,"abstract":"Complex skill learning depends on the joint contribution of multiple interacting systems: working memory (WM), declarative long‐term memory (LTM) and reinforcement learning (RL). The present study aims to understand individual differences in the relative contributions of these systems during learning. We built four idiographic, ACT‐R models of performance on the stimulus‐response learning, Reinforcement Learning Working Memory task. The task consisted of short 3‐image, and long 6‐image, feedback‐based learning blocks. A no‐feedback test phase was administered after learning, with an interfering task inserted between learning and test. Our four models included two single‐mechanism RL and LTM models, and two integrated RL‐LTM models: (a) RL‐based meta‐learning, which selects RL or LTM to learn based on recent success, and (b) a parameterized RL‐LTM selection model at fixed proportions independent of learning success. Each model was the best fit for some proportion of our learners (LTM: 68.7%, RL: 4.8%, Meta‐RL: 13.25%, bias‐RL:13.25% of participants), suggesting fundamental differences in the way individuals deploy basic learning mechanisms, even for a simple stimulus‐response task. Finally, long‐term declarative memory seems to be the preferred learning strategy for this task regardless of block length (3‐ vs 6‐image blocks), as determined by the large number of subjects whose learning characteristics were best captured by the LTM only model, and a preference for LTM over RL in both of our integrated‐models, owing to the strength of our idiographic approach.","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140570117","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Introduction to topiCS Volume 16, Issue 2. TopiCS 简介》第 16 卷第 2 期。
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-30 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12732
Andrea Bender
{"title":"Introduction to topiCS Volume 16, Issue 2.","authors":"Andrea Bender","doi":"10.1111/tops.12732","DOIUrl":"10.1111/tops.12732","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140330222","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Collaborative Recall and the Construction of Collective Memory Organization: The Impact of Group Structure. 协作回忆与集体记忆组织的构建:群体结构的影响
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-02-13 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12639
Garrett D Greeley, Vanessa Chan, Hae-Yoon Choi, Suparna Rajaram

Collaborative recall synchronizes downstream individual retrieval processes, giving rise to collective organization. However, little is known about whether particular stimulus features (e.g., semantic relatedness) are necessary for constructing collective organization and how group dynamics (e.g., reconfiguration) moderates it. We leveraged novel quantitative measures and a rich dataset reported in recent articles to address, (a) whether collective organization emerges even for semantically unrelated material and (b) how group reconfiguration-changing partners from one recall to the next-influences collective organization. Participants studied unrelated words and completed three consecutive recalls in one of three conditions: Always recalling individually (III), collaborating with the same partners twice before recalling alone (CCI), or collaborating with different group members during two initial recalls, before recalling alone (CRI). Collective organization increased significantly following any collaboration (CCI or CRI), relative to "groups" who never collaborated (III). Interestingly, collaborating repeatedly with the same partners (CCI) did not increase collective organization compared to reconfigured groups, irrespective of the reference group structure (from Recall 1 or 2). Individuals, however, did tend to base their final individual retrieval on the most recent group recall. We discuss how the fundamental processes that underlie dynamic social interactions align the cognitive processes of many, laying the foundation for other collective phenomena, including shared biases, attitudes, and beliefs.

协作回忆可以同步下游的个体检索过程,从而产生集体组织。然而,对于特定的刺激特征(如语义相关性)是否是构建集体组织的必要条件,以及群体动态(如重组)如何调节集体组织,我们却知之甚少。我们利用新颖的定量测量方法和近期文章中报道的丰富数据集来解决以下问题:(a)即使是语义不相关的材料是否也会出现集体组织;(b)从一次回忆到下一次回忆,群体重组--改变伙伴--如何影响集体组织。受试者学习不相关的单词,并在三种条件之一下完成三次连续回忆:总是单独回忆(III),在单独回忆之前与相同的伙伴合作两次(CCI),或在最初的两次回忆中与不同的小组成员合作,然后再单独回忆(CRI)。相对于从未合作过的 "小组"(III)而言,任何合作(CCI 或 CRI)后的集体组织性都明显增强。有趣的是,与重新组合的小组相比,无论参考小组结构如何(来自回忆 1 或回忆 2),与相同的伙伴反复合作(CCI)都不会提高集体组织性。然而,个体确实倾向于根据最近的群体回忆进行最终的个体检索。我们讨论了动态社会互动的基本过程如何使许多人的认知过程保持一致,从而为其他集体现象(包括共同的偏见、态度和信念)奠定基础。
{"title":"Collaborative Recall and the Construction of Collective Memory Organization: The Impact of Group Structure.","authors":"Garrett D Greeley, Vanessa Chan, Hae-Yoon Choi, Suparna Rajaram","doi":"10.1111/tops.12639","DOIUrl":"10.1111/tops.12639","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Collaborative recall synchronizes downstream individual retrieval processes, giving rise to collective organization. However, little is known about whether particular stimulus features (e.g., semantic relatedness) are necessary for constructing collective organization and how group dynamics (e.g., reconfiguration) moderates it. We leveraged novel quantitative measures and a rich dataset reported in recent articles to address, (a) whether collective organization emerges even for semantically unrelated material and (b) how group reconfiguration-changing partners from one recall to the next-influences collective organization. Participants studied unrelated words and completed three consecutive recalls in one of three conditions: Always recalling individually (III), collaborating with the same partners twice before recalling alone (CCI), or collaborating with different group members during two initial recalls, before recalling alone (CRI). Collective organization increased significantly following any collaboration (CCI or CRI), relative to \"groups\" who never collaborated (III). Interestingly, collaborating repeatedly with the same partners (CCI) did not increase collective organization compared to reconfigured groups, irrespective of the reference group structure (from Recall 1 or 2). Individuals, however, did tend to base their final individual retrieval on the most recent group recall. We discuss how the fundamental processes that underlie dynamic social interactions align the cognitive processes of many, laying the foundation for other collective phenomena, including shared biases, attitudes, and beliefs.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9263931","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Conspiracy Theory as Individual and Group Behavior: Observations from the Flat Earth International Conference. 作为个人和群体行为的阴谋论:来自平地国际会议的观察。
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-18 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12662
Philip M Fernbach, Jonathan E Bogard

Conspiratorial thinking has been with humanity for a long time but has recently grown as a source of societal concern and as a subject of research in the cognitive and social sciences. We propose a three-tiered framework for the study of conspiracy theories: (1) cognitive processes, (2) the individual, and (3) social processes and communities of knowledge. At the level of cognitive processes, we identify explanatory coherence and faulty belief updating as critical ideas. At the level of the community of knowledge, we explore how conspiracy communities facilitate false belief by promoting a contagious sense of understanding, and how community norms catalyze the biased assimilation of evidence. We review recent research on conspiracy theories and explain how conspiratorial thinking emerges from the interaction of individual and group processes. As a case study, we describe observations the first author made while attending the Flat Earth International Conference, a meeting of conspiracy theorists who believe the Earth is flat. Rather than treating conspiracy belief as pathological, we take the perspective that is an extreme outcome of common cognitive processes.

阴谋论思想与人类相伴已久,但近来已逐渐成为社会关注的焦点以及认知科学和社会科学的研究课题。我们提出了一个研究阴谋论的三层框架:(1) 认知过程,(2) 个人,(3) 社会过程和知识群体。在认知过程层面,我们将解释一致性和错误信念更新确定为关键观点。在知识社群层面,我们探讨了阴谋社群如何通过促进一种传染性的理解感来促进错误信念,以及社群规范如何催化对证据的偏颇吸收。我们回顾了最近关于阴谋论的研究,并解释了阴谋论思维是如何从个人和群体的互动过程中产生的。作为一个案例研究,我们描述了第一作者在参加 "平地球国际会议 "时的观察,这是一个由相信地球是平的阴谋论者组成的会议。我们并没有把阴谋论信仰视为病态,而是认为这是普通认知过程的极端结果。
{"title":"Conspiracy Theory as Individual and Group Behavior: Observations from the Flat Earth International Conference.","authors":"Philip M Fernbach, Jonathan E Bogard","doi":"10.1111/tops.12662","DOIUrl":"10.1111/tops.12662","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Conspiratorial thinking has been with humanity for a long time but has recently grown as a source of societal concern and as a subject of research in the cognitive and social sciences. We propose a three-tiered framework for the study of conspiracy theories: (1) cognitive processes, (2) the individual, and (3) social processes and communities of knowledge. At the level of cognitive processes, we identify explanatory coherence and faulty belief updating as critical ideas. At the level of the community of knowledge, we explore how conspiracy communities facilitate false belief by promoting a contagious sense of understanding, and how community norms catalyze the biased assimilation of evidence. We review recent research on conspiracy theories and explain how conspiratorial thinking emerges from the interaction of individual and group processes. As a case study, we describe observations the first author made while attending the Flat Earth International Conference, a meeting of conspiracy theorists who believe the Earth is flat. Rather than treating conspiracy belief as pathological, we take the perspective that is an extreme outcome of common cognitive processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9480646","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Wisdom of the Crowd is not a Forgone Conclusion. Effects of Self-Selection on (Collaborative) Knowledge Construction. 群众的智慧不是被遗忘的结论。自我选择对(合作)知识构建的影响。
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-22 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12647
Marie-Christin Krebs, Aileen Oeberst, Ina von der Beck

Web 2.0 has elevated the possibilities of collaboration to unprecedented levels. Therein lies great potential, as the aptly coined phenomenon "Wisdom of the Crowd" implies. When it comes to controversial topics, however, there is no safety in numbers alone. On the contrary, collaboration among only like-minded people may even exacerbate biases (e.g., Echo Chambers). Yet, it is human nature to seek out like-minded others. Consequently, the process of self-selection is crucial if the heterogeneity of opinions serves as a safeguard against undesirable effects of group processes (e.g., attitude polarization). Accordingly, online environments that invite more heterogeneous (vs. homogeneous) users should produce less biased content. We tested this hypothesis in a field study, comparing articles on the same 20 controversial topics from the online encyclopedias Conservapedia and RationalWiki with Wikipedia (and Britannica serving as a gold standard) and exploring the opinions of discussants in the three online encyclopedias. As expected, articles from Conservapedia and RationalWiki were significantly less balanced than articles from Wikipedia and Britannica. We replicated this finding in a lab study with 257 participants who self-selected to one of three online wikis (Vegan Love, Nutrition, Meat & Fish) and individually as well as collaboratively wrote an encyclopedia-like article about "Diets." As expected, Wikis with a specific focus (Vegan Love, Meat & Fish) predominantly attracted authors with a positive attitude toward this focus and, as a consequence, resulted in more biased content than in the Nutrition Wiki. Overall, our results suggest that crowds alone do not guarantee wisdom-self-selection is a crucial process that needs to be taken into account.

Web 2.0 将协作的可能性提升到了前所未有的高度。正如 "群众的智慧 "这一恰当的现象所暗示的那样,其中蕴藏着巨大的潜力。然而,当涉及到有争议的话题时,单靠人多并不安全。相反,只有志同道合者之间的合作甚至会加剧偏见(如回声室)。然而,寻找志同道合者是人类的天性。因此,如果意见的异质性可以防止群体过程的不良影响(如态度极化),那么自我选择的过程就是至关重要的。因此,邀请更多异质(相对于同质)用户的在线环境所产生的内容偏差应该更小。我们在一项实地研究中对这一假设进行了验证,比较了在线百科全书 Conservapedia 和 RationalWiki 与维基百科(以及作为黄金标准的《大英百科全书》)中关于同样 20 个有争议主题的文章,并探讨了这三个在线百科全书中讨论者的观点。不出所料,Conservapedia 和 RationalWiki 的文章明显不如维基百科和大英百科的文章平衡。我们在一项实验室研究中重复了这一发现,257 名参与者自主选择了三个在线维基(爱素食、营养、肉类和鱼类)中的一个,并单独或合作撰写了一篇关于 "饮食 "的百科全书式文章。不出所料,有特定关注点的维基("爱素食"、"肉与鱼")主要吸引了对这一关注点持积极态度的作者,因此,与 "营养 "维基相比,"爱素食"、"肉与鱼 "维基的内容更有倾向性。总之,我们的研究结果表明,仅靠人群并不能保证智慧,自我选择是一个需要考虑的重要过程。
{"title":"The Wisdom of the Crowd is not a Forgone Conclusion. Effects of Self-Selection on (Collaborative) Knowledge Construction.","authors":"Marie-Christin Krebs, Aileen Oeberst, Ina von der Beck","doi":"10.1111/tops.12647","DOIUrl":"10.1111/tops.12647","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Web 2.0 has elevated the possibilities of collaboration to unprecedented levels. Therein lies great potential, as the aptly coined phenomenon \"Wisdom of the Crowd\" implies. When it comes to controversial topics, however, there is no safety in numbers alone. On the contrary, collaboration among only like-minded people may even exacerbate biases (e.g., Echo Chambers). Yet, it is human nature to seek out like-minded others. Consequently, the process of self-selection is crucial if the heterogeneity of opinions serves as a safeguard against undesirable effects of group processes (e.g., attitude polarization). Accordingly, online environments that invite more heterogeneous (vs. homogeneous) users should produce less biased content. We tested this hypothesis in a field study, comparing articles on the same 20 controversial topics from the online encyclopedias Conservapedia and RationalWiki with Wikipedia (and Britannica serving as a gold standard) and exploring the opinions of discussants in the three online encyclopedias. As expected, articles from Conservapedia and RationalWiki were significantly less balanced than articles from Wikipedia and Britannica. We replicated this finding in a lab study with 257 participants who self-selected to one of three online wikis (Vegan Love, Nutrition, Meat & Fish) and individually as well as collaboratively wrote an encyclopedia-like article about \"Diets.\" As expected, Wikis with a specific focus (Vegan Love, Meat & Fish) predominantly attracted authors with a positive attitude toward this focus and, as a consequence, resulted in more biased content than in the Nutrition Wiki. Overall, our results suggest that crowds alone do not guarantee wisdom-self-selection is a crucial process that needs to be taken into account.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9658149","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
By Design: How People Adapt to Cognitive Limitations in Politics. 设计:人们如何适应政治中的认知局限》(How People Adapt to Cognitive Limitations in Politics)。
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-14 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12690
Arthur Lupia

Elections, jury deliberations, lawmaking, high-stakes negotiations and related activities are human attempts to answer the question "How should we live?" Collectively, we know these activities as politics. Politics are how societies attempt to reconcile diverse individual needs with potential benefits of social coordination. People's beliefs about what others will do ainfluence many political strategies and outcomes. This article reviews how properties of cognition affect these political phenomena. Contrary to the common belief that many citizens are too ignorant to make competent political decisions, we focus on a central finding of social science-how societies can design contexts and environments to overcome individual cognitive limitations. These adaptations expand societal capacities to provide essential goods, services, and protections. In addition to explaining these adaptations, we also show how greater collaborations between cognitive science and the social sciences can help societies do even better.

选举、陪审团审议、法律制定、高风险谈判和相关活动都是人类试图回答 "我们应该如何生活 "这一问题的尝试。我们把这些活动统称为政治。政治是社会试图调和不同个人需求与社会协调潜在利益的方式。人们对他人所作所为的信念会影响许多政治策略和结果。本文回顾了认知的特性如何影响这些政治现象。人们普遍认为许多公民过于无知,无法做出合格的政治决策,与此相反,我们关注的是社会科学的一个核心发现--社会如何设计背景和环境来克服个人认知的局限性。这些调整扩大了社会提供基本商品、服务和保护的能力。除了解释这些适应性之外,我们还展示了认知科学与社会科学之间如何加强合作,帮助社会做得更好。
{"title":"By Design: How People Adapt to Cognitive Limitations in Politics.","authors":"Arthur Lupia","doi":"10.1111/tops.12690","DOIUrl":"10.1111/tops.12690","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Elections, jury deliberations, lawmaking, high-stakes negotiations and related activities are human attempts to answer the question \"How should we live?\" Collectively, we know these activities as politics. Politics are how societies attempt to reconcile diverse individual needs with potential benefits of social coordination. People's beliefs about what others will do ainfluence many political strategies and outcomes. This article reviews how properties of cognition affect these political phenomena. Contrary to the common belief that many citizens are too ignorant to make competent political decisions, we focus on a central finding of social science-how societies can design contexts and environments to overcome individual cognitive limitations. These adaptations expand societal capacities to provide essential goods, services, and protections. In addition to explaining these adaptations, we also show how greater collaborations between cognitive science and the social sciences can help societies do even better.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10590165","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What Makes Us Smart? 是什么让我们变得聪明?
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-22 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12656
Joseph Henrich, Michael Muthukrishna

How did humans become clever enough to live in nearly every major ecosystem on earth, create vaccines against deadly plagues, explore the oceans depths, and routinely traverse the globe at 30,000 feet in aluminum tubes while nibbling on roasted almonds? Drawing on recent developments in our understanding of human evolution, we consider what makes us distinctively smarter than other animals. Contrary to conventional wisdom, human brilliance emerges not from our innate brainpower or raw computational capacities, but from the sharing of information in communities and networks over generations. We review how larger, more diverse, and more optimally interconnected networks of minds give rise to faster innovation and how the cognitive products of this cumulative cultural evolutionary process feedback to make us individually "smarter"-in the sense of being better at meeting the challenges and problems posed by our societies and socioecologies. Here, we consider not only how cultural evolution supplies us with "thinking tools" (like counting systems and fractions) but also how it has shaped our ontologies (e.g., do germs and witches exist?) and epistemologies, including our notions of what constitutes a "good reason" or "good evidence" (e.g., are dreams a source of evidence?). Building on this, we consider how cultural evolution has organized and distributed cultural knowledge and cognitive tasks among subpopulations, effectively shifting both thinking and production to the level of the community, population, or network, resulting in collective information processing and group decisions. Cultural evolution can turn mindless mobs into wise crowds by facilitating and constraining cognition through a wide variety of epistemic institutions-political, legal, and scientific. These institutions process information and aid better decision-making by suppressing or encouraging the use of different cultural epistemologies and ontologies.

人类是如何变得足够聪明,能够生活在地球上几乎所有主要的生态系统中,创造出抵御致命瘟疫的疫苗,探索海洋深处,并经常乘坐铝管在 3 万英尺高空一边啃烤杏仁一边穿越地球?根据我们对人类进化的最新理解,我们将探讨是什么让我们比其他动物更加聪明。与传统观点相反,人类的聪明才智并非来自与生俱来的脑力或原始计算能力,而是来自世世代代在社区和网络中的信息共享。我们回顾了更大规模、更多样化和更优化的思维互联网络如何带来更快的创新,以及这种累积性文化进化过程的认知产品如何反馈性地使我们个人变得 "更聪明"--即更善于应对我们的社会和社会生态所带来的挑战和问题。在这里,我们不仅要考虑文化进化如何为我们提供 "思维工具"(如计数系统和分数),还要考虑文化进化如何塑造了我们的本体论(如细菌和女巫是否存在?)和认识论,包括我们对什么是 "好理由 "或 "好证据 "的概念(如梦是证据的来源吗?)在此基础上,我们将考虑文化进化是如何在亚人群中组织和分配文化知识和认知任务的,从而有效地将思维和生产转移到社区、人群或网络层面,形成集体信息处理和群体决策。文化进化可以通过政治、法律和科学等各种认识论机构来促进和约束认知,从而将无意识的暴民变成有智慧的人群。这些机构通过抑制或鼓励使用不同的文化认识论和本体论来处理信息,帮助人们更好地做出决策。
{"title":"What Makes Us Smart?","authors":"Joseph Henrich, Michael Muthukrishna","doi":"10.1111/tops.12656","DOIUrl":"10.1111/tops.12656","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How did humans become clever enough to live in nearly every major ecosystem on earth, create vaccines against deadly plagues, explore the oceans depths, and routinely traverse the globe at 30,000 feet in aluminum tubes while nibbling on roasted almonds? Drawing on recent developments in our understanding of human evolution, we consider what makes us distinctively smarter than other animals. Contrary to conventional wisdom, human brilliance emerges not from our innate brainpower or raw computational capacities, but from the sharing of information in communities and networks over generations. We review how larger, more diverse, and more optimally interconnected networks of minds give rise to faster innovation and how the cognitive products of this cumulative cultural evolutionary process feedback to make us individually \"smarter\"-in the sense of being better at meeting the challenges and problems posed by our societies and socioecologies. Here, we consider not only how cultural evolution supplies us with \"thinking tools\" (like counting systems and fractions) but also how it has shaped our ontologies (e.g., do germs and witches exist?) and epistemologies, including our notions of what constitutes a \"good reason\" or \"good evidence\" (e.g., are dreams a source of evidence?). Building on this, we consider how cultural evolution has organized and distributed cultural knowledge and cognitive tasks among subpopulations, effectively shifting both thinking and production to the level of the community, population, or network, resulting in collective information processing and group decisions. Cultural evolution can turn mindless mobs into wise crowds by facilitating and constraining cognition through a wide variety of epistemic institutions-political, legal, and scientific. These institutions process information and aid better decision-making by suppressing or encouraging the use of different cultural epistemologies and ontologies.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9386827","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How to Become a Memory: The Individual and Collective Aspects of Mnemicity. 如何成为记忆:记忆的个人和集体方面。
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-17 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12646
Johannes B Mahr

Human adults distinguish their mental event simulations along various dimensions-most prominently according to their "mnemicity": we track whether these simulations are outcomes of past personal experiences or not (i.e., whether we are "remembering" or "imagining"). This distinction between memory and imagination is commonly thought to reflect a deep architectural distinction in the mind. Against this idea, I argue that mnemicity is not based on a fundamentalstructural difference between memories and imaginations but is instead the result of metacognitive attribution and social construction. On this attributional view, mnemicity is likely a uniquely human capacity that both serves collective functions and has been shaped by collective norms. First, on the individual level, mnemicity attribution is an outcome of metacognitive learning: it relies on acquired interpretations of the phenomenal features of mental event simulations. Such interpretations are in part acquired through interactive reminiscing with other community members. Further, how the distinction between memory and imagination is drawn is likely sensitive to cultural norms about what remembering is, when it is appropriate to claim to remember, what can be remembered, and what remembering entails. As a result, how individuals determine whether they remember or imagine is bound to be deeply enculturated. Second, mnemicity attribution solves an important collective challenge: who to grant epistemic authority about the past. Solving this challenge is important because-for humans-the past represents not just an opportunity to learn about the future but to coordinate present social realities. How a community determines such social realities both draws on individuals' remembering and in turn shapes when, what, and how individuals remember.

成年人会根据不同的维度来区分他们的心理事件模拟--其中最突出的维度是根据它们的 "半明半暗性":我们会追踪这些模拟是否是过去个人经历的结果(即,我们是在 "记忆 "还是在 "想象")。人们通常认为,记忆与想象之间的这种区别反映了思维中深层次的结构区别。针对这一观点,我认为 "记忆"(mnemicity)并非基于记忆与想象之间的基本结构差异,而是元认知归因和社会建构的结果。根据这种归因观点,记忆力很可能是人类独有的能力,既能发挥集体功能,又受集体规范的影响。首先,在个人层面上,迷思归因是元认知学习的结果:它依赖于对心理事件模拟的现象特征的后天解释。这种解释部分是通过与其他社区成员的互动回忆获得的。此外,如何区分记忆和想象,很可能对有关记忆是什么、何时适合声称记忆、可以记忆什么以及记忆需要什么的文化规范很敏感。因此,个人如何确定自己是记忆还是想象,必然会受到文化的深刻影响。其次,记忆归属解决了一个重要的集体难题:由谁来授予关于过去的认识论权威。解决这一难题之所以重要,是因为对于人类来说,过去不仅是了解未来的机会,也是协调当前社会现实的机会。一个群体如何确定这种社会现实既依赖于个人的记忆,又反过来影响着个人记忆的时间、内容和方式。
{"title":"How to Become a Memory: The Individual and Collective Aspects of Mnemicity.","authors":"Johannes B Mahr","doi":"10.1111/tops.12646","DOIUrl":"10.1111/tops.12646","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Human adults distinguish their mental event simulations along various dimensions-most prominently according to their \"mnemicity\": we track whether these simulations are outcomes of past personal experiences or not (i.e., whether we are \"remembering\" or \"imagining\"). This distinction between memory and imagination is commonly thought to reflect a deep architectural distinction in the mind. Against this idea, I argue that mnemicity is not based on a fundamentalstructural difference between memories and imaginations but is instead the result of metacognitive attribution and social construction. On this attributional view, mnemicity is likely a uniquely human capacity that both serves collective functions and has been shaped by collective norms. First, on the individual level, mnemicity attribution is an outcome of metacognitive learning: it relies on acquired interpretations of the phenomenal features of mental event simulations. Such interpretations are in part acquired through interactive reminiscing with other community members. Further, how the distinction between memory and imagination is drawn is likely sensitive to cultural norms about what remembering is, when it is appropriate to claim to remember, what can be remembered, and what remembering entails. As a result, how individuals determine whether they remember or imagine is bound to be deeply enculturated. Second, mnemicity attribution solves an important collective challenge: who to grant epistemic authority about the past. Solving this challenge is important because-for humans-the past represents not just an opportunity to learn about the future but to coordinate present social realities. How a community determines such social realities both draws on individuals' remembering and in turn shapes when, what, and how individuals remember.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9696696","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Topics in Cognitive Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1