首页 > 最新文献

Topics in Cognitive Science最新文献

英文 中文
A Matter of Memory? Age-Invariant Relative Clause Disambiguation and Memory Interference in Older Adults. 记忆问题?不随年龄变化的相对句歧义和老年人的记忆干扰。
IF 2.9 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-16 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12753
Willem S van Boxtel, Laurel A Lawyer

Past research suggests that Working Memory plays a role in determining relative clause attachment bias. Disambiguation preferences may further depend on Processing Speed and explicit memory demands in linguistic tasks. Given that Working Memory and Processing Speed decline with age, older adults offer a way of investigating the factors underlying disambiguation preferences. Additionally, older adults might be subject to more severe similarity-based memory interference given their larger vocabularies and slower lexical access. Nevertheless, memory interference and sentence disambiguation have not been combined in studies on older adults before. We used a self-paced reading paradigm under memory load interference conditions. Older (n = 30) and Younger (n = 35) readers took part in the study online; reading times were recorded and measures of comprehension accuracy and load recall were collected. This setup allowed for the implicit measurement of attachment biases and memory interference effects interactively. Results show that similarity-based interference affected both age groups equally, but was more pronounced in NP2-biased structures, which took participants generally longer to read. Attachment preferences did not differ by group and were unaffected by Working Memory span. However, accuracy on recall prompts was predicted by Working Memory span in both groups. Findings of greater interference in syntactically dispreferred structures support unified processing models where parsing constraints naturally interact. The lack of age differences on our measures further aligns with research finding age-invariant implicit language processing.

过去的研究表明,工作记忆在决定相对从句附着偏向方面起着一定的作用。消歧偏好可能进一步取决于语言任务中的处理速度和显性记忆要求。鉴于工作记忆和处理速度会随着年龄的增长而下降,老年人为研究消歧偏好的基础因素提供了一种方法。此外,老年人的词汇量较大,词汇访问速度较慢,因此他们可能会受到更严重的基于相似性的记忆干扰。然而,记忆干扰和句子消歧以前从未在针对老年人的研究中结合过。我们在记忆负荷干扰条件下使用了自定步调阅读范式。老年读者(n = 30)和青年读者(n = 35)参加了在线研究;我们记录了阅读时间,并收集了理解准确性和负荷回忆的测量数据。通过这种设置,可以对依恋偏差和记忆干扰效应进行交互式隐性测量。结果表明,基于相似性的干扰对两个年龄组的人都有同样的影响,但在 NP2 偏向结构中更为明显,参与者一般需要更长的时间来阅读。各年龄组的依恋偏好没有差异,也不受工作记忆跨度的影响。然而,在两个组别中,工作记忆跨度都会影响回忆提示的准确性。研究结果表明,句法偏好结构的干扰更大,这支持了统一加工模型,在该模型中,解析约束自然会相互作用。我们的测量结果没有年龄差异,这与研究发现的与年龄无关的内隐语言加工进一步吻合。
{"title":"A Matter of Memory? Age-Invariant Relative Clause Disambiguation and Memory Interference in Older Adults.","authors":"Willem S van Boxtel, Laurel A Lawyer","doi":"10.1111/tops.12753","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12753","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Past research suggests that Working Memory plays a role in determining relative clause attachment bias. Disambiguation preferences may further depend on Processing Speed and explicit memory demands in linguistic tasks. Given that Working Memory and Processing Speed decline with age, older adults offer a way of investigating the factors underlying disambiguation preferences. Additionally, older adults might be subject to more severe similarity-based memory interference given their larger vocabularies and slower lexical access. Nevertheless, memory interference and sentence disambiguation have not been combined in studies on older adults before. We used a self-paced reading paradigm under memory load interference conditions. Older (n = 30) and Younger (n = 35) readers took part in the study online; reading times were recorded and measures of comprehension accuracy and load recall were collected. This setup allowed for the implicit measurement of attachment biases and memory interference effects interactively. Results show that similarity-based interference affected both age groups equally, but was more pronounced in NP2-biased structures, which took participants generally longer to read. Attachment preferences did not differ by group and were unaffected by Working Memory span. However, accuracy on recall prompts was predicted by Working Memory span in both groups. Findings of greater interference in syntactically dispreferred structures support unified processing models where parsing constraints naturally interact. The lack of age differences on our measures further aligns with research finding age-invariant implicit language processing.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142477782","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social Rationality and Human Reasoning: Logical Expressivism and the Flat Mind. 社会理性与人类推理:逻辑表达主义与扁平思维》。
IF 2.9 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-16 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12757
Mike Oaksford

This paper attempts to reconcile the claims that the mind is both flat (Chater, 2018) and highly rational (Oaksford & Chater, 2020). According to the flat mind hypothesis, the mind is a mass of inconsistent and contradictory fragments of experience. However, standard accounts of rationality from formal epistemology argue that to be rational, our beliefs must be consistent, and we must believe all the logical consequences of our beliefs. A social account of rationality is developed based on Brandom's (1994) logical expressivism, in which respecting the norms of logic and probability theory is still central but where these standards apply to our public commitments in social dialogical contexts rather than to our individual belief systems (Skovgaard-Olsen, 2017). According to this account, even if someone's individual beliefs are inconsistent, they cannot be condemned as irrational if they acknowledge the inconsistency and seek to resolve it. It is shown how this approach interacts with people's fragmented and shallow world knowledge, and its social distribution yields some counterintuitive consequences, such as it sometimes being rational individually not seeking to resolve contradictions. Other consequences of this social expressivist approach are considered, including for dual process theories of reasoning, our view of beliefs, the status of logic, and Fodor's (1983) view of central systems. It is concluded that people can have flat minds and yet be highly rational.

本文试图调和心灵是扁平的(Chater,2018)和高度理性的(Oaksford & Chater,2020)这两种说法。根据扁平心灵假说,心灵是由大量不一致、相互矛盾的经验片段组成的。然而,形式认识论对理性的标准解释认为,要做到理性,我们的信念必须是一致的,我们必须相信我们信念的所有逻辑结果。布兰德姆(Brandom,1994)的逻辑表达主义(logical expressivism)发展出了一种理性的社会论述,在这种论述中,尊重逻辑和概率论的规范仍然是核心,但这些标准适用于我们在社会对话语境中的公共承诺,而不是我们的个人信念体系(Skovgaard-Olsen,2017)。根据这一观点,即使某人的个人信念不一致,但如果他们承认不一致并寻求解决,就不会被谴责为非理性。本文展示了这种方法如何与人们零散而浅薄的世界知识相互作用,其社会分布产生了一些反直觉的后果,例如有时个人不寻求解决矛盾是理性的。研究还考虑了这种社会表达主义方法的其他后果,包括对推理的双重过程理论、我们对信念的看法、逻辑的地位以及福多(1983 年)对中心系统的看法。最后得出的结论是,人的思维可以是扁平的,但也可以是高度理性的。
{"title":"Social Rationality and Human Reasoning: Logical Expressivism and the Flat Mind.","authors":"Mike Oaksford","doi":"10.1111/tops.12757","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12757","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This paper attempts to reconcile the claims that the mind is both flat (Chater, 2018) and highly rational (Oaksford & Chater, 2020). According to the flat mind hypothesis, the mind is a mass of inconsistent and contradictory fragments of experience. However, standard accounts of rationality from formal epistemology argue that to be rational, our beliefs must be consistent, and we must believe all the logical consequences of our beliefs. A social account of rationality is developed based on Brandom's (1994) logical expressivism, in which respecting the norms of logic and probability theory is still central but where these standards apply to our public commitments in social dialogical contexts rather than to our individual belief systems (Skovgaard-Olsen, 2017). According to this account, even if someone's individual beliefs are inconsistent, they cannot be condemned as irrational if they acknowledge the inconsistency and seek to resolve it. It is shown how this approach interacts with people's fragmented and shallow world knowledge, and its social distribution yields some counterintuitive consequences, such as it sometimes being rational individually not seeking to resolve contradictions. Other consequences of this social expressivist approach are considered, including for dual process theories of reasoning, our view of beliefs, the status of logic, and Fodor's (1983) view of central systems. It is concluded that people can have flat minds and yet be highly rational.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142477786","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What Kind of Cognitive Technology Is the "Memory House"? 记忆屋 "是一种什么样的认知技术?
IF 2.9 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-16 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12761
Andrew M Riggsby

Ancient Roman "technical memory" is not (as much of the modern specialist literature would have it) a generative technology of association. Rather it is (as a literal reading of the texts would suggest) a specialized tool for precise serial recall. Modern experimental evidence both confirms the fitness for the purpose of the technique and shows why that purpose is not trivial, as some have suggested. While the mechanism(s) by which the technique operates are not fully understood, a review of the current literature suggests that it would have had the advantage over other mnemonic techniques by virtue of recruiting a variety of cognitive capacities. These likely include spatial/navigational mechanisms and possibly visual/imagery-based ones as well. Finally, small differences between the method as recorded in the ancient texts and similar methods that have been the subject of laboratory experiments are used to suggest possible directions for further experimentation.

古罗马的 "技术记忆 "并非(如许多现代专业文献所言)一种联想的生成技术。相反,它(正如对文本的字面解读所表明的那样)是一种用于精确序列记忆的专门工具。现代实验证据既证实了这一技术的适用性,也说明了为什么这一目的并不像某些人所说的那样微不足道。尽管人们对该技术的运作机制还不完全了解,但对现有文献的回顾表明,该技术与其他记忆技术相比具有优势,因为它能调动多种认知能力。这些能力可能包括空间/导航机制,也可能包括基于视觉/图像的机制。最后,我们利用古籍中记载的方法与实验室实验中的类似方法之间的细微差别,提出了进一步实验的可能方向。
{"title":"What Kind of Cognitive Technology Is the \"Memory House\"?","authors":"Andrew M Riggsby","doi":"10.1111/tops.12761","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12761","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Ancient Roman \"technical memory\" is not (as much of the modern specialist literature would have it) a generative technology of association. Rather it is (as a literal reading of the texts would suggest) a specialized tool for precise serial recall. Modern experimental evidence both confirms the fitness for the purpose of the technique and shows why that purpose is not trivial, as some have suggested. While the mechanism(s) by which the technique operates are not fully understood, a review of the current literature suggests that it would have had the advantage over other mnemonic techniques by virtue of recruiting a variety of cognitive capacities. These likely include spatial/navigational mechanisms and possibly visual/imagery-based ones as well. Finally, small differences between the method as recorded in the ancient texts and similar methods that have been the subject of laboratory experiments are used to suggest possible directions for further experimentation.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142477788","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Personalized Model-Driven Interventions for Decisions From Experience. 个性化模型驱动的经验决策干预。
IF 2.9 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-16 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12758
Edward A Cranford, Christian Lebiere, Cleotilde Gonzalez, Palvi Aggarwal, Sterling Somers, Konstantinos Mitsopoulos, Milind Tambe

Cognitive models that represent individuals provide many benefits for understanding the full range of human behavior. One way in which individual differences emerge is through differences in knowledge. In dynamic situations, where decisions are made from experience, models built upon a theory of experiential choice (instance-based learning theory; IBLT) can provide accurate predictions of individual human learning and adaptivity to changing environments. Here, we demonstrate how an instance-based learning (IBL) cognitive model, implemented in a cognitive architecture (Adaptive Control of Thought-Rational), can be used to model an individual's decisions in a cybersecurity defense task, accounting for both population average and individual variances. The same IBL model structure with identical architectural parameters generates the full range of human behavior through stochastic memory retrieval processes operating over and contributing to unique experiences. Recurrence quantification analyses allow us to look beyond average behavior between and within individuals to sequential patterns of trial-to-trial behavior. We show how model-tracing and knowledge-tracing techniques can be used to align the model to an individual in real time to drive adaptive and personalized signaling algorithms for a cybersecurity defense system. We also present a method for introspecting into the cognitive model to gain further insight into the cognitive salience of features factored into individual decisions. The combination of techniques provides a blueprint for personalized modeling of individuals. We discuss the results and implications of this adaptive and personalized method for cybersecurity defense and more generally for intelligent artifacts tailored to individual differences in domains such as human-machine teaming.

代表个体的认知模型为理解人类的各种行为提供了许多益处。个体差异的一种表现形式是知识差异。在动态情况下,决策是根据经验做出的,建立在经验选择理论(基于实例的学习理论;IBLT)基础上的模型可以准确预测人类个体的学习和对不断变化的环境的适应能力。在此,我们展示了在认知架构(自适应控制思维--理性)中实施的基于实例学习(IBL)的认知模型如何用于模拟个人在网络安全防御任务中的决策,同时考虑到群体平均值和个体差异。相同的 IBL 模型结构具有相同的架构参数,通过随机记忆检索过程的运行和独特经验的贡献,可生成人类的各种行为。通过递归量化分析,我们可以超越个体之间和个体内部的平均行为,观察试验到试验行为的连续模式。我们展示了如何利用模型追踪和知识追踪技术将模型与个体实时匹配,从而为网络安全防御系统提供自适应和个性化的信号算法。我们还介绍了一种对认知模型进行内省的方法,以进一步深入了解个人决策中考虑的特征的认知显著性。这些技术的结合为个人个性化建模提供了蓝图。我们将讨论这种自适应和个性化方法的结果和对网络安全防御的影响,以及在人机协作等领域针对个体差异量身定制智能人工智能的影响。
{"title":"Personalized Model-Driven Interventions for Decisions From Experience.","authors":"Edward A Cranford, Christian Lebiere, Cleotilde Gonzalez, Palvi Aggarwal, Sterling Somers, Konstantinos Mitsopoulos, Milind Tambe","doi":"10.1111/tops.12758","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12758","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Cognitive models that represent individuals provide many benefits for understanding the full range of human behavior. One way in which individual differences emerge is through differences in knowledge. In dynamic situations, where decisions are made from experience, models built upon a theory of experiential choice (instance-based learning theory; IBLT) can provide accurate predictions of individual human learning and adaptivity to changing environments. Here, we demonstrate how an instance-based learning (IBL) cognitive model, implemented in a cognitive architecture (Adaptive Control of Thought-Rational), can be used to model an individual's decisions in a cybersecurity defense task, accounting for both population average and individual variances. The same IBL model structure with identical architectural parameters generates the full range of human behavior through stochastic memory retrieval processes operating over and contributing to unique experiences. Recurrence quantification analyses allow us to look beyond average behavior between and within individuals to sequential patterns of trial-to-trial behavior. We show how model-tracing and knowledge-tracing techniques can be used to align the model to an individual in real time to drive adaptive and personalized signaling algorithms for a cybersecurity defense system. We also present a method for introspecting into the cognitive model to gain further insight into the cognitive salience of features factored into individual decisions. The combination of techniques provides a blueprint for personalized modeling of individuals. We discuss the results and implications of this adaptive and personalized method for cybersecurity defense and more generally for intelligent artifacts tailored to individual differences in domains such as human-machine teaming.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142477784","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Teachers' Gestures and How They Matter. 教师的手势及其重要性。
IF 2.9 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-14 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12755
R Breckinridge Church, Michelle Perry, Melissa A Singer, Susan Wagner Cook, Martha Wagner Alibali

How do teachers' gestures influence students' learning? This article reviews research investigating the role of gestures in communication, focusing on teachers' communication with their students, primarily in mathematics and science instruction. We first briefly consider gesture's role in communication more generally as a backdrop for considering teaching as a special context for communication. We then describe teachers' spontaneous gesturing in teaching contexts, and we consider how teachers' spontaneous gestures might influence students' learning. We then consider experimental studies that provide causal support for the effects of teachers' gestures on students' learning. We conclude by discussing future directions and implications for educational practice.

教师的手势如何影响学生的学习?本文回顾了有关手势在交流中的作用的研究,重点关注教师与学生的交流,主要是在数学和科学教学中。我们首先简要介绍了手势在一般交流中的作用,并以此为背景将教学视为一种特殊的交流环境。然后,我们描述了教师在教学情境中的自发手势,并探讨了教师的自发手势如何影响学生的学习。然后,我们考虑了一些实验研究,这些研究为教师手势对学生学习的影响提供了因果支持。最后,我们讨论了未来的发展方向和对教育实践的影响。
{"title":"Teachers' Gestures and How They Matter.","authors":"R Breckinridge Church, Michelle Perry, Melissa A Singer, Susan Wagner Cook, Martha Wagner Alibali","doi":"10.1111/tops.12755","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12755","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How do teachers' gestures influence students' learning? This article reviews research investigating the role of gestures in communication, focusing on teachers' communication with their students, primarily in mathematics and science instruction. We first briefly consider gesture's role in communication more generally as a backdrop for considering teaching as a special context for communication. We then describe teachers' spontaneous gesturing in teaching contexts, and we consider how teachers' spontaneous gestures might influence students' learning. We then consider experimental studies that provide causal support for the effects of teachers' gestures on students' learning. We conclude by discussing future directions and implications for educational practice.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142477787","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Mind Hidden in Our Hands. 藏在我们手中的心灵
IF 2.9 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-11 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12756
Susan Goldin-Meadow

Our hands are always with us and are used for communication all over the world. When children do not have an established language model to learn from, they use their hands to gesture, and these gestures take on the forms of language. In this role, the hands reveal the fundamental properties of the mind that give shape to language. When children do learn an established language, they again use their hands to gesture. These gestures do not look like language but form an integrated system with language. In this role, the hands can convey ideas not found in the language they accompany. In both contexts, gesture provides a clear view of the mind hidden in our hands.

我们的手总是和我们在一起,在世界各地都用于交流。当孩子们没有既定的语言模式可借鉴时,他们就会用手来比划,而这些手势就具有了语言的形式。手的这一作用揭示了思维的基本特性,而思维的基本特性正是语言的形态。当儿童学会一种既定的语言时,他们会再次用手比划。这些手势看起来不像语言,但却与语言形成了一个完整的系统。在这一角色中,手势可以传达它们所伴随的语言所没有的思想。在这两种情况下,手势都能让我们清楚地看到隐藏在我们双手中的思想。
{"title":"The Mind Hidden in Our Hands.","authors":"Susan Goldin-Meadow","doi":"10.1111/tops.12756","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12756","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Our hands are always with us and are used for communication all over the world. When children do not have an established language model to learn from, they use their hands to gesture, and these gestures take on the forms of language. In this role, the hands reveal the fundamental properties of the mind that give shape to language. When children do learn an established language, they again use their hands to gesture. These gestures do not look like language but form an integrated system with language. In this role, the hands can convey ideas not found in the language they accompany. In both contexts, gesture provides a clear view of the mind hidden in our hands.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142406947","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Brief Overview of Research into the Forgot-It-All-Along Effect. 遗忘曲线效应研究概述。
IF 2.9 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-05 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12670
Kristine Anthony, Steve M J Janssen

People often have difficulties remembering prior episodes of remembering, a phenomenon known as the forgot-it-all-along (FIA) effect. Although the effect was first discovered among victims of spontaneously recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse, laboratory paradigms of the FIA have shown that difficulties in remembering "remembering" can be elicited when the memory was previously recalled in a different context. Although much attention has been paid to establishing the robustness of the FIA phenomenon, little emphasis has been placed on the cognitive mechanisms underlying the effect. The goal of the present review is, therefore, to organize the literature surrounding the FIA effect and to discuss cognitive mechanisms that may explain the effect: source monitoring errors, the Encoding Specificity principle, and dating inaccuracies. By providing a brief overview of the literature surrounding the FIA phenomenon, this review can serve as a guide for how future studies may approach the FIA effect in the context of recovered memories.

人们在回忆之前的记忆时常常会遇到困难,这种现象被称为 "全忘效应"(FIA)。虽然这种效应最早是在自发恢复童年性虐待记忆的受害者中发现的,但实验室的 FIA 范例表明,当记忆之前是在不同的情境下被唤起时,也会引起 "回忆 "困难。尽管人们非常重视确定 "记忆障碍 "现象的稳健性,但却很少强调这种效应的认知机制。因此,本综述的目的是整理有关 FIA 效应的文献,并讨论可能解释该效应的认知机制:来源监控错误、编码特异性原则和日期不准确。本综述对有关 FIA 现象的文献进行了简要概述,可以为今后的研究如何在恢复记忆的背景下处理 FIA 效应提供指导。
{"title":"A Brief Overview of Research into the Forgot-It-All-Along Effect.","authors":"Kristine Anthony, Steve M J Janssen","doi":"10.1111/tops.12670","DOIUrl":"10.1111/tops.12670","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People often have difficulties remembering prior episodes of remembering, a phenomenon known as the forgot-it-all-along (FIA) effect. Although the effect was first discovered among victims of spontaneously recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse, laboratory paradigms of the FIA have shown that difficulties in remembering \"remembering\" can be elicited when the memory was previously recalled in a different context. Although much attention has been paid to establishing the robustness of the FIA phenomenon, little emphasis has been placed on the cognitive mechanisms underlying the effect. The goal of the present review is, therefore, to organize the literature surrounding the FIA effect and to discuss cognitive mechanisms that may explain the effect: source monitoring errors, the Encoding Specificity principle, and dating inaccuracies. By providing a brief overview of the literature surrounding the FIA phenomenon, this review can serve as a guide for how future studies may approach the FIA effect in the context of recovered memories.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":" ","pages":"675-690"},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9929551","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Did Dissociative Amnesia Evolve? 分离性健忘症是演变而来的吗?
IF 2.9 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-21 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12655
Lawrence Patihis

Dissociative amnesia is a diagnosis category that implies a proposed mechanism (often called dissociation) by which amnesia is caused by psychogenic means, such as trauma, and that amnesia is reversible later. Dissociative amnesia is listed in some of the most influential diagnostic manuals. Authors have noted the similarities in definition to repressed memories. Dissociative amnesia is a disputed category and phenomenon, and here I discuss the plausibility that this cognitive mechanism evolved. I discuss some general conditions by which cognitive functions will evolve, that is, the relatively continuous adaptive pressure by which a cognitive ability would clearly be adaptive if variation produced it. I discuss how adaptive gene mutations typically spread from one individual to the whole species. The article also discusses a few hypothetical scenarios and several types of trauma, to examine the likely adaptive benefits of blocking out memories of trauma, or not. I conclude that it is unlikely that dissociative amnesia evolved, and invite further development of these ideas and scenarios by others.

解离性失忆症是一种诊断类别,它意味着一种拟议的机制(通常称为解离),通过这种机制,失忆症是由精神原因(如创伤)引起的,而且失忆症在以后是可逆的。解离性失忆症被列入一些最有影响力的诊断手册中。作者们注意到其定义与被压抑的记忆相似。解离性遗忘症是一个有争议的类别和现象,我在这里讨论这种认知机制演变的可能性。我讨论了认知功能进化的一些一般条件,即相对持续的适应压力,如果变异产生了认知能力,这种能力显然是适应性的。我讨论了适应性基因突变通常是如何从一个个体扩散到整个物种的。文章还讨论了一些假设情景和几种类型的创伤,以研究屏蔽或不屏蔽创伤记忆可能带来的适应性益处。我的结论是,解离性健忘症不太可能是进化而来的,并邀请其他人进一步发展这些观点和情景。
{"title":"Did Dissociative Amnesia Evolve?","authors":"Lawrence Patihis","doi":"10.1111/tops.12655","DOIUrl":"10.1111/tops.12655","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Dissociative amnesia is a diagnosis category that implies a proposed mechanism (often called dissociation) by which amnesia is caused by psychogenic means, such as trauma, and that amnesia is reversible later. Dissociative amnesia is listed in some of the most influential diagnostic manuals. Authors have noted the similarities in definition to repressed memories. Dissociative amnesia is a disputed category and phenomenon, and here I discuss the plausibility that this cognitive mechanism evolved. I discuss some general conditions by which cognitive functions will evolve, that is, the relatively continuous adaptive pressure by which a cognitive ability would clearly be adaptive if variation produced it. I discuss how adaptive gene mutations typically spread from one individual to the whole species. The article also discusses a few hypothetical scenarios and several types of trauma, to examine the likely adaptive benefits of blocking out memories of trauma, or not. I conclude that it is unlikely that dissociative amnesia evolved, and invite further development of these ideas and scenarios by others.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":" ","pages":"608-615"},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9665123","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Suppression and Memory for Childhood Traumatic Events: Trauma Symptoms and Non-Disclosure. 童年创伤事件的抑制与记忆:创伤症状与不披露。
IF 2.9 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-23 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12667
Yuerui Wu, Dana Hartman, Yan Wang, Deborah Goldfarb, Gail S Goodman

Self-reported lost memory of child sexual abuse (CSA) can be mistaken for "repressed memory." Based on our longitudinal studies of memory and disclosure in child maltreatment victims who are now adults, we discuss findings relevant to "repressed memory cases." We examined relations between self-report of temporarily lost memory of CSA (subjective forgetting) and memory accuracy for maltreatment-related experiences (objective memory). Across two studies involving separate samples, we find evidence for memory suppression rather than repression: (1) Most adults who claimed temporary lost memory of CSA reported memory suppression and clarified that they could have remembered the event if asked; (2) subjective forgetting was positively associated with accurate objective memory for maltreatment-related experiences. Subjective forgetting was also related to increased adult trauma symptoms and related to childhood non-disclosure of CSA. Moreover, trauma-related psychopathology mediated the relation between non-disclosure and subjective forgetting. Implications for psychological theory and repressed memory cases are discussed.

自我报告的儿童性虐待(CSA)失忆可能会被误认为是 "压抑记忆"。基于我们对现已成年的儿童虐待受害者的记忆和披露的纵向研究,我们讨论了与 "压抑记忆案例 "相关的发现。我们研究了关于暂时失去 CSA 记忆的自我报告(主观遗忘)与虐待相关经历的记忆准确性(客观记忆)之间的关系。在两项涉及不同样本的研究中,我们发现了记忆抑制而非压抑的证据:(1) 大多数声称暂时失去 CSA 记忆的成年人都报告了记忆抑制,并澄清说如果被问及,他们可能会记得该事件;(2) 主观遗忘与虐待相关经历的客观记忆准确性呈正相关。主观遗忘还与成人创伤症状的增加有关,并与童年时未披露 CSA 有关。此外,与创伤相关的精神病理学对不披露和主观遗忘之间的关系起到了中介作用。本文讨论了心理学理论和压抑记忆案例的意义。
{"title":"Suppression and Memory for Childhood Traumatic Events: Trauma Symptoms and Non-Disclosure.","authors":"Yuerui Wu, Dana Hartman, Yan Wang, Deborah Goldfarb, Gail S Goodman","doi":"10.1111/tops.12667","DOIUrl":"10.1111/tops.12667","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Self-reported lost memory of child sexual abuse (CSA) can be mistaken for \"repressed memory.\" Based on our longitudinal studies of memory and disclosure in child maltreatment victims who are now adults, we discuss findings relevant to \"repressed memory cases.\" We examined relations between self-report of temporarily lost memory of CSA (subjective forgetting) and memory accuracy for maltreatment-related experiences (objective memory). Across two studies involving separate samples, we find evidence for memory suppression rather than repression: (1) Most adults who claimed temporary lost memory of CSA reported memory suppression and clarified that they could have remembered the event if asked; (2) subjective forgetting was positively associated with accurate objective memory for maltreatment-related experiences. Subjective forgetting was also related to increased adult trauma symptoms and related to childhood non-disclosure of CSA. Moreover, trauma-related psychopathology mediated the relation between non-disclosure and subjective forgetting. Implications for psychological theory and repressed memory cases are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":" ","pages":"718-730"},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9679372","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What the Acute Stress Response Suggests about Memory. 急性压力反应对记忆的启示
IF 2.9 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-18 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12664
Ayanna K Thomas, Alia N Wulff

Research suggests that stress has immediate and long-term effects on attention and memory. Rather than disrupting memory formation and consolidation, acute stress has been shown to shift attention processes resulting in a tradeoff between prioritized and nonprioritized information. Both arousal and stress result in cognitive and neurobiological shifts that often support memory formation. When an acute stressor occurs, it can distort immediate attentional focus, increasing processing for high-priority features while reducing processing for extraneous features. The downstream cognitive consequences for this shift in attention are better memory for some features and poorer memory for others when compared to conditions of low stress. However, individual differences (e.g., sex, age, basal stress response, and stress reactivity) all impact the relationship between the acute stress response and memory. Although acute stress generally benefits memory formation, we suggest that forgetting and later recovery of stressful memories can better be understood by examining factors that influence the subjective experience of stress and stress reactivity.

研究表明,压力会对注意力和记忆力产生直接和长期的影响。研究表明,急性压力不仅不会破坏记忆的形成和巩固,反而会转移注意力,从而在优先信息和非优先信息之间做出权衡。唤醒和压力都会导致认知和神经生物学上的转变,而这些转变往往有助于记忆的形成。当急性应激反应发生时,会扭曲即时的注意力焦点,增加对高优先级特征的处理,同时减少对无关特征的处理。与压力较小的情况相比,这种注意力转移的下游认知结果是对某些特征的记忆更好,而对其他特征的记忆较差。然而,个体差异(如性别、年龄、基础压力反应和压力反应性)都会影响急性压力反应与记忆之间的关系。虽然急性应激一般有利于记忆的形成,但我们认为,通过研究影响应激主观体验和应激反应性的因素,可以更好地理解应激记忆的遗忘和后期恢复。
{"title":"What the Acute Stress Response Suggests about Memory.","authors":"Ayanna K Thomas, Alia N Wulff","doi":"10.1111/tops.12664","DOIUrl":"10.1111/tops.12664","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research suggests that stress has immediate and long-term effects on attention and memory. Rather than disrupting memory formation and consolidation, acute stress has been shown to shift attention processes resulting in a tradeoff between prioritized and nonprioritized information. Both arousal and stress result in cognitive and neurobiological shifts that often support memory formation. When an acute stressor occurs, it can distort immediate attentional focus, increasing processing for high-priority features while reducing processing for extraneous features. The downstream cognitive consequences for this shift in attention are better memory for some features and poorer memory for others when compared to conditions of low stress. However, individual differences (e.g., sex, age, basal stress response, and stress reactivity) all impact the relationship between the acute stress response and memory. Although acute stress generally benefits memory formation, we suggest that forgetting and later recovery of stressful memories can better be understood by examining factors that influence the subjective experience of stress and stress reactivity.</p>","PeriodicalId":47822,"journal":{"name":"Topics in Cognitive Science","volume":" ","pages":"691-706"},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9489280","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Topics in Cognitive Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1