首页 > 最新文献

Infancy最新文献

英文 中文
Infant screen media and child development: A prospective community study 婴儿屏幕媒体与儿童发展:前瞻性社区研究
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-29 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12575
Ortal Slobodin, Orit E. Hetzroni, Moran Mandel, Sappir Saad Nuttman, Zainab Gawi Damashi, Eden Machluf, Michael Davidovitch

The current study examined longitudinal associations between early screen media exposure (assessed at 6, 12, and 24 months) and the child's motor and language/communication development at the ages of 24 and 36 months. We also aimed to study whether these associations varied by socioeconomic status (SES). Participants were 179 parent-infant dyads, recruited from well-baby clinic services during routine visits. Child development measures included standardized measures of developmental milestones as assessed by professionals and referral data to child developmental centers. Both measures were retrieved from the official health maintenance organization records by an expert in child development. Results indicated that screen exposure at 6 and 12 months was associated with a higher risk for language/communication deficits at 36 months in children with moderate or high SES but not in children with low SES. Our findings are consistent with existing literature demonstrating cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between early screen exposure and language development deficits. Given that media use practices and motivations vary among families from different backgrounds, further investigation of the interaction between SES and screen exposure is needed.

本研究考察了儿童早期接触屏幕媒体(在 6、12 和 24 个月时进行评估)与儿童在 24 和 36 个月时的运动和语言/沟通能力发展之间的纵向联系。我们还旨在研究这些关联是否因社会经济地位(SES)而异。研究对象是 179 个父母-婴儿二人组,他们都是在常规就诊期间从健康婴儿诊所服务机构招募的。儿童发育指标包括由专业人士评估的发育里程碑标准化指标和儿童发育中心的转介数据。这两项指标均由儿童发育专家从官方健康维护组织的记录中提取。结果表明,在中等或高社会经济地位的儿童中,6 个月和 12 个月时接触屏幕与 36 个月时出现语言/沟通障碍的较高风险有关,但与低社会经济地位的儿童无关。我们的研究结果与现有的文献一致,这些文献显示了早期屏幕接触与语言发展缺陷之间的横截面和纵向联系。鉴于不同背景的家庭使用媒体的习惯和动机各不相同,因此需要进一步研究社会经济地位与屏幕接触之间的相互作用。
{"title":"Infant screen media and child development: A prospective community study","authors":"Ortal Slobodin,&nbsp;Orit E. Hetzroni,&nbsp;Moran Mandel,&nbsp;Sappir Saad Nuttman,&nbsp;Zainab Gawi Damashi,&nbsp;Eden Machluf,&nbsp;Michael Davidovitch","doi":"10.1111/infa.12575","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12575","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The current study examined longitudinal associations between early screen media exposure (assessed at 6, 12, and 24 months) and the child's motor and language/communication development at the ages of 24 and 36 months. We also aimed to study whether these associations varied by socioeconomic status (SES). Participants were 179 parent-infant dyads, recruited from well-baby clinic services during routine visits. Child development measures included standardized measures of developmental milestones as assessed by professionals and referral data to child developmental centers. Both measures were retrieved from the official health maintenance organization records by an expert in child development. Results indicated that screen exposure at 6 and 12 months was associated with a higher risk for language/communication deficits at 36 months in children with moderate or high SES but not in children with low SES. Our findings are consistent with existing literature demonstrating cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between early screen exposure and language development deficits. Given that media use practices and motivations vary among families from different backgrounds, further investigation of the interaction between SES and screen exposure is needed.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 2","pages":"155-174"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12575","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139068198","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attachment security and problematic media use in infancy: A longitudinal study in the United States 婴儿期的依恋安全和问题媒体使用:美国的一项纵向研究
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-18 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12570
Jane Shawcroft, Sarah M. Coyne, Lisa Linder, Brandon N. Clifford, Brandon T. McDaniel

Media use during childhood has quickly become a norm across the United States and in other countries. One area still not well understood is the development of problematic (or maladaptive and disruptive) media use in children. This research examines the role of attachment security as a central component in the development of problematic media use over time in a sample of 248 parent-child dyads (9.50% African American, 20.66% Hispanic, 62.81% White, 2.07% Asian, 4.96% other ethnicities). We examined the relationship between attachment security and problematic media use one and 2 years later. We then constructed a mediation model examining parent responsiveness while jointly engaging in media use and during play as mediators between infant attachment security and problematic media use over time. Results suggest that while infant attachment security may be protective against developing problematic media use patterns, this relationship does not seem to be mediated by parent-child interactions while engaging in media or during play.

在美国和其他国家,儿童时期使用媒体已迅速成为一种常态。但人们对儿童问题性(或适应不良和破坏性)媒体使用的发展仍不甚了解。本研究以 248 个亲子家庭(9.50% 非裔美国人、20.66% 西班牙裔美国人、62.81% 白人、2.07% 亚洲人、4.96% 其他种族)为样本,探讨了依恋安全感作为问题性媒体使用发展过程中的核心要素所起的作用。我们研究了依恋安全与一年和两年后问题媒体使用之间的关系。然后,我们构建了一个中介模型,研究了父母在共同使用媒体和玩耍时的反应能力,以此作为婴儿依恋安全感与随着时间推移的问题媒体使用之间的中介。结果表明,虽然婴儿的依恋安全感可能对形成问题媒体使用模式具有保护作用,但这种关系似乎并不以亲子在使用媒体或游戏时的互动为中介。
{"title":"Attachment security and problematic media use in infancy: A longitudinal study in the United States","authors":"Jane Shawcroft,&nbsp;Sarah M. Coyne,&nbsp;Lisa Linder,&nbsp;Brandon N. Clifford,&nbsp;Brandon T. McDaniel","doi":"10.1111/infa.12570","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12570","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Media use during childhood has quickly become a norm across the United States and in other countries. One area still not well understood is the development of problematic (or maladaptive and disruptive) media use in children. This research examines the role of attachment security as a central component in the development of problematic media use over time in a sample of 248 parent-child dyads (9.50% African American, 20.66% Hispanic, 62.81% White, 2.07% Asian, 4.96% other ethnicities). We examined the relationship between attachment security and problematic media use one and 2 years later. We then constructed a mediation model examining parent responsiveness while jointly engaging in media use and during play as mediators between infant attachment security and problematic media use over time. Results suggest that while infant attachment security may be protective against developing problematic media use patterns, this relationship does not seem to be mediated by parent-child interactions while engaging in media or during play.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 2","pages":"137-154"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12570","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138716572","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Handedness as a major determinant of lateral bias in human functional cradling 手型是人类功能性摇篮侧向偏差的主要决定因素
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-15 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12572
Audrey L. H. van der Meer

Studies examining infant cradling have almost uniformly concluded with a general human left-side bias for cradling, indicating that people prefer to hold an infant to the left of their body. Explanations for the notion of the left-side cradling bias have traditionally been searched for in a variety of factors, for example, in terms of maternal heartbeat, genetic factors, in the form of an ear asymmetry where auditory information is perceived faster through the left ear, as a result of a right hemispheric functional specialization for perception of emotions and faces, and in identifying a motor bias of the infant, such as the tendency of newborn infants to lie with the face to the right when placed supine. Interestingly, handedness is generally considered an inadequate explanation for the lateralized cradling bias, despite it being an intuitively plausible one. In this brief review, I put forward the cradler's handedness as the most convincing and elegant determinant of the cradling bias. This explanation is consistent with a developmental cascades' framework where the cradling bias can be understood as the result of a multitude of factors across a range of levels and systems.

对婴儿摇篮的研究几乎一致得出结论,人类普遍存在左侧摇篮的偏好,这表明人们更喜欢将婴儿抱在自己身体的左侧。传统上,人们从多种因素中寻找左侧抱养偏好的解释,例如,母亲的心跳、遗传因素、耳朵不对称(即左耳更快地感知到听觉信息)、右半球对情绪和面孔感知的功能特化,以及确定婴儿的运动偏好,如新生儿仰卧时面部倾向于右侧。有趣的是,人们普遍认为 "手性 "不足以解释侧向摇篮抱的偏向,尽管它在直觉上是合理的。在这篇简短的综述中,我提出了摇篮者的手性是摇篮偏向最有说服力和最优雅的决定因素。这一解释与发展级联框架相一致,即摇篮偏向可被理解为一系列层次和系统中多种因素共同作用的结果。
{"title":"Handedness as a major determinant of lateral bias in human functional cradling","authors":"Audrey L. H. van der Meer","doi":"10.1111/infa.12572","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12572","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Studies examining infant cradling have almost uniformly concluded with a general human left-side bias for cradling, indicating that people prefer to hold an infant to the left of their body. Explanations for the notion of the left-side cradling bias have traditionally been searched for in a variety of factors, for example, in terms of maternal heartbeat, genetic factors, in the form of an ear asymmetry where auditory information is perceived faster through the left ear, as a result of a right hemispheric functional specialization for perception of emotions and faces, and in identifying a motor bias of the infant, such as the tendency of newborn infants to lie with the face to the right when placed supine. Interestingly, handedness is generally considered an inadequate explanation for the lateralized cradling bias, despite it being an intuitively plausible one. In this brief review, I put forward the cradler's handedness as the most convincing and elegant determinant of the cradling bias. This explanation is consistent with a developmental cascades' framework where the cradling bias can be understood as the result of a multitude of factors across a range of levels and systems.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 2","pages":"84-94"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12572","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138716397","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Characterization of children's verbal input in a forager-farmer population using long-form audio recordings and diverse input definitions 利用长格式录音和不同的输入定义对采农族儿童的语言输入进行表征。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-28 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12568
Camila Scaff, Marisa Casillas, Jonathan Stieglitz, Alejandrina Cristia

There is little systematically collected quantitative empirical data on how much linguistic input children in small-scale societies encounter, with some estimates suggesting low levels of directed speech. We report on an ecologically-valid analysis of speech experienced over the course of a day by young children (N = 24, 6–58 months old, 33% female) in a forager-horticulturalist population of lowland Bolivia. A permissive definition of input (i.e., including overlapping, background, and non-linguistic vocalizations) leads to massive changes in terms of input quantity, including a quadrupling of the estimate for overall input compared to a restrictive definition (only near and clear speech), while who talked to and around a focal child is relatively stable across input definitions. We discuss implications of these results for theoretical and empirical research into language acquisition.

很少有系统收集的定量经验数据表明,在小规模社会中,儿童会遇到多少语言输入,一些估计表明定向言语的水平很低。我们报告了一项生态学上有效的分析,分析了玻利维亚低地一个采食-园艺师人口中幼儿(N = 24, 6-58个月,33%为女性)在一天中所经历的语言。一个宽松的输入定义(即,包括重叠、背景和非语言发声)会导致输入量的巨大变化,包括与限制性定义(只有近距离和清晰的语音)相比,总体输入的估计是四倍,而谁与焦点儿童交谈以及围绕焦点儿童的输入定义相对稳定。我们讨论了这些结果对语言习得的理论和实证研究的意义。
{"title":"Characterization of children's verbal input in a forager-farmer population using long-form audio recordings and diverse input definitions","authors":"Camila Scaff,&nbsp;Marisa Casillas,&nbsp;Jonathan Stieglitz,&nbsp;Alejandrina Cristia","doi":"10.1111/infa.12568","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12568","url":null,"abstract":"<p>There is little systematically collected quantitative empirical data on how much linguistic input children in small-scale societies encounter, with some estimates suggesting low levels of directed speech. We report on an ecologically-valid analysis of speech experienced over the course of a day by young children (<i>N</i> = 24, 6–58 months old, 33% female) in a forager-horticulturalist population of lowland Bolivia. A permissive definition of input (i.e., including overlapping, background, and non-linguistic vocalizations) leads to massive changes in terms of input quantity, including a quadrupling of the estimate for overall input compared to a restrictive definition (only near and clear speech), while who talked to and around a focal child is relatively stable across input definitions. We discuss implications of these results for theoretical and empirical research into language acquisition.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 2","pages":"196-215"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-11-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138446608","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Older infants' social learning behavior under uncertainty is modulated by the interaction of face and speech processing 不确定条件下大婴儿的社会学习行为受面部加工和言语加工的相互作用调节。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-17 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12566
Julia Dillmann, Judith Evertz, Anna Krasotkina, Olivier Clerc, Olivier Pascalis, Gudrun Schwarzer

The origin of face or language influences infants' perceptual processing and social learning behavior. However, it remains unclear how infants' social learning behavior is affected when both information are provided simultaneously. Hence, the current study investigated whether and how infants' social learning in terms of gaze following is influenced by face race and language origin of an interaction partner in an uncertain situation. Our sample consisted of 91 Caucasian infants from German speaking families. They were divided into 2 age groups: Younger infants were 5- to 8-month-old (n = 46) and the older infants 11- to 20-month-old (n = 45). We used a modified online version of the gaze following paradigm by Xiao and colleagues by varying face race (Caucasian, and Asian faces) and language (German and French) of a female actor. We recorded infants looking behavior via webcam and coded it offline. Our results revealed that older but not younger infants were biased to follow the gaze of own-race adults speaking their native language. Our findings show that older infants are clearly influenced by adults' ethnicity and language in social learning situations of uncertainty.

面孔或语言的来源影响着婴儿的知觉加工和社会学习行为。然而,当这两种信息同时提供时,婴儿的社会学习行为是如何受到影响的,目前尚不清楚。因此,本研究旨在探讨在不确定情境下,婴儿在注视跟随方面的社会学习是否以及如何受到互动伙伴的面孔种族和语言来源的影响。我们的样本包括来自德语家庭的91名高加索婴儿。他们被分为两个年龄组:年龄较小的婴儿为5至8个月(n = 46),年龄较大的婴儿为11至20个月(n = 45)。我们使用了Xiao和他的同事通过改变女演员的面部种族(高加索人和亚洲人的面孔)和语言(德语和法语)来修改的在线版本的凝视跟随范式。我们通过网络摄像头记录下婴儿看东西的行为,并离线编码。我们的研究结果显示,年龄较大但年龄较小的婴儿倾向于跟随说母语的同族成年人的目光。我们的研究结果表明,在不确定的社会学习情境中,大婴儿明显受到成人种族和语言的影响。
{"title":"Older infants' social learning behavior under uncertainty is modulated by the interaction of face and speech processing","authors":"Julia Dillmann,&nbsp;Judith Evertz,&nbsp;Anna Krasotkina,&nbsp;Olivier Clerc,&nbsp;Olivier Pascalis,&nbsp;Gudrun Schwarzer","doi":"10.1111/infa.12566","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12566","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The origin of face or language influences infants' perceptual processing and social learning behavior. However, it remains unclear how infants' social learning behavior is affected when both information are provided simultaneously. Hence, the current study investigated whether and how infants' social learning in terms of gaze following is influenced by face race and language origin of an interaction partner in an uncertain situation. Our sample consisted of 91 Caucasian infants from German speaking families. They were divided into 2 age groups: Younger infants were 5- to 8-month-old (<i>n</i> = 46) and the older infants 11- to 20-month-old (<i>n</i> = 45). We used a modified online version of the gaze following paradigm by Xiao and colleagues by varying face race (Caucasian, and Asian faces) and language (German and French) of a female actor. We recorded infants looking behavior via webcam and coded it offline. Our results revealed that older but not younger infants were biased to follow the gaze of own-race adults speaking their native language. Our findings show that older infants are clearly influenced by adults' ethnicity and language in social learning situations of uncertainty.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 1","pages":"56-71"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-11-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12566","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136399804","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Look before you reach: Fixation-reach latencies predict reaching kinematics in toddlers 伸手前先看:注视-伸手潜伏期预测幼儿的伸手运动。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-11 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12567
Drew H. Abney, Christian M. Jerry, Linda B. Smith, Chen Yu

Research on infant and toddler reaching has shown evidence for motor planning after the initiation of the reaching action. However, the reach action sequence does not begin after the initiation of a reach but rather includes the initial visual fixations onto the target object occurring before the reach. We developed a paradigm that synchronizes head-mounted eye-tracking and motion capture to determine whether the latency between the first visual fixation on a target object and the first reaching movement toward the object predicts subsequent reaching behavior in toddlers. In a corpus of over one hundred reach sequences produced by 17 toddlers, we found that longer fixation-reach latencies during the pre-reach phase predicted slower reaches. If the slowness of an executed reach indicates reach difficulty, then the duration of pre-reach planning would be correlated with reach difficulty. However, no relation was found with pre-reach planning duration when reach difficulty was measured by usual factors and independent of reach duration. The findings raise important questions about the measurement of reach difficulty, models of motor control, and possible developmental changes in the relations between pre-planning and continuously unfolding motor plans throughout an action sequence.

对婴幼儿伸手动作的研究显示了伸手动作开始后运动规划的证据。然而,到达动作序列不是在到达开始之后开始的,而是包括在到达之前发生的对目标物体的初始视觉注视。我们开发了一个同步头戴式眼球追踪和动作捕捉的范式,以确定第一次注视目标物体和第一次向目标物体移动之间的延迟是否预测了幼儿随后的伸手行为。在17名幼儿的100多个到达序列的语料库中,我们发现在到达前阶段较长的注视到达潜伏期预示着较慢的到达。如果执行到达的缓慢表示到达的难度,那么到达前计划的持续时间将与到达的难度相关。然而,当采用常规因素测量到达难度且与到达时间无关时,到达难度与到达前计划时间没有关系。这一发现提出了一些重要的问题,包括到达难度的测量、运动控制模型,以及在整个动作序列中,预先计划和不断展开的运动计划之间的关系可能发生的发展变化。
{"title":"Look before you reach: Fixation-reach latencies predict reaching kinematics in toddlers","authors":"Drew H. Abney,&nbsp;Christian M. Jerry,&nbsp;Linda B. Smith,&nbsp;Chen Yu","doi":"10.1111/infa.12567","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12567","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Research on infant and toddler reaching has shown evidence for motor planning after the initiation of the reaching action. However, the reach action sequence does not begin after the initiation of a reach but rather includes the initial visual fixations onto the target object occurring before the reach. We developed a paradigm that synchronizes head-mounted eye-tracking and motion capture to determine whether the latency between the first visual fixation on a target object and the first reaching movement toward the object predicts subsequent reaching behavior in toddlers. In a corpus of over one hundred reach sequences produced by 17 toddlers, we found that longer fixation-reach latencies during the pre-reach phase predicted slower reaches. If the slowness of an executed reach indicates reach difficulty, then the duration of pre-reach planning would be correlated with reach difficulty. However, no relation was found with pre-reach planning duration when reach difficulty was measured by usual factors and independent of reach duration. The findings raise important questions about the measurement of reach difficulty, models of motor control, and possible developmental changes in the relations between pre-planning and continuously unfolding motor plans throughout an action sequence.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 1","pages":"6-21"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-11-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12567","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89719957","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Newborns' perception of approach and withdrawal from biological movement: A closeness story 新生儿对生物运动的接近和退出的感知:一个亲密的故事。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-10-23 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12565
Elisa Roberti, Margaret Addabbo, Lorenzo Colombo, Matteo Porro, Chiara Turati

Since birth, infants discriminate the biological motion (BM) revealed by point-light displays (PLDs). To date, no studies have explored whether newborns differentiate BM that approaches rather than withdraws from them. Yet, approach and withdrawal are two fundamental motivations in the socio-emotional world, key to developing empathy and prosocial behavior. Through a looking-behavior paradigm, we demonstrated that a few hours after birth, a human figure approaching attracted more visual attention than a human figure receding, showing that newborns are attuned to PLDs of others moving toward rather than walking away from them. Further, a withdrawing body appears to be less attractive than withdrawing scrambled points. Altogether, these observations support the existence of an early predisposition toward social closeness that might have its roots in an evolutionary perspective.

从出生起,婴儿就可以辨别点光显示器(PLD)显示的生物运动(BM)。到目前为止,还没有研究探讨新生儿是否区分接近而不是退出的BM。然而,接近和退缩是社会情感世界中的两个基本动机,是发展同理心和亲社会行为的关键。通过观察行为范式,我们证明,在出生后几个小时,接近的人形比后退的人形吸引了更多的视觉注意力,这表明新生儿适应了其他人向他们靠近而不是离开他们的PLD。此外,撤回的主体似乎不如撤回混乱的点更有吸引力。总之,这些观察结果支持了早期社会亲密倾向的存在,这可能源于进化的角度。
{"title":"Newborns' perception of approach and withdrawal from biological movement: A closeness story","authors":"Elisa Roberti,&nbsp;Margaret Addabbo,&nbsp;Lorenzo Colombo,&nbsp;Matteo Porro,&nbsp;Chiara Turati","doi":"10.1111/infa.12565","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12565","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Since birth, infants discriminate the biological motion (BM) revealed by point-light displays (PLDs). To date, no studies have explored whether newborns differentiate BM that approaches rather than withdraws from them. Yet, approach and withdrawal are two fundamental motivations in the socio-emotional world, key to developing empathy and prosocial behavior. Through a looking-behavior paradigm, we demonstrated that a few hours after birth, a human figure approaching attracted more visual attention than a human figure receding, showing that newborns are attuned to PLDs of others moving toward rather than walking away from them. Further, a withdrawing body appears to be less attractive than withdrawing scrambled points. Altogether, these observations support the existence of an early predisposition toward social closeness that might have its roots in an evolutionary perspective.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 1","pages":"22-30"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-10-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12565","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49693149","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Validation of an open source, remote web-based eye-tracking method (WebGazer) for research in early childhood 验证用于儿童早期研究的开源、远程基于网络的眼动追踪方法(WebGazer)。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-10-18 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12564
Adrian Steffan, Lucie Zimmer, Natalia Arias-Trejo, Manuel Bohn, Rodrigo Dal Ben, Marco A. Flores-Coronado, Laura Franchin, Isa Garbisch, Charlotte Grosse Wiesmann, J. Kiley Hamlin, Naomi Havron, Jessica F. Hay, Tone K. Hermansen, Krisztina V. Jakobsen, Steven Kalinke, Eon-Suk Ko, Louisa Kulke, Julien Mayor, Marek Meristo, David Moreau, Seongmin Mun, Julia Prein, Hannes Rakoczy, Katrin Rothmaler, Daniela Santos Oliveira, Elizabeth A. Simpson, Sylvain Sirois, Eleanor S. Smith, Karin Strid, Anna-Lena Tebbe, Maleen Thiele, Francis Yuen, Tobias Schuwerk

Measuring eye movements remotely via the participant's webcam promises to be an attractive methodological addition to in-person eye-tracking in the lab. However, there is a lack of systematic research comparing remote web-based eye-tracking with in-lab eye-tracking in young children. We report a multi-lab study that compared these two measures in an anticipatory looking task with toddlers using WebGazer.js and jsPsych. Results of our remotely tested sample of 18-27-month-old toddlers (N = 125) revealed that web-based eye-tracking successfully captured goal-based action predictions, although the proportion of the goal-directed anticipatory looking was lower compared to the in-lab sample (N = 70). As expected, attrition rate was substantially higher in the web-based (42%) than the in-lab sample (10%). Excluding trials based on visual inspection of the match of time-locked gaze coordinates and the participant's webcam video overlayed on the stimuli was an important preprocessing step to reduce noise in the data. We discuss the use of this remote web-based method in comparison with other current methodological innovations. Our study demonstrates that remote web-based eye-tracking can be a useful tool for testing toddlers, facilitating recruitment of larger and more diverse samples; a caveat to consider is the larger drop-out rate.

通过参与者的网络摄像头远程测量眼球运动有望成为实验室中面对面眼动追踪的一种有吸引力的方法。然而,缺乏对幼儿远程网络眼动追踪与实验室眼动追踪进行比较的系统研究。我们报告了一项多实验室研究,该研究使用WebGazer.js和jsPsych对学步儿童的前瞻性任务中的这两项指标进行了比较。我们对18-27个月大幼儿的远程测试样本(N=125)的结果显示,基于网络的眼动追踪成功地捕捉到了基于目标的动作预测,尽管与实验室样本(N=70)相比,目标导向的预期注视的比例较低。正如预期的那样,网络样本的流失率(42%)明显高于实验室样本(10%)。排除基于时间锁定凝视坐标匹配的视觉检查和覆盖在刺激物上的参与者的网络摄像头视频的试验是减少数据噪声的重要预处理步骤。我们讨论了这种基于远程网络的方法与当前其他方法创新的比较。我们的研究表明,基于网络的远程眼动追踪可以成为测试幼儿的有用工具,有助于招募更大、更多样的样本;需要考虑的一个警告是辍学率更高。
{"title":"Validation of an open source, remote web-based eye-tracking method (WebGazer) for research in early childhood","authors":"Adrian Steffan,&nbsp;Lucie Zimmer,&nbsp;Natalia Arias-Trejo,&nbsp;Manuel Bohn,&nbsp;Rodrigo Dal Ben,&nbsp;Marco A. Flores-Coronado,&nbsp;Laura Franchin,&nbsp;Isa Garbisch,&nbsp;Charlotte Grosse Wiesmann,&nbsp;J. Kiley Hamlin,&nbsp;Naomi Havron,&nbsp;Jessica F. Hay,&nbsp;Tone K. Hermansen,&nbsp;Krisztina V. Jakobsen,&nbsp;Steven Kalinke,&nbsp;Eon-Suk Ko,&nbsp;Louisa Kulke,&nbsp;Julien Mayor,&nbsp;Marek Meristo,&nbsp;David Moreau,&nbsp;Seongmin Mun,&nbsp;Julia Prein,&nbsp;Hannes Rakoczy,&nbsp;Katrin Rothmaler,&nbsp;Daniela Santos Oliveira,&nbsp;Elizabeth A. Simpson,&nbsp;Sylvain Sirois,&nbsp;Eleanor S. Smith,&nbsp;Karin Strid,&nbsp;Anna-Lena Tebbe,&nbsp;Maleen Thiele,&nbsp;Francis Yuen,&nbsp;Tobias Schuwerk","doi":"10.1111/infa.12564","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12564","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Measuring eye movements remotely via the participant's webcam promises to be an attractive methodological addition to in-person eye-tracking in the lab. However, there is a lack of systematic research comparing remote web-based eye-tracking with in-lab eye-tracking in young children. We report a multi-lab study that compared these two measures in an anticipatory looking task with toddlers using WebGazer.js and jsPsych. Results of our remotely tested sample of 18-27-month-old toddlers (<i>N</i> = 125) revealed that web-based eye-tracking successfully captured goal-based action predictions, although the proportion of the goal-directed anticipatory looking was lower compared to the in-lab sample (<i>N</i> = 70). As expected, attrition rate was substantially higher in the web-based (42%) than the in-lab sample (10%). Excluding trials based on visual inspection of the match of time-locked gaze coordinates and the participant's webcam video overlayed on the stimuli was an important preprocessing step to reduce noise in the data. We discuss the use of this remote web-based method in comparison with other current methodological innovations. Our study demonstrates that remote web-based eye-tracking can be a useful tool for testing toddlers, facilitating recruitment of larger and more diverse samples; a caveat to consider is the larger drop-out rate.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 1","pages":"31-55"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12564","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41239896","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The impact of a music enrichment program during infancy and early toddlerhood on effortful control at age 3: A preliminary investigation 婴儿期和幼儿期音乐强化计划对3岁时努力控制的影响:初步调查。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-10-12 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12563
Amy R. Smith, Casey M. McGregor, Katelyn Carr, Leonard H. Epstein, Catherine Serwatka, Rocco Paluch, Jacqueline Piazza, Shannon Shisler, Kai Ling Kong

Effortful control (EC), a self-regulation skill, is associated with long-term developmental outcomes. Music has been associated with infant self-regulation and may be an intervention strategy for enhancing EC during toddlerhood. This investigation included 32 parent-child dyads from a previously conducted randomized controlled trial (RCT). Participants (9-15-months old at baseline) attended either a music enrichment program or a playdate control once a week for 1 year and monthly for an additional year. At age 3, participants completed snack and gift delay effortful control tasks. Groups were compared using one-way ANOVA. We found that participants in the music group had a significantly higher score during snack delay (music mean = 3.47 ± 0.94; control mean = 2.45 ± 1.51; p = 0.03; Cohen's d = 0.84). We did not find a significant group difference for latency to peek (music mean = 39.10 ± 20.10; control mean = 30.90 ± 19.88; p = 0.25; d = 0.57) or latency to touch (music mean = 105.73 ± 417.69; control mean = 98.35 ± 28.84; p = 0.38; d = 0.29) for the gift task. This study provides initial evidence that early participation in a music enrichment program may benefit later development of EC. This study is registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT02936284).

努力控制(EC)是一种自我调节技能,与长期发展结果有关。音乐与婴儿的自我调节有关,可能是幼儿期增强EC的一种干预策略。这项调查包括了来自先前进行的随机对照试验(RCT)的32对亲子二人组。参与者(基线时9-15个月大)每周参加一次音乐强化计划或游戏日期控制,为期1年,每月参加一次,为期一年。在3岁时,参与者完成了零食和礼物延迟的努力控制任务。使用单因素方差分析对各组进行比较。我们发现,音乐组的参与者在零食延迟期间得分显著较高(音乐平均值=3.47±0.94;对照组平均值=2.45±1.51;p=0.03;Cohen’s d=0.84)(音乐平均值=105.73±417.69;对照平均值=98.35±28.84;p=0.38;d=0.29)。这项研究提供了初步证据,证明早期参与音乐强化计划可能有利于EC的后期发展。这项研究在ClinicalTrials.gov(NCT02936284)上注册。
{"title":"The impact of a music enrichment program during infancy and early toddlerhood on effortful control at age 3: A preliminary investigation","authors":"Amy R. Smith,&nbsp;Casey M. McGregor,&nbsp;Katelyn Carr,&nbsp;Leonard H. Epstein,&nbsp;Catherine Serwatka,&nbsp;Rocco Paluch,&nbsp;Jacqueline Piazza,&nbsp;Shannon Shisler,&nbsp;Kai Ling Kong","doi":"10.1111/infa.12563","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12563","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Effortful control (EC), a self-regulation skill, is associated with long-term developmental outcomes. Music has been associated with infant self-regulation and may be an intervention strategy for enhancing EC during toddlerhood. This investigation included 32 parent-child dyads from a previously conducted randomized controlled trial (RCT). Participants (9-15-months old at baseline) attended either a music enrichment program or a playdate control once a week for 1 year and monthly for an additional year. At age 3, participants completed snack and gift delay effortful control tasks. Groups were compared using one-way ANOVA. We found that participants in the music group had a significantly higher score during snack delay (music mean = 3.47 ± 0.94; control mean = 2.45 ± 1.51; <i>p</i> = 0.03; Cohen's <i>d</i> = 0.84). We did not find a significant group difference for latency to peek (music mean = 39.10 ± 20.10; control mean = 30.90 ± 19.88; <i>p</i> = 0.25; <i>d</i> = 0.57) or latency to touch (music mean = 105.73 ± 417.69; control mean = 98.35 ± 28.84; <i>p</i> = 0.38; <i>d</i> = 0.29) for the gift task. This study provides initial evidence that early participation in a music enrichment program may benefit later development of EC. This study is registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT02936284).</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 1","pages":"72-79"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-10-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41216143","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Double it up: Vocabulary size comparisons between UK bilingual and monolingual toddlers 加倍:英国双语和单语幼儿的词汇大小比较。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-10-04 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12562
Serene Siow, Nicola A. Gillen, Irina Lepădatu, Kim Plunkett

We compared vocabulary sizes in comprehension and production between bilingual toddlers growing up in the United Kingdom (UK) and age-matched UK English monolinguals (12–36 months old) using parent-report vocabulary questionnaires. We found that bilingual toddlers' vocabulary sizes in English were smaller than the vocabulary sizes of their monolingual peers. Notably, this vocabulary gap was not found when groups were compared on conceptual vocabulary in comprehension. Conceptual scoring also reduced the vocabulary gap in production but group differences were still significant. Bilingual toddlers knew more words than monolinguals when words across their two languages were added together, for both comprehension and production. This large total vocabulary size could be attributed to a high proportion of doublets (cross-linguistic word pairs with the same meaning) in bilinguals' vocabularies. These findings are discussed in relation to language exposure, facilitation from cross-linguistic overlap and maturation constraints on vocabulary size.

我们使用家长报告词汇问卷,比较了在英国(UK)长大的双语幼儿和年龄匹配的英国英语单语儿童(12-36个月大)在理解和生产方面的词汇大小。我们发现,双语幼儿的英语词汇量小于单语同龄人的词汇量。值得注意的是,当对理解中的概念词汇进行比较时,并没有发现这种词汇差距。概念评分也减少了生产中的词汇差距,但组间差异仍然显著。当两种语言的单词加在一起,既能理解又能产生时,双语幼儿比单语幼儿知道更多的单词。这种庞大的总词汇量可能归因于双语者词汇中双字(具有相同含义的跨语言单词对)的比例很高。这些发现与语言暴露、跨语言重叠的促进以及词汇大小的成熟限制有关。
{"title":"Double it up: Vocabulary size comparisons between UK bilingual and monolingual toddlers","authors":"Serene Siow,&nbsp;Nicola A. Gillen,&nbsp;Irina Lepădatu,&nbsp;Kim Plunkett","doi":"10.1111/infa.12562","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12562","url":null,"abstract":"<p>We compared vocabulary sizes in comprehension and production between bilingual toddlers growing up in the United Kingdom (UK) and age-matched UK English monolinguals (12–36 months old) using parent-report vocabulary questionnaires. We found that bilingual toddlers' vocabulary sizes in English were smaller than the vocabulary sizes of their monolingual peers. Notably, this vocabulary gap was not found when groups were compared on conceptual vocabulary in comprehension. Conceptual scoring also reduced the vocabulary gap in production but group differences were still significant. Bilingual toddlers knew more words than monolinguals when words across their two languages were added together, for both comprehension and production. This large total vocabulary size could be attributed to a high proportion of doublets (cross-linguistic word pairs with the same meaning) in bilinguals' vocabularies. These findings are discussed in relation to language exposure, facilitation from cross-linguistic overlap and maturation constraints on vocabulary size.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"28 6","pages":"1030-1051"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-10-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12562","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41173496","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Infancy
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1