首页 > 最新文献

Basic and Applied Social Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
The Sobering Effects of Jailhouse Informant Testimony on Perceptions of an Intoxicated Rape Victim 监狱线人证词对醉酒强奸受害者认知的清醒作用
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Pub Date : 2021-03-24 DOI: 10.1080/01973533.2021.1896520
A. L. Le Grand, Baylee D. Jenkins, J. Golding, J. Neuschatz, Andrea M. Pals, S. A. Wetmore
Abstract This study investigated the impact of jailhouse informant (JI) testimony on mock-jurors’ perceptions of an alcohol-facilitated rape trial. Male and female participants (N = 186) read a rape trial summary of an adult female after attending a concert. The trial varied whether the victim was intoxicated or sober and whether a JI testified that the defendant confessed to the rape. The results indicated that participants were more pro-victim (e.g., voted guilty more) when the JI testified than no JI testimony, and that women were more pro-victim than men. Although victim intoxication negatively impacted perceptions of the defendant, it did not influence verdicts. The results are discussed concerning the legal implications of presenting JI testimony and factors that influence perceptions of rape victims.
摘要本研究调查了监狱线人(JI)证词对模拟陪审员对酒精促成强奸案审判的看法的影响。男性和女性参与者(N = 186)在参加一场音乐会后宣读了一名成年女性的强奸案审判摘要。审判中,受害者是喝醉了还是清醒了,以及JI是否作证说被告承认强奸。结果表明,当联合执行作证时,参与者比没有联合执行作证的参与者更支持受害者(例如,投票有罪的人更多),女性比男性更支持受害者。尽管受害者醉酒对被告的认知产生了负面影响,但并不影响判决。讨论了提交联合执行证词的法律影响以及影响强奸受害者看法的因素。
{"title":"The Sobering Effects of Jailhouse Informant Testimony on Perceptions of an Intoxicated Rape Victim","authors":"A. L. Le Grand, Baylee D. Jenkins, J. Golding, J. Neuschatz, Andrea M. Pals, S. A. Wetmore","doi":"10.1080/01973533.2021.1896520","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2021.1896520","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This study investigated the impact of jailhouse informant (JI) testimony on mock-jurors’ perceptions of an alcohol-facilitated rape trial. Male and female participants (N = 186) read a rape trial summary of an adult female after attending a concert. The trial varied whether the victim was intoxicated or sober and whether a JI testified that the defendant confessed to the rape. The results indicated that participants were more pro-victim (e.g., voted guilty more) when the JI testified than no JI testimony, and that women were more pro-victim than men. Although victim intoxication negatively impacted perceptions of the defendant, it did not influence verdicts. The results are discussed concerning the legal implications of presenting JI testimony and factors that influence perceptions of rape victims.","PeriodicalId":48014,"journal":{"name":"Basic and Applied Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2021-03-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01973533.2021.1896520","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49145394","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Sustainable Consumption: What Works Best, Carbon Taxes, Subsidies and/or Nudges? 可持续消费:什么最有效,碳税,补贴和/或核能?
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Pub Date : 2021-03-17 DOI: 10.1080/01973533.2021.1889553
M. Osman, Pauline Schwartz, Saul Wodak
Abstract Behavioral change techniques may show positive changes to sustainable consumption, but as with many other domains, how they interact with other typical regulatory measures is unknown. To address the empirical lacuna, the present study uses a discrete-choice set-up to simulate a lunchtime canteen in order to investigate the effects of choice preserving and choice incentivizing interventions on meal choices. Carbon tax (Experiment 1, Experiment 2) alone, behavioral interventions (Experiment 1) alone, as well in combination (Experiment 1, Experiment 2) shifted choices to a less degree than in combination. The most compelling positive behavioral change was found when introducing a redistributive pricing system that combines carbon tax and subsidies (Experiment 2), in combination with choice preserving instruments (Experiment 2, Experiment 3).
抽象行为改变技术可能显示出可持续消费的积极变化,但与许多其他领域一样,它们如何与其他典型的监管措施相互作用尚不清楚。为了解决经验空白,本研究使用离散选择设置来模拟午餐食堂,以调查保留选择和激励选择干预对用餐选择的影响。单独的碳税(实验1、实验2)、单独的行为干预(实验1)以及组合(实验1和实验2)比组合在较小程度上改变了选择。最引人注目的积极行为变化是在引入将碳税和补贴相结合的再分配定价体系(实验2),以及保留选择的工具(实验2,实验3)时发现的。
{"title":"Sustainable Consumption: What Works Best, Carbon Taxes, Subsidies and/or Nudges?","authors":"M. Osman, Pauline Schwartz, Saul Wodak","doi":"10.1080/01973533.2021.1889553","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2021.1889553","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Behavioral change techniques may show positive changes to sustainable consumption, but as with many other domains, how they interact with other typical regulatory measures is unknown. To address the empirical lacuna, the present study uses a discrete-choice set-up to simulate a lunchtime canteen in order to investigate the effects of choice preserving and choice incentivizing interventions on meal choices. Carbon tax (Experiment 1, Experiment 2) alone, behavioral interventions (Experiment 1) alone, as well in combination (Experiment 1, Experiment 2) shifted choices to a less degree than in combination. The most compelling positive behavioral change was found when introducing a redistributive pricing system that combines carbon tax and subsidies (Experiment 2), in combination with choice preserving instruments (Experiment 2, Experiment 3).","PeriodicalId":48014,"journal":{"name":"Basic and Applied Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2021-03-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01973533.2021.1889553","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42378232","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
When It’s Bad to Be Lucky: Observers’ Judgments of Fortuitous Victims 运气不好的时候:观察者对幸运受害者的判断
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Pub Date : 2021-03-04 DOI: 10.1080/01973533.2020.1863797
Olof Wrede, Erik Mac Giolla
Abstract We framed crime victims as lucky, through downward counterfactual comparisons, and tested this “luck framing” influence on observers’ judgments of the victims. Victims framed as lucky and aware (Experiment 1) or unaware (Experiment 3) of their luck were rated as in need of less social support than victims who were not framed as lucky. This luck framing effect generalized to victim compensation: lucky aware victims were rated as deserving of less compensation compared to unlucky victims (Experiment 2). In contrast, luck framing of victims had little influence on observers’ judgments of the severity of the sentence a perpetrator should receive (Experiment 4). Taken together, for crime victims it can be bad to be thought of as lucky.
摘要我们通过向下的反事实比较,将犯罪受害者框定为幸运的,并测试了这种“幸运框”对观察者对受害者判断的影响。被诬陷为幸运并意识到(实验1)或不知道(实验3)自己的运气的受害者被评为需要比没有被诬陷幸运的受害者更少的社会支持。这种运气框架效应适用于受害者赔偿:与不幸的受害者相比,幸运的受害者被认为应该得到更少的赔偿(实验2)。相比之下,受害者的运气框架对观察者对犯罪者应该受到的判决的严重程度的判断几乎没有影响(实验4)。总之,对于犯罪受害者来说,被认为是幸运的可能是不好的。
{"title":"When It’s Bad to Be Lucky: Observers’ Judgments of Fortuitous Victims","authors":"Olof Wrede, Erik Mac Giolla","doi":"10.1080/01973533.2020.1863797","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2020.1863797","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract We framed crime victims as lucky, through downward counterfactual comparisons, and tested this “luck framing” influence on observers’ judgments of the victims. Victims framed as lucky and aware (Experiment 1) or unaware (Experiment 3) of their luck were rated as in need of less social support than victims who were not framed as lucky. This luck framing effect generalized to victim compensation: lucky aware victims were rated as deserving of less compensation compared to unlucky victims (Experiment 2). In contrast, luck framing of victims had little influence on observers’ judgments of the severity of the sentence a perpetrator should receive (Experiment 4). Taken together, for crime victims it can be bad to be thought of as lucky.","PeriodicalId":48014,"journal":{"name":"Basic and Applied Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2021-03-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01973533.2020.1863797","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44716012","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
COVID-19 Behavioral Health Mindset Inventory: A Method for Enhancing Employee and Consumer Safety COVID-19行为健康心态量表:提高员工和消费者安全的一种方法
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Pub Date : 2021-03-04 DOI: 10.1080/01973533.2021.1881520
M. Cunningham, P. B. Druen, A. Barbee, John W. Jones, Brian W. Dreschler
Abstract The COVID-19 Behavioral Health Mindset inventory (CVBHM) was designed to protect consumers and workers by assessing biosafety risk through acceptance of responsibility for the protection of oneself and others, social distancing, and adhering to prevention measures. The 30-item inventory was evaluated in two surveys of 1,455 (Time 1) and 431 respondents (Time 2). The CVBHM has strong internal consistency (T1 α = .90, T2 α = .88), test-retest reliability (r = .84) and predictive validity with the criterion of wearing a facemask (T1 β = .64, T2 β= .60). The CVBHM was a stronger predictor of safe behavior than other individual differences, although employment status, vocational interest, personality, and demographics were related. There was a drop in CVBHM scores over the study period, with predictable exceptions.
COVID-19行为健康心态量表(CVBHM)旨在通过接受保护自己和他人的责任、保持社交距离和坚持预防措施,评估消费者和工人的生物安全风险,以保护消费者和工人。采用1455人(时间1)和431人(时间2)对30项问卷进行评估。CVBHM具有较强的内部一致性(T1 α = 0.90, T2 α = 0.88)、重测信度(r = 0.84)和与佩戴口罩标准的预测效度(T1 β= 0.64, T2 β= 0.60)。尽管就业状况、职业兴趣、个性和人口统计学相关,但CVBHM比其他个体差异更能预测安全行为。在研究期间,CVBHM得分有所下降,但也有例外。
{"title":"COVID-19 Behavioral Health Mindset Inventory: A Method for Enhancing Employee and Consumer Safety","authors":"M. Cunningham, P. B. Druen, A. Barbee, John W. Jones, Brian W. Dreschler","doi":"10.1080/01973533.2021.1881520","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2021.1881520","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The COVID-19 Behavioral Health Mindset inventory (CVBHM) was designed to protect consumers and workers by assessing biosafety risk through acceptance of responsibility for the protection of oneself and others, social distancing, and adhering to prevention measures. The 30-item inventory was evaluated in two surveys of 1,455 (Time 1) and 431 respondents (Time 2). The CVBHM has strong internal consistency (T1 α = .90, T2 α = .88), test-retest reliability (r = .84) and predictive validity with the criterion of wearing a facemask (T1 β = .64, T2 β= .60). The CVBHM was a stronger predictor of safe behavior than other individual differences, although employment status, vocational interest, personality, and demographics were related. There was a drop in CVBHM scores over the study period, with predictable exceptions.","PeriodicalId":48014,"journal":{"name":"Basic and Applied Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2021-03-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01973533.2021.1881520","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41408199","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Qualitative Exploration of Chinese Students’ Perspectives on Long-Term Goal Striving 中国学生长期目标追求观的质性探索
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Pub Date : 2021-02-24 DOI: 10.1080/01973533.2021.1889552
K. Lam, Mingming Zhou
Abstract Many scholars have emphasized the role of passion and effort in successfully achieving goals, but others have argued that many factors can affect goal achievement. This qualitative study explored Chinese students’ perspectives on how one achieves long-term goals. We conducted individual, in-depth, semi-structured interviews with 13 Chinese university students to capture their past experiences of and perspectives on pursuing long-term goals. A new model in understanding Chinese students’ long-term goal striving was proposed with five elements identified: persistent effort, adaptability, self-confidence, self-talk, and social support. Findings from this study enriched grit research by bringing attention to the role of culture in the conceptualization of goal striving. The theoretical and practical implications of the findings are discussed.
摘要许多学者强调激情和努力在成功实现目标中的作用,但也有人认为许多因素会影响目标的实现。这项定性研究探讨了中国学生对如何实现长期目标的看法。我们对13名中国大学生进行了个人、深入、半结构化的访谈,以捕捉他们过去追求长期目标的经历和观点。提出了一种理解中国学生长期目标奋斗的新模式,确定了五个要素:坚持不懈的努力、适应性、自信、自言自语和社会支持。这项研究的发现通过关注文化在目标追求概念化中的作用,丰富了砂砾研究。讨论了研究结果的理论和实践意义。
{"title":"Qualitative Exploration of Chinese Students’ Perspectives on Long-Term Goal Striving","authors":"K. Lam, Mingming Zhou","doi":"10.1080/01973533.2021.1889552","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2021.1889552","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Many scholars have emphasized the role of passion and effort in successfully achieving goals, but others have argued that many factors can affect goal achievement. This qualitative study explored Chinese students’ perspectives on how one achieves long-term goals. We conducted individual, in-depth, semi-structured interviews with 13 Chinese university students to capture their past experiences of and perspectives on pursuing long-term goals. A new model in understanding Chinese students’ long-term goal striving was proposed with five elements identified: persistent effort, adaptability, self-confidence, self-talk, and social support. Findings from this study enriched grit research by bringing attention to the role of culture in the conceptualization of goal striving. The theoretical and practical implications of the findings are discussed.","PeriodicalId":48014,"journal":{"name":"Basic and Applied Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2021-02-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01973533.2021.1889552","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45545409","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Meta-Analytic Analysis of Invariance Across Samples: Introducing a Method That Does Not Require Raw Data 跨样本不变性的元分析:引入一种不需要原始数据的方法
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Pub Date : 2020-11-09 DOI: 10.1080/01973533.2020.1843461
A. af Wåhlberg, G. Madison, U. Aasa, Jeong Jin Yu
Abstract Invariance of surveys across different groups means that the respondents interpret the items in the same way, as reflected in similar factor loadings, for example. Invariance can be assessed using various statistical procedures, such as Multi-Group Confirmatory Factor Analysis. However, these analyses require access to raw data. Here, we introduce a meta-analytic method that requires only the factor correlation matrices of samples as input. It compares the structures of intercorrelations of factors by correlating these values across two samples, yielding a value of overall similarity for how the factors intercorrelate in different samples. This method was tested in three different ways. We conclude that the method yields useful results and can assess invariance when raw data are not available.
不同群体间调查的不变性意味着受访者以相同的方式解释项目,例如,反映在相似的因素负荷中。不变性可以使用各种统计程序进行评估,例如多组验证性因子分析。然而,这些分析需要访问原始数据。在这里,我们引入一种元分析方法,只需要样本的因子相关矩阵作为输入。它通过在两个样本中关联这些值来比较因素相互关系的结构,从而得出不同样本中因素相互关系的总体相似性值。这个方法用三种不同的方式进行了测试。我们得出结论,该方法产生了有用的结果,并且可以在没有原始数据时评估不变性。
{"title":"Meta-Analytic Analysis of Invariance Across Samples: Introducing a Method That Does Not Require Raw Data","authors":"A. af Wåhlberg, G. Madison, U. Aasa, Jeong Jin Yu","doi":"10.1080/01973533.2020.1843461","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2020.1843461","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Invariance of surveys across different groups means that the respondents interpret the items in the same way, as reflected in similar factor loadings, for example. Invariance can be assessed using various statistical procedures, such as Multi-Group Confirmatory Factor Analysis. However, these analyses require access to raw data. Here, we introduce a meta-analytic method that requires only the factor correlation matrices of samples as input. It compares the structures of intercorrelations of factors by correlating these values across two samples, yielding a value of overall similarity for how the factors intercorrelate in different samples. This method was tested in three different ways. We conclude that the method yields useful results and can assess invariance when raw data are not available.","PeriodicalId":48014,"journal":{"name":"Basic and Applied Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2020-11-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01973533.2020.1843461","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44933222","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does Repetition Always Make Perfect? Differential Effects of Repetition on Learning of Own-Race and Other-Race Faces 重复总是完美的吗?重复对本种族和其他种族面孔学习的差异效应
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Pub Date : 2020-11-05 DOI: 10.1080/01973533.2020.1843462
Tomás A. Palma, L. Garcia-Marques
Abstract People have a remarkable capacity to process and recognize faces. Yet, they fail to recognize the faces of individuals from other racial groups - the Other-Race Effect (ORE). We investigated the role of repetition - a powerful determinant of learning and memory - in reducing the ORE. We predicted that repetition would improve face learning, particularly for other-race faces, as these are poorly learned based on a single presentation. Because own-race faces are easily learned based on a unique presentation, they should benefit less from repetition. We tested this hypothesis across five experiments. Results showed that repetition not only did not reduce the ORE, but instead, it increased it. We discuss the theoretical implications of these findings for the ORE.
人类具有非凡的处理和识别面孔的能力。然而,他们无法识别来自其他种族群体的人的面孔-其他种族效应(ORE)。我们研究了重复的作用——学习和记忆的一个强大的决定因素——在减少ORE方面的作用。我们预测重复会提高面孔学习,特别是对其他种族的面孔,因为这些面孔是基于单一的呈现而学习的。因为自己种族的面孔很容易通过独特的呈现来学习,所以他们从重复中获益较少。我们通过五个实验验证了这个假设。结果表明,重复不仅没有降低ORE,反而增加了ORE。我们讨论了这些发现对矿石的理论意义。
{"title":"Does Repetition Always Make Perfect? Differential Effects of Repetition on Learning of Own-Race and Other-Race Faces","authors":"Tomás A. Palma, L. Garcia-Marques","doi":"10.1080/01973533.2020.1843462","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2020.1843462","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract People have a remarkable capacity to process and recognize faces. Yet, they fail to recognize the faces of individuals from other racial groups - the Other-Race Effect (ORE). We investigated the role of repetition - a powerful determinant of learning and memory - in reducing the ORE. We predicted that repetition would improve face learning, particularly for other-race faces, as these are poorly learned based on a single presentation. Because own-race faces are easily learned based on a unique presentation, they should benefit less from repetition. We tested this hypothesis across five experiments. Results showed that repetition not only did not reduce the ORE, but instead, it increased it. We discuss the theoretical implications of these findings for the ORE.","PeriodicalId":48014,"journal":{"name":"Basic and Applied Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2020-11-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01973533.2020.1843462","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45011973","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Multiple Feet-in-the-Door and Obedience 多脚进门和服从
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Pub Date : 2020-10-22 DOI: 10.1080/01973533.2020.1837134
T. Grzyb, D. Doliński
Abstract Gilbert’s hypothesis regarding the possible effect of the feet-in-the-door procedure on obedience to an authority figure in Milgram’s paradigm was tested in the course of two studies. Neither the first experiment, conducted in a laboratory (N = 80), which was a true copy of the model proposed by Milgram, nor the second study, conducted online (N = 485), validated Gilbert’s hypothesis. Actually, the results demonstrated the opposite–fewer of those subjects who were asked to shock the “learner” with high voltage straight away refused to follow the order than those who reached the same voltage level gradually. In Study 2, we also tested the hypothesis regarding the role of a postponement as a factor in decreasing one’s obedience.
摘要通过两项研究验证了吉尔伯特关于米尔格拉姆范式中“脚在门上”过程对服从权威人物可能产生影响的假设。第一个实验是在实验室进行的(N = 80),这是Milgram提出的模型的真实副本,第二个研究是在网上进行的(N = 485),都没有验证吉尔伯特的假设。事实上,结果恰恰相反——那些被要求立即用高电压电击“学习者”的人比那些逐渐达到相同电压水平的人更少拒绝服从命令。在研究2中,我们还检验了关于延迟作为降低一个人服从的因素的作用的假设。
{"title":"Multiple Feet-in-the-Door and Obedience","authors":"T. Grzyb, D. Doliński","doi":"10.1080/01973533.2020.1837134","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2020.1837134","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Gilbert’s hypothesis regarding the possible effect of the feet-in-the-door procedure on obedience to an authority figure in Milgram’s paradigm was tested in the course of two studies. Neither the first experiment, conducted in a laboratory (N = 80), which was a true copy of the model proposed by Milgram, nor the second study, conducted online (N = 485), validated Gilbert’s hypothesis. Actually, the results demonstrated the opposite–fewer of those subjects who were asked to shock the “learner” with high voltage straight away refused to follow the order than those who reached the same voltage level gradually. In Study 2, we also tested the hypothesis regarding the role of a postponement as a factor in decreasing one’s obedience.","PeriodicalId":48014,"journal":{"name":"Basic and Applied Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2020-10-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01973533.2020.1837134","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47824118","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
To Apologize or Justify: Leader Responses to Task and Relational Mistakes 道歉或辩护:领导者对任务和关系错误的反应
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Pub Date : 2020-10-19 DOI: 10.1080/01973533.2020.1828083
A. Hetrick, Lily D. Blocker, Joshua Fairchild, Samuel T. Hunter
Abstract Leadership roles are complex, creating an environment where leaders are likely to make mistakes that result in negative outcomes. We shift the conversation in the literature from examining the effectiveness of mistake responses toward exploring why leaders use different mistake recovery methods. We employ an online experimental method to distinguish between task and relationship mistakes for leaders and suggest that different attributions are made for these two types of mistakes (Study 1). We found that task mistakes are viewed by leaders as more specific and less personal, and that relationship mistakes are viewed as more global, describing the leader’s stable characteristics rather than a specific event. From these findings, we predict that leaders are more likely to apologize for task mistakes and are more likely to justify their relationship mistakes rather than admit wrongdoing for them. We find support for these predictions using a mixed methods approach, employing a laboratory experiment (Study 2) and a critical incident method surveying a panel of leaders (Study 3). As such, this paper extends our understanding of the mistake recovery process for leaders by demonstrating that 1) leaders’ relationship mistakes are viewed as more globally representative of the leader than task mistakes, and 2) leaders are more likely to apologize for task mistakes but more likely to justify relationship mistakes. Relational and task mistakes, however, were not found to be different in regard to their ambiguity or the extent to which they were viewed as a mistake.
摘要领导角色是复杂的,在这种环境中,领导者很可能会犯错误,从而导致负面结果。我们将文献中的对话从研究错误反应的有效性转向探索领导者为什么使用不同的错误恢复方法。我们采用在线实验方法来区分领导者的任务错误和关系错误,并建议对这两种类型的错误进行不同的归因(研究1)。我们发现,领导者认为任务错误更具体,不那么个人化,而关系错误则更具全局性,描述的是领导者的稳定特征,而不是特定事件。根据这些发现,我们预测,领导者更有可能为任务错误道歉,更有可能为他们的关系错误辩护,而不是为他们承认错误。我们使用混合方法,采用实验室实验(研究2)和调查领导小组的关键事件方法(研究3),找到了对这些预测的支持。因此,本文通过证明1)领导者的关系错误比任务错误更具全球代表性,2)领导者更有可能为任务错误道歉,但更有可能为关系错误辩护,从而扩展了我们对领导者错误恢复过程的理解。然而,在关系错误和任务错误的模糊性或被视为错误的程度方面,并没有发现它们的不同。
{"title":"To Apologize or Justify: Leader Responses to Task and Relational Mistakes","authors":"A. Hetrick, Lily D. Blocker, Joshua Fairchild, Samuel T. Hunter","doi":"10.1080/01973533.2020.1828083","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2020.1828083","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Leadership roles are complex, creating an environment where leaders are likely to make mistakes that result in negative outcomes. We shift the conversation in the literature from examining the effectiveness of mistake responses toward exploring why leaders use different mistake recovery methods. We employ an online experimental method to distinguish between task and relationship mistakes for leaders and suggest that different attributions are made for these two types of mistakes (Study 1). We found that task mistakes are viewed by leaders as more specific and less personal, and that relationship mistakes are viewed as more global, describing the leader’s stable characteristics rather than a specific event. From these findings, we predict that leaders are more likely to apologize for task mistakes and are more likely to justify their relationship mistakes rather than admit wrongdoing for them. We find support for these predictions using a mixed methods approach, employing a laboratory experiment (Study 2) and a critical incident method surveying a panel of leaders (Study 3). As such, this paper extends our understanding of the mistake recovery process for leaders by demonstrating that 1) leaders’ relationship mistakes are viewed as more globally representative of the leader than task mistakes, and 2) leaders are more likely to apologize for task mistakes but more likely to justify relationship mistakes. Relational and task mistakes, however, were not found to be different in regard to their ambiguity or the extent to which they were viewed as a mistake.","PeriodicalId":48014,"journal":{"name":"Basic and Applied Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2020-10-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01973533.2020.1828083","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44298309","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Ambivalent Classism: The Importance of Assessing Hostile and Benevolent Ideologies about Poor People 二元古典主义:评价穷人的敌对和仁爱思想的重要性
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Pub Date : 2020-10-05 DOI: 10.1080/01973533.2020.1828084
J. Jordan, Joanna Lawler, J. Bosson
Abstract We present a model of ambivalent classism in which hostile (overtly negative and insulting) and benevolent (subjectively positive but condescending) attitudes about poor people co-exist and independently predict endorsement of restrictive and poverty-perpetuating welfare policies. Whereas existing classism scales predominantly measure antipathy toward poor people, we developed and validated the Ambivalent Classism Inventory (ACI), a 20-item scale that captures both hostile and benevolent attitudes toward poor people. The ACI has one hostile factor (hostile classism) and two benevolent factors (protective paternalism and complementary class differentiation). Data from four samples (total n = 1,543) indicate that the ACI has good reliability and validity. Findings underscore the role of benevolent beliefs in classist attitudes. Item generation, analytical methods, and implications are discussed.
我们提出了一个矛盾的阶级主义模型,其中对穷人的敌对(公开消极和侮辱)和仁慈(主观上积极但居高临下)态度并存,并独立地预测了限制性和持续贫困的福利政策的支持。鉴于现有的阶级歧视量表主要衡量对穷人的反感,我们开发并验证了矛盾阶级歧视量表(ACI),这是一个20个项目的量表,捕捉了对穷人的敌意和仁慈态度。ACI有一个敌对因素(敌对阶级主义)和两个友好因素(保护性家长制和互补的阶级分化)。四个样本(共1543个)的数据表明,ACI具有良好的信度和效度。研究结果强调了仁慈信仰在阶级主义态度中的作用。讨论了项目生成、分析方法和含义。
{"title":"Ambivalent Classism: The Importance of Assessing Hostile and Benevolent Ideologies about Poor People","authors":"J. Jordan, Joanna Lawler, J. Bosson","doi":"10.1080/01973533.2020.1828084","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2020.1828084","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract We present a model of ambivalent classism in which hostile (overtly negative and insulting) and benevolent (subjectively positive but condescending) attitudes about poor people co-exist and independently predict endorsement of restrictive and poverty-perpetuating welfare policies. Whereas existing classism scales predominantly measure antipathy toward poor people, we developed and validated the Ambivalent Classism Inventory (ACI), a 20-item scale that captures both hostile and benevolent attitudes toward poor people. The ACI has one hostile factor (hostile classism) and two benevolent factors (protective paternalism and complementary class differentiation). Data from four samples (total n = 1,543) indicate that the ACI has good reliability and validity. Findings underscore the role of benevolent beliefs in classist attitudes. Item generation, analytical methods, and implications are discussed.","PeriodicalId":48014,"journal":{"name":"Basic and Applied Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2020-10-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/01973533.2020.1828084","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42196224","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
期刊
Basic and Applied Social Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1