首页 > 最新文献

Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung最新文献

英文 中文
How well can young adults and children discriminate between odors? 青壮年和儿童分辨气味的能力如何?
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-19 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-01981-6
E Leslie Cameron, Sierra R Follett, Autumn S Rajcevich Schwer

It is widely believed that people can distinguish between many odors although there is limited empirical evidence. Odor discrimination tasks are employed much less often than other measures of olfaction, but, interestingly, performance is typically ~ 75% correct. This less-than-perfect performance is rarely highlighted, although it suggests that people may not be as good at discriminating odors as is commonly believed. Odor discrimination is understudied in children, and although available evidence suggests that it improves with age, children perform better when the task is simpler. In the present study, we explored odor discrimination in children and young adults with a relatively simple same-different task using common and uncommon odors. We found that children perform as well as young adults, but that overall performance was less than perfect and depended on the odors to be discriminated. We found evidence that ability to discriminate between odors improves as the difference in pleasantness of the odors increases. In a second experiment, we tested this directly by exploring whether odors that differ in pleasantness and edibility, two dimensions that appear to be important in olfactory perception, are easier to discriminate than odors that are the same on those dimensions. We found further evidence that odors that differ in pleasantness are easier to discriminate.

尽管经验证据有限,但人们普遍认为人可以区分多种气味。与其他嗅觉测量方法相比,气味辨别任务的使用频率要低得多,但有趣的是,正确率通常在 75% 左右。这种不尽如人意的表现很少被强调,但它表明人们对气味的辨别能力可能并不像人们通常认为的那样好。对儿童气味辨别能力的研究不足,尽管现有证据表明,儿童的辨别能力会随着年龄的增长而提高,但当任务较为简单时,儿童的表现会更好。在本研究中,我们通过一个相对简单的使用常见和不常见气味的 "同-异 "任务,对儿童和青少年的气味辨别能力进行了探索。我们发现,儿童的表现与青壮年一样好,但总体表现并不完美,这取决于要辨别的气味。我们发现有证据表明,对不同气味的辨别能力会随着气味的愉悦程度差异的增加而提高。在第二个实验中,我们直接测试了这一点,探讨了在宜人性和可食性这两个似乎对嗅觉感知很重要的维度上存在差异的气味是否比在这两个维度上相同的气味更容易分辨。我们发现有进一步的证据表明,愉悦度不同的气味更容易辨别。
{"title":"How well can young adults and children discriminate between odors?","authors":"E Leslie Cameron, Sierra R Follett, Autumn S Rajcevich Schwer","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-01981-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-01981-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is widely believed that people can distinguish between many odors although there is limited empirical evidence. Odor discrimination tasks are employed much less often than other measures of olfaction, but, interestingly, performance is typically ~ 75% correct. This less-than-perfect performance is rarely highlighted, although it suggests that people may not be as good at discriminating odors as is commonly believed. Odor discrimination is understudied in children, and although available evidence suggests that it improves with age, children perform better when the task is simpler. In the present study, we explored odor discrimination in children and young adults with a relatively simple same-different task using common and uncommon odors. We found that children perform as well as young adults, but that overall performance was less than perfect and depended on the odors to be discriminated. We found evidence that ability to discriminate between odors improves as the difference in pleasantness of the odors increases. In a second experiment, we tested this directly by exploring whether odors that differ in pleasantness and edibility, two dimensions that appear to be important in olfactory perception, are easier to discriminate than odors that are the same on those dimensions. We found further evidence that odors that differ in pleasantness are easier to discriminate.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2360-2375"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141421461","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Strategy variability in computational estimation and its association with mathematical achievement. 计算估算中的策略变化及其与数学成绩的关系。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-14 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02008-w
Reba Koenen, Sashank Varma

Computational estimation requires a breadth of strategies and selection of the relevant strategy given a problem's features. We used the new Test of Estimation Strategies (TES), composed of 20 arithmetic problems (e.g., 144 x 0.38), to investigate variability in strategy use in young adults. The TES targets the five estimation strategies that adults use most frequently, which fall into two Classes. The three Class One strategies are general-purpose and taught in schools. Proceed Algorithmically entails applying an algorithm (e.g., shifting a decimal place). Round One and Round Two are defined as rounding one or both operands, respectively. The two Class Two strategies are more advanced, requiring application of conceptual knowledge of mathematics. Known-and-Nice is used when a participant relies on a well-known mathematical fact (e.g., 25 × 4 = 100) to form an estimate. Fractions uses a fraction or percentage in the estimation process (e.g., 943 x 0.48 is about 50% or half of 900). We divided our sample of adult participants into two groups (i.e., high, average) based on their estimation performance on the TES. The high-performance group used a broader range of strategies and more frequently applied the most relevant strategy given a problem's features. Overall estimation accuracy was correlated with mathematical achievement, as were strategy breadth and strategy relevance. However, none of these associations survived first controlling for verbal achievement. Participants' strategy reports suggested that the TES problems were generally successful in eliciting the five target strategies and provided evidence for a new strategy, Partitioning. These findings provide a basis for future instructional studies to improve students' computational estimation.

计算估算需要多种策略,并根据问题的特征选择相关策略。我们使用了由 20 个算术问题(如 144 x 0.38)组成的新估算策略测试 (TES),以研究青壮年在策略使用方面的变异性。估算策略测试针对成年人最常用的五种估算策略,这些策略分为两类。第一类的三种策略是通用策略,在学校中教授。按照算法进行估算需要应用一种算法(例如,移动一位小数)。四舍五入(Round One)和四舍五入(Round Two)分别指对一个操作数或两个操作数进行四舍五入。第二类的两种策略更高级,需要应用数学概念知识。当学员根据一个众所周知的数学事实(如 25 × 4 = 100)来进行估算时,就会使用 "已知并不错 "策略。分数在估算过程中使用分数或百分比(例如,943 x 0.48 大约是 900 的 50%或一半)。我们根据成人参与者在 TES 中的估算成绩将其分为两组(即高分组和普通组)。成绩优秀组使用的策略范围更广,并且更经常地根据问题的特点使用最相关的策略。总体估算准确率与数学成绩相关,策略广度和策略相关性也与数学成绩相关。然而,在控制了言语成绩之后,这些相关性都不复存在。参与者的策略报告表明,TES 问题总体上成功地激发了五种目标策略,并为一种新策略--"分割 "提供了证据。这些发现为今后提高学生计算估算能力的教学研究提供了依据。
{"title":"Strategy variability in computational estimation and its association with mathematical achievement.","authors":"Reba Koenen, Sashank Varma","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02008-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02008-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Computational estimation requires a breadth of strategies and selection of the relevant strategy given a problem's features. We used the new Test of Estimation Strategies (TES), composed of 20 arithmetic problems (e.g., 144 x 0.38), to investigate variability in strategy use in young adults. The TES targets the five estimation strategies that adults use most frequently, which fall into two Classes. The three Class One strategies are general-purpose and taught in schools. Proceed Algorithmically entails applying an algorithm (e.g., shifting a decimal place). Round One and Round Two are defined as rounding one or both operands, respectively. The two Class Two strategies are more advanced, requiring application of conceptual knowledge of mathematics. Known-and-Nice is used when a participant relies on a well-known mathematical fact (e.g., 25 × 4 = 100) to form an estimate. Fractions uses a fraction or percentage in the estimation process (e.g., 943 x 0.48 is about 50% or half of 900). We divided our sample of adult participants into two groups (i.e., high, average) based on their estimation performance on the TES. The high-performance group used a broader range of strategies and more frequently applied the most relevant strategy given a problem's features. Overall estimation accuracy was correlated with mathematical achievement, as were strategy breadth and strategy relevance. However, none of these associations survived first controlling for verbal achievement. Participants' strategy reports suggested that the TES problems were generally successful in eliciting the five target strategies and provided evidence for a new strategy, Partitioning. These findings provide a basis for future instructional studies to improve students' computational estimation.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2303-2319"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141976895","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The association between physical activity and memory interference. 体育锻炼与记忆干扰之间的关系
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-24 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02021-z
Paul D Loprinzi, Myungjin Jung, Monika Undorf

Habitual physical activity has been shown to improve memory performance, yet investigations into its effects concerning memory interference remain limited. Additionally, minimal research has evaluated the association between habitual physical activity behaviors occurring in different contexts (e.g., walking, basketball, swimming) and memory. Based on these gaps in the literature, the present set of six experiments evaluated the association between contextually-different physical activity behaviors (e.g., individual physical activities, physical activities performed in social settings) and memory interference among young adult samples from America and Germany. Across six experiments, we reliably demonstrated that Germans exhibited greater memory performance than Americans. We also reliably demonstrated that contextually-different physical activities are not associated with memory performance or attenuated memory interference.

习惯性体育锻炼已被证明能提高记忆力,但对其对记忆干扰的影响的研究仍然有限。此外,很少有研究对不同情境下的习惯性体育锻炼行为(如散步、篮球、游泳)与记忆之间的关联进行评估。基于这些文献空白,本组六项实验评估了来自美国和德国的年轻成人样本中不同情境下的体育活动行为(如个人体育活动、在社交场合进行的体育活动)与记忆干扰之间的关联。通过六项实验,我们可靠地证明德国人比美国人表现出更强的记忆能力。我们还可靠地证明,不同情境下的体育活动与记忆表现或记忆干扰的减弱无关。
{"title":"The association between physical activity and memory interference.","authors":"Paul D Loprinzi, Myungjin Jung, Monika Undorf","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02021-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02021-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Habitual physical activity has been shown to improve memory performance, yet investigations into its effects concerning memory interference remain limited. Additionally, minimal research has evaluated the association between habitual physical activity behaviors occurring in different contexts (e.g., walking, basketball, swimming) and memory. Based on these gaps in the literature, the present set of six experiments evaluated the association between contextually-different physical activity behaviors (e.g., individual physical activities, physical activities performed in social settings) and memory interference among young adult samples from America and Germany. Across six experiments, we reliably demonstrated that Germans exhibited greater memory performance than Americans. We also reliably demonstrated that contextually-different physical activities are not associated with memory performance or attenuated memory interference.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2280-2291"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142047313","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Altered temporal awareness during Covid-19 pandemic. Covid-19 大流行期间的时间意识改变。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-22 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02004-0
Şerife Leman Runyun, Virginie van Wassenhove, Fuat Balci

Social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic had profound effects on human well-being. A handful of studies have focused on how time perception was altered during the COVID-19 pandemic, while no study has tested whether temporal metacognition is also affected by the lockdown. We examined the impact of long-term social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic on the ability to monitor errors in timing performance. We recruited 1232 participants from 12 countries during lockdown, 211 of which were retested "post-pandemic" for within-group comparisons. We also tested a new group of 331 participants during the "post-pandemic" period and compared their data to those of 1232 participants tested during the lockdown (between-group comparison). Participants produced a 3600 ms target interval and assessed the magnitude and direction of their time production error. Both within and between-group comparisons showed reduced metric error monitoring performance during the lockdown, even after controlling for government-imposed stringency indices. A higher level of reported social isolation also predicted reduced temporal error monitoring ability. Participants produced longer duration during lockdown compared to post-lockdown (again controlling for government stringency indices). We reason that these effects may be underlain by altered biological and behavioral rhythms during social isolation experienced during the COVID-19 pandemic. Understanding these effects is crucial for a more complete characterization of the cognitive consequences of long-term social isolation.

COVID-19 大流行期间的社会隔离对人类福祉产生了深远影响。少数研究关注的是 COVID-19 大流行期间时间感知是如何改变的,但还没有研究测试时间元认知是否也受到封锁的影响。我们研究了 COVID-19 大流行期间长期社会隔离对监测计时错误能力的影响。在封锁期间,我们从 12 个国家招募了 1232 名参与者,其中 211 人在 "大流行后 "接受了重新测试,以进行组内比较。我们还在 "大流行后 "期间测试了一组新的 331 名参与者,并将他们的数据与封锁期间测试的 1232 名参与者的数据进行了比较(组间比较)。参与者制作了一个 3600 毫秒的目标时间间隔,并对其时间制作误差的大小和方向进行了评估。组内比较和组间比较均显示,即使在控制了政府规定的严格指数后,封锁期间的度量误差监测表现仍有所下降。较高的社会隔离水平也预示着较低的时间误差监测能力。与封锁后相比,参与者在封锁期间产生了更长的持续时间(同样控制了政府的严格指数)。我们推断,这些影响可能是由于 COVID-19 大流行期间经历的社会隔离改变了生物和行为节律。了解这些影响对于更全面地描述长期社会隔离的认知后果至关重要。
{"title":"Altered temporal awareness during Covid-19 pandemic.","authors":"Şerife Leman Runyun, Virginie van Wassenhove, Fuat Balci","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02004-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02004-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic had profound effects on human well-being. A handful of studies have focused on how time perception was altered during the COVID-19 pandemic, while no study has tested whether temporal metacognition is also affected by the lockdown. We examined the impact of long-term social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic on the ability to monitor errors in timing performance. We recruited 1232 participants from 12 countries during lockdown, 211 of which were retested \"post-pandemic\" for within-group comparisons. We also tested a new group of 331 participants during the \"post-pandemic\" period and compared their data to those of 1232 participants tested during the lockdown (between-group comparison). Participants produced a 3600 ms target interval and assessed the magnitude and direction of their time production error. Both within and between-group comparisons showed reduced metric error monitoring performance during the lockdown, even after controlling for government-imposed stringency indices. A higher level of reported social isolation also predicted reduced temporal error monitoring ability. Participants produced longer duration during lockdown compared to post-lockdown (again controlling for government stringency indices). We reason that these effects may be underlain by altered biological and behavioral rhythms during social isolation experienced during the COVID-19 pandemic. Understanding these effects is crucial for a more complete characterization of the cognitive consequences of long-term social isolation.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2335-2345"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141735348","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Erroneous saccade co-execution during manual action control is independent of oculomotor stimulus-response translation ease. 在手动动作控制过程中,错误的囊回共同执行与眼动刺激-反应转换的难易程度无关。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-30 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-01989-y
Jens Kürten, Tim Raettig, Lynn Huestegge

Recent multiple action control studies have demonstrated difficulties with single-action (vs. dual-action) execution when accompanied by the requirement to inhibit a prepotent additional response (e.g., a highly automatic eye movement). Such a dual-action performance benefit is typically characterized by frequent false-positive executions of the currently unwarranted response. Here, we investigated whether the frequency of false-positive saccades is affected by the ease of translating a stimulus into a spatial oculomotor response (S-R translation ease): Is it harder to inhibit a saccade that is more automatically triggered via the stimulus? Participants switched on a trial-by-trial basis between executing a single saccade, a single manual button press, and a saccadic-manual dual action in response to a single visual stimulus. Importantly, we employed three different stimulus modes that varied in oculomotor S-R translation ease (peripheral square > central arrow > central shape). The hierarchy of S-R translation ease was reflected by increasing saccade and manual reaction times. Critically, however, the frequency of false-positive saccades in single manual trials was not substantially affected by the stimulus mode. Our results rule out explanations related to limited capacity sharing (between inhibitory control and S-R translation demands) as well as accounts related to the time available for the completion of saccade inhibition. Instead, the findings suggest that the erroneous co-activation of the oculomotor system was elicited by the mere execution of a (frequently associated) manual response (action-based co-activation).

最近的多动作控制研究表明,当要求抑制一个先行的附加反应(如高度自动的眼球运动)时,单动作(相对于双动作)执行就会遇到困难。这种双重行动的性能优势通常表现为对当前不需要的反应的频繁假阳性执行。在此,我们研究了将刺激转化为空间眼球运动反应的难易程度(S-R 转化难易程度)是否会影响假阳性囊回视的频率:通过刺激自动触发的囊回是否更难抑制?受试者在逐次试验的基础上,针对单个视觉刺激在执行单次囊闪、单次手动按键和囊闪-手动双重动作之间进行切换。重要的是,我们采用了三种不同的刺激模式,它们的眼动 S-R 转换难易程度各不相同(外围方形 > 中央箭头 > 中央形状)。S-R转换难易程度的高低通过增加囊回和手动反应时间来反映。但重要的是,单次手动试验中的假阳性囊回转频率并没有受到刺激模式的实质性影响。我们的结果排除了与有限的能力共享(抑制控制和 S-R 翻译需求之间)有关的解释,也排除了与完成囊闪抑制的可用时间有关的解释。相反,研究结果表明,眼球运动系统的错误共同激活是由仅仅执行(经常相关的)手动反应(基于动作的共同激活)引起的。
{"title":"Erroneous saccade co-execution during manual action control is independent of oculomotor stimulus-response translation ease.","authors":"Jens Kürten, Tim Raettig, Lynn Huestegge","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-01989-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-01989-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent multiple action control studies have demonstrated difficulties with single-action (vs. dual-action) execution when accompanied by the requirement to inhibit a prepotent additional response (e.g., a highly automatic eye movement). Such a dual-action performance benefit is typically characterized by frequent false-positive executions of the currently unwarranted response. Here, we investigated whether the frequency of false-positive saccades is affected by the ease of translating a stimulus into a spatial oculomotor response (S-R translation ease): Is it harder to inhibit a saccade that is more automatically triggered via the stimulus? Participants switched on a trial-by-trial basis between executing a single saccade, a single manual button press, and a saccadic-manual dual action in response to a single visual stimulus. Importantly, we employed three different stimulus modes that varied in oculomotor S-R translation ease (peripheral square > central arrow > central shape). The hierarchy of S-R translation ease was reflected by increasing saccade and manual reaction times. Critically, however, the frequency of false-positive saccades in single manual trials was not substantially affected by the stimulus mode. Our results rule out explanations related to limited capacity sharing (between inhibitory control and S-R translation demands) as well as accounts related to the time available for the completion of saccade inhibition. Instead, the findings suggest that the erroneous co-activation of the oculomotor system was elicited by the mere execution of a (frequently associated) manual response (action-based co-activation).</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2376-2388"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11522101/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141856838","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of task measurements on sequential dependence: a comparison between temporal reproduction and discrimination tasks. 任务测量对顺序依赖性的影响:时间再现和辨别任务的比较。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-27 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02023-x
Si Cheng, Siyi Chen, Xuefeng Yang, Zhuanghua Shi

Decisions about a current stimulus are influenced by previously encountered stimuli, leading to sequential bias. However, the specific processing levels at which serial dependence emerges remain unclear. Despite considerable evidence pointing to contributions from perceptual and post-perceptual processes, as well as response carryover effects impacting subsequent judgments, research into how different task measurements affect sequential dependencies is limited. To address this gap, the present study investigated the role of task type in shaping sequential effects in time perception, employing a random-dot kinematogram (RDK) in a post-cue paradigm. Participants had to remember both the duration and the direction of the RDK movement and perform the task based on a post-cue, which was equally likely to be direction or duration. To delineate the task type, we employed the temporal bisection task in Experiment 1 and the duration reproduction task in Experiment 2. Both experiments revealed a significant sequential bias: durations were perceived as longer following longer previous durations, and vice versa. Intriguingly, the sequential effect was enhanced in the reproduction task following the same reproduction task (Experiment 2), but did not show significant variation by the task type in the bisection task (Experiment 1). Moreover, comparable response carryover effects were observed across two experiments. We argue that the differential impacts of task types on sequential dependence lies in the involvement of memory reactivation process in the decision stage, while the post-decision response carryover effect may reflect the assimilation by subjective, rather than objective, durations, potentially linking to the sticky pacemaker rate and/or decisional inertia.

关于当前刺激的决定会受到之前遇到的刺激的影响,从而导致序列偏差。然而,序列依赖出现的具体处理水平仍不清楚。尽管有大量证据表明,知觉和后知觉过程以及影响后续判断的反应带入效应会对序列依赖性产生影响,但有关不同任务测量如何影响序列依赖性的研究却十分有限。为了填补这一空白,本研究采用随机点运动图(RDK)的后提示范式,调查了任务类型在时间感知中形成序列效应的作用。参与者必须同时记住随机点运动图运动的持续时间和方向,并根据后提示完成任务。为了区分任务类型,我们在实验 1 中采用了时间分割任务,在实验 2 中采用了时长再现任务。这两项实验都发现了明显的顺序偏差:在前一个持续时间较长时,后一个持续时间也会被认为较长,反之亦然。耐人寻味的是,在同一再现任务(实验 2)之后的再现任务中,顺序效应增强了,但在分段任务(实验 1)中,任务类型的不同并没有显示出明显的差异。此外,在两个实验中还观察到了类似的反应携带效应。我们认为,任务类型对顺序依赖性的不同影响在于决策阶段记忆再激活过程的参与,而决策后的反应带入效应可能反映了主观而非客观持续时间的同化,可能与粘性起搏器速率和/或决策惰性有关。
{"title":"The impact of task measurements on sequential dependence: a comparison between temporal reproduction and discrimination tasks.","authors":"Si Cheng, Siyi Chen, Xuefeng Yang, Zhuanghua Shi","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02023-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02023-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Decisions about a current stimulus are influenced by previously encountered stimuli, leading to sequential bias. However, the specific processing levels at which serial dependence emerges remain unclear. Despite considerable evidence pointing to contributions from perceptual and post-perceptual processes, as well as response carryover effects impacting subsequent judgments, research into how different task measurements affect sequential dependencies is limited. To address this gap, the present study investigated the role of task type in shaping sequential effects in time perception, employing a random-dot kinematogram (RDK) in a post-cue paradigm. Participants had to remember both the duration and the direction of the RDK movement and perform the task based on a post-cue, which was equally likely to be direction or duration. To delineate the task type, we employed the temporal bisection task in Experiment 1 and the duration reproduction task in Experiment 2. Both experiments revealed a significant sequential bias: durations were perceived as longer following longer previous durations, and vice versa. Intriguingly, the sequential effect was enhanced in the reproduction task following the same reproduction task (Experiment 2), but did not show significant variation by the task type in the bisection task (Experiment 1). Moreover, comparable response carryover effects were observed across two experiments. We argue that the differential impacts of task types on sequential dependence lies in the involvement of memory reactivation process in the decision stage, while the post-decision response carryover effect may reflect the assimilation by subjective, rather than objective, durations, potentially linking to the sticky pacemaker rate and/or decisional inertia.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2346-2359"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11522143/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142074176","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The influence of reward and loss outcomes after free- and forced-tasks on voluntary task choice. 自由任务和强迫任务后的奖励和损失结果对自愿任务选择的影响。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-30 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02009-9
Victor Mittelstädt, Ian G Mackenzie, Hartmut Leuthold

In four experiments, we investigated the impact of outcomes and processing mode (free versus forced) on subsequent voluntary task-switching behavior. Participants freely chose between two tasks or were forced to perform one, and the feedback they received randomly varied after correct performance (reward or no-reward; loss or no-loss). In general, we reasoned that the most recently applied task goal is usually the most valued one, leading people to prefer task repetitions over switches. However, the task values might be additionally biased by previous outcomes and the previous processing mode. Indeed, negatively reinforcing tasks with no-reward or losses generally resulted in more subsequent switches. Additionally, participants demonstrated a stronger attachment to free- compared to forced-tasks, as indicated by more switches when the previous task was forced, suggesting that people generally value free over forced-choice task goals. Moreover, the reward manipulation had a greater influence on switching behavior following free- compared to forced-tasks in Exp. 1 and Exp. 3, suggesting a stronger emphasis on evaluating rewarding outcomes associated with free-task choices. However, this inflationary effect on task choice seemed to be limited to reward and situations where task choice and performance more strongly overlap. Specifically, there was no evidence that switching behavior was differentially influenced after free-and forced-task as a function of losses (Exp. 2) or reward when task choice and task performance were separated (Exp. 4). Overall, the results provide new insights into how the valuation of task goals based on choice freedom and outcome feedback can influence voluntary task choices.

在四项实验中,我们研究了结果和处理模式(自由与强迫)对后续自愿任务转换行为的影响。参与者在两个任务之间自由选择,或被迫完成一个任务,正确完成任务后,他们收到的反馈随机变化(奖励或无奖励;损失或无损失)。一般来说,我们的推理是,最近应用的任务目标通常是最有价值的目标,这导致人们更喜欢任务重复而不是任务切换。然而,任务价值可能还会受到先前结果和先前处理模式的影响。事实上,无回报或损失的负强化任务通常会导致更多的后续转换。此外,与强迫任务相比,受试者对自由任务表现出更强的依恋,这表现在当前一个任务是强迫任务时,受试者会进行更多的转换,这表明人们通常更看重自由选择任务目标,而不是强迫选择任务目标。此外,在实验 1 和实验 3 中,与强迫任务相比,奖励操作对自由任务后的转换行为影响更大,这表明人们更重视评估与自由任务选择相关的奖励结果。然而,这种对任务选择的膨胀效应似乎仅限于奖励和任务选择与成绩重叠较多的情况。具体来说,当任务选择和任务表现分开时,没有证据表明自由任务和强迫任务后的转换行为会受到损失(实验 2)或奖励(实验 4)的不同影响。总之,这些结果为我们提供了新的见解,即基于选择自由和结果反馈的任务目标估值如何影响自愿任务选择。
{"title":"The influence of reward and loss outcomes after free- and forced-tasks on voluntary task choice.","authors":"Victor Mittelstädt, Ian G Mackenzie, Hartmut Leuthold","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02009-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02009-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In four experiments, we investigated the impact of outcomes and processing mode (free versus forced) on subsequent voluntary task-switching behavior. Participants freely chose between two tasks or were forced to perform one, and the feedback they received randomly varied after correct performance (reward or no-reward; loss or no-loss). In general, we reasoned that the most recently applied task goal is usually the most valued one, leading people to prefer task repetitions over switches. However, the task values might be additionally biased by previous outcomes and the previous processing mode. Indeed, negatively reinforcing tasks with no-reward or losses generally resulted in more subsequent switches. Additionally, participants demonstrated a stronger attachment to free- compared to forced-tasks, as indicated by more switches when the previous task was forced, suggesting that people generally value free over forced-choice task goals. Moreover, the reward manipulation had a greater influence on switching behavior following free- compared to forced-tasks in Exp. 1 and Exp. 3, suggesting a stronger emphasis on evaluating rewarding outcomes associated with free-task choices. However, this inflationary effect on task choice seemed to be limited to reward and situations where task choice and performance more strongly overlap. Specifically, there was no evidence that switching behavior was differentially influenced after free-and forced-task as a function of losses (Exp. 2) or reward when task choice and task performance were separated (Exp. 4). Overall, the results provide new insights into how the valuation of task goals based on choice freedom and outcome feedback can influence voluntary task choices.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"2059-2079"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11450031/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141793804","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Yin and Yang of Editing. 编辑的阴与阳
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-20 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02026-8
Bernhard Hommel
{"title":"The Yin and Yang of Editing.","authors":"Bernhard Hommel","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02026-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02026-8","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"1921-1922"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298851","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The ownership memory self-reference effect shifts recognition criterion but not recognition sensitivity. 所有权记忆自我参照效应会改变识别标准,但不会改变识别敏感度。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-21 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-01994-1
S D Sparks, A Kritikos

Information referenced to the self is retrieved more accurately than information referenced to others, known as the memory self-reference effect. It is unclear, however, whether social context (identity of the other) or task factors alter decision-making processes. In a virtual object allocation task, female participants sorted objects into their own or another's (stranger or mother) basket based on a colour cue. Subsequently, they performed a recognition memory task in which they first indicated whether each object was old or new, and then whether it had been allocated to themselves or to the other. We obtained owner-specific hit rates and false-alarm rates and applied signal detection theory to derive separate recognition sensitivity (d') and recognition criterion parameters (c) for self- and other-owned objects. While there was no clear evidence of a recognition self-reference effect, or a change in sensitivity, participants adopted a more conservative recognition criterion for self- compared with other-owned objects, and particularly when the other-referent was the participant's mother compared with the stranger. Moreover, when discriminating whether the originally presented objects were self- or other-owned, participants were biased toward ascribing ownership to the 'other'. We speculate that these findings reflect ownership-based changes in decisional processing during the recognition memory self-reference paradigm.

与参考他人的信息相比,参考自己的信息会被更准确地检索出来,这就是所谓的记忆自我参考效应。然而,目前还不清楚社会背景(他人身份)或任务因素是否会改变决策过程。在一项虚拟物品分配任务中,女性参与者根据颜色线索将物品分类放入自己或他人(陌生人或母亲)的篮子中。随后,她们进行了一项识别记忆任务,在这项任务中,她们首先要指出每个物品是新的还是旧的,然后再指出是分配给自己还是分配给他人。我们获得了特定所有者的命中率和误报率,并运用信号检测理论分别得出了自己和他人所有物品的识别灵敏度(d')和识别标准参数(c)。虽然没有明确的证据表明识别自我参照效应或灵敏度的变化,但与他人拥有的物品相比,参与者对自己拥有的物品采用了更保守的识别标准,特别是当他人参照物是参与者的母亲而不是陌生人时。此外,在辨别最初呈现的物品是自己所有还是他人所有时,参与者更倾向于将所有权归于 "他人"。我们推测这些发现反映了在识别记忆自我参照范式中基于所有权的决策加工变化。
{"title":"The ownership memory self-reference effect shifts recognition criterion but not recognition sensitivity.","authors":"S D Sparks, A Kritikos","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-01994-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-01994-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Information referenced to the self is retrieved more accurately than information referenced to others, known as the memory self-reference effect. It is unclear, however, whether social context (identity of the other) or task factors alter decision-making processes. In a virtual object allocation task, female participants sorted objects into their own or another's (stranger or mother) basket based on a colour cue. Subsequently, they performed a recognition memory task in which they first indicated whether each object was old or new, and then whether it had been allocated to themselves or to the other. We obtained owner-specific hit rates and false-alarm rates and applied signal detection theory to derive separate recognition sensitivity (d') and recognition criterion parameters (c) for self- and other-owned objects. While there was no clear evidence of a recognition self-reference effect, or a change in sensitivity, participants adopted a more conservative recognition criterion for self- compared with other-owned objects, and particularly when the other-referent was the participant's mother compared with the stranger. Moreover, when discriminating whether the originally presented objects were self- or other-owned, participants were biased toward ascribing ownership to the 'other'. We speculate that these findings reflect ownership-based changes in decisional processing during the recognition memory self-reference paradigm.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"1952-1968"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11450121/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141433074","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A meta-analysis on study and sample characteristics modulating mock earwitness performance. 关于影响模拟耳证人表现的研究和样本特征的荟萃分析。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-03 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-01991-4
Anja Leue, Philipp Wichert

Mock earwitness performance mainly addresses cognitive functions like free recall and recognition of auditory information. Based on primary studies a-priori experimental moderator effects on mock earwitness performance have been hypothesized. Including 46 articles with k = 66 experimental studies, a bare-bones and a random-effects, artefact-corrected meta-analysis have been performed. The results show a substantial ratio of the population effect size and the standard deviation of the population effect size (δ/SDδ) for the a-priori moderators bimodal compared to unimodal stimuli and for gender of listener. These results indicate that bimodal stimuli compared to unimodal stimuli yield substantially better mock earwitness performance. Women outperform men in mock earwitness performance. The fail-safe number demonstrates robust population effects for both a-priori moderators. As a post-hoc moderator, line-ups with target-present revealed substantial δ/SDδ ratios for stimulus length and gender of listener. These results for post-hoc moderators suggest that longer compared to shorter stimuli reveal better mock earwitness performance. The gender of listener effect is in target-present line-ups comparabale to the before mentioned gender of listener effect. G*Power calculations for future primary studies suggest that sample sizes in several primary studies were too small. Single-person earwitness assessments should be substantiated by reality monitoring and disentangled from hearsay evidence (i.e., situations in that individuals have heard information from other persons). The data recommend that best-practice options for earwitnesses in court settings should be derived from meta-analytic results, corresponding to the jurisdictions of the countries and exclusively for results that were substantial (δ/SDδ ratios) and robust (fail-safe number).

模拟耳证人的表现主要涉及听觉信息的自由回忆和识别等认知功能。在主要研究的基础上,假设了模拟耳证表现的先验实验调节效应。我们对 46 篇文章(k = 66 项实验研究)进行了基本分析和随机效应、人工影响校正荟萃分析。结果表明,与单模态刺激和听者性别相比,双模态先验调节因子的群体效应大小和群体效应大小的标准偏差(δ/SDδ)的比率很大。这些结果表明,与单模态刺激相比,双模态刺激能大大提高模拟耳证人的表现。女性的模拟耳证表现优于男性。对于两个先验调节因子,失效安全数都显示出了稳健的群体效应。作为事后调节因子,目标出现的排队游戏显示了刺激长度和听者性别的显著δ/SDδ比值。这些事后调节因素的结果表明,与较短的刺激相比,较长的刺激显示出更好的模拟耳闻者表现。听者性别效应在目标呈现阵容中与之前提到的听者性别效应相比较。未来初步研究的 G*Power 计算表明,几项初步研究的样本量太小。单人耳证评估应通过现实监测来证实,并与道听途说的证据(即个人从其他人那里听到信息的情况)区分开来。数据建议,法庭环境下的耳证最佳实践方案应从元分析结果中得出,与各国的司法管辖区相对应,并专门针对实质性(δ/SDδ比率)和稳健性(失效安全数)的结果。
{"title":"A meta-analysis on study and sample characteristics modulating mock earwitness performance.","authors":"Anja Leue, Philipp Wichert","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-01991-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-01991-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Mock earwitness performance mainly addresses cognitive functions like free recall and recognition of auditory information. Based on primary studies a-priori experimental moderator effects on mock earwitness performance have been hypothesized. Including 46 articles with k = 66 experimental studies, a bare-bones and a random-effects, artefact-corrected meta-analysis have been performed. The results show a substantial ratio of the population effect size and the standard deviation of the population effect size (δ/SD<sub>δ</sub>) for the a-priori moderators bimodal compared to unimodal stimuli and for gender of listener. These results indicate that bimodal stimuli compared to unimodal stimuli yield substantially better mock earwitness performance. Women outperform men in mock earwitness performance. The fail-safe number demonstrates robust population effects for both a-priori moderators. As a post-hoc moderator, line-ups with target-present revealed substantial δ/SD<sub>δ</sub> ratios for stimulus length and gender of listener. These results for post-hoc moderators suggest that longer compared to shorter stimuli reveal better mock earwitness performance. The gender of listener effect is in target-present line-ups comparabale to the before mentioned gender of listener effect. G*Power calculations for future primary studies suggest that sample sizes in several primary studies were too small. Single-person earwitness assessments should be substantiated by reality monitoring and disentangled from hearsay evidence (i.e., situations in that individuals have heard information from other persons). The data recommend that best-practice options for earwitnesses in court settings should be derived from meta-analytic results, corresponding to the jurisdictions of the countries and exclusively for results that were substantial (δ/SD<sub>δ</sub> ratios) and robust (fail-safe number).</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":" ","pages":"1923-1940"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141493936","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1