首页 > 最新文献

Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung最新文献

英文 中文
Crossmodal semantic congruence guides spontaneous orienting in real-life scenes. 跨模态语义一致性引导真实场景中的自发定向。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-06 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02018-8
Daria Kvasova, Llucia Coll, Travis Stewart, Salvador Soto-Faraco

In real-world scenes, the different objects and events are often interconnected within a rich web of semantic relationships. These semantic links help parse information efficiently and make sense of the sensory environment. It has been shown that, during goal-directed search, hearing the characteristic sound of an everyday life object helps finding the affiliate objects in artificial visual search arrays as well as in naturalistic, real-life videoclips. However, whether crossmodal semantic congruence also triggers orienting during spontaneous, not goal-directed observation is unknown. Here, we investigated this question addressing whether crossmodal semantic congruence can attract spontaneous, overt visual attention when viewing naturalistic, dynamic scenes. We used eye-tracking whilst participants (N = 45) watched video clips presented alongside sounds of varying semantic relatedness with objects present within the scene. We found that characteristic sounds increased the probability of looking at, the number of fixations to, and the total dwell time on semantically corresponding visual objects, in comparison to when the same scenes were presented with semantically neutral sounds or just with background noise only. Interestingly, hearing object sounds not met with an object in the scene led to increased visual exploration. These results suggest that crossmodal semantic information has an impact on spontaneous gaze on realistic scenes, and therefore on how information is sampled. Our findings extend beyond known effects of object-based crossmodal interactions with simple stimuli arrays and shed new light on the role that audio-visual semantic relationships out in the perception of everyday life scenarios.

在现实世界的场景中,不同的物体和事件往往在丰富的语义关系网中相互关联。这些语义联系有助于高效地解析信息,并使感官环境变得有意义。有研究表明,在目标定向搜索过程中,听到日常生活中物体的特征声音有助于在人工视觉搜索阵列以及自然真实的视频片段中找到关联物体。然而,跨模态语义一致性是否也能在自发而非目标导向的观察中触发定向,目前尚不清楚。在此,我们对这一问题进行了研究,探讨在观看自然动态场景时,跨模态语义一致性是否能吸引自发的、明显的视觉注意力。我们在参与者(45 人)观看视频剪辑时使用了眼动跟踪技术,同时播放与场景中物体语义相关的不同声音。我们发现,与播放语义中性的声音或仅播放背景噪声的场景相比,有特征的声音会增加视觉对象的注视概率、注视次数以及在语义对应的视觉对象上的总停留时间。有趣的是,听到与场景中的物体不相符合的物体声音会增加视觉探索。这些结果表明,跨模态语义信息会对现实场景中的自发注视产生影响,从而影响信息采样的方式。我们的研究结果超越了已知的简单刺激阵列中基于物体的跨模态交互作用的影响,并揭示了视听语义关系在日常生活场景感知中的作用。
{"title":"Crossmodal semantic congruence guides spontaneous orienting in real-life scenes.","authors":"Daria Kvasova, Llucia Coll, Travis Stewart, Salvador Soto-Faraco","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02018-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02018-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In real-world scenes, the different objects and events are often interconnected within a rich web of semantic relationships. These semantic links help parse information efficiently and make sense of the sensory environment. It has been shown that, during goal-directed search, hearing the characteristic sound of an everyday life object helps finding the affiliate objects in artificial visual search arrays as well as in naturalistic, real-life videoclips. However, whether crossmodal semantic congruence also triggers orienting during spontaneous, not goal-directed observation is unknown. Here, we investigated this question addressing whether crossmodal semantic congruence can attract spontaneous, overt visual attention when viewing naturalistic, dynamic scenes. We used eye-tracking whilst participants (N = 45) watched video clips presented alongside sounds of varying semantic relatedness with objects present within the scene. We found that characteristic sounds increased the probability of looking at, the number of fixations to, and the total dwell time on semantically corresponding visual objects, in comparison to when the same scenes were presented with semantically neutral sounds or just with background noise only. Interestingly, hearing object sounds not met with an object in the scene led to increased visual exploration. These results suggest that crossmodal semantic information has an impact on spontaneous gaze on realistic scenes, and therefore on how information is sampled. Our findings extend beyond known effects of object-based crossmodal interactions with simple stimuli arrays and shed new light on the role that audio-visual semantic relationships out in the perception of everyday life scenarios.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141894653","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of different types of leisure activities on working memory across the adult lifespan. 不同类型的休闲活动对成年人一生中工作记忆的影响。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-06 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-01998-x
Selene Cansino, Frine Torres-Trejo, Cinthya Estrada-Manilla, Silvia Ruiz-Velasco

The aim of the present study was to contrast the potential influence of five independent types of leisure activities (physical, mental, social, cultural, and passive) on working memory in a lifespan sample and in specific stages of adulthood (young, middle-aged, and older adults). A sample of 1652 healthy adults between 21 and 80 years of age participated in the study. Leisure activities were assessed through a lifestyle questionnaire created for the study. Working memory was measured in the verbal and spatial domains using a computerized n-back task that allowed us to reliably measure discrimination and reaction times. Across adulthood, mental (computer use and hobbies) and social leisure activities predicted greater verbal and spatial working memory discrimination; mental (reading) and social activities predicted faster verbal working memory; and mental (computer use) and physical activities predicted faster spatial working memory. In young adults, mental (computer use) and social activities were associated with greater verbal and spatial working memory performance. In middle-aged adults, physical and mental activities (computer use) were associated with greater working memory performance. In older adults, physical, mental (hobbies), and social activities were associated with greater working memory performance. Leisure activities can enhance working memory discrimination and speed independent of individuals' age.

本研究旨在对比五种独立类型的休闲活动(体育、心理、社交、文化和被动)对一生样本和特定成年阶段(青年、中年和老年)工作记忆的潜在影响。这项研究的样本包括 1652 名 21 至 80 岁的健康成年人。休闲活动是通过为研究制作的生活方式问卷进行评估的。工作记忆是通过计算机化的 "n-back "任务对言语和空间领域进行测量的,该任务使我们能够可靠地测量辨别和反应时间。在整个成年期,脑力活动(使用电脑和业余爱好)和社交休闲活动预示着更强的言语和空间工作记忆辨别能力;脑力活动(阅读)和社交活动预示着更快的言语工作记忆能力;脑力活动(使用电脑)和体育活动预示着更快的空间工作记忆能力。在年轻人中,脑力活动(使用电脑)和社交活动与更高的言语和空间工作记忆能力有关。在中年人中,体力和脑力活动(使用电脑)与工作记忆能力的提高有关。在老年人中,体力、脑力(业余爱好)和社交活动与工作记忆能力的提高有关。休闲活动可以提高工作记忆的辨别能力和速度,与个人的年龄无关。
{"title":"Effects of different types of leisure activities on working memory across the adult lifespan.","authors":"Selene Cansino, Frine Torres-Trejo, Cinthya Estrada-Manilla, Silvia Ruiz-Velasco","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-01998-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-01998-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The aim of the present study was to contrast the potential influence of five independent types of leisure activities (physical, mental, social, cultural, and passive) on working memory in a lifespan sample and in specific stages of adulthood (young, middle-aged, and older adults). A sample of 1652 healthy adults between 21 and 80 years of age participated in the study. Leisure activities were assessed through a lifestyle questionnaire created for the study. Working memory was measured in the verbal and spatial domains using a computerized n-back task that allowed us to reliably measure discrimination and reaction times. Across adulthood, mental (computer use and hobbies) and social leisure activities predicted greater verbal and spatial working memory discrimination; mental (reading) and social activities predicted faster verbal working memory; and mental (computer use) and physical activities predicted faster spatial working memory. In young adults, mental (computer use) and social activities were associated with greater verbal and spatial working memory performance. In middle-aged adults, physical and mental activities (computer use) were associated with greater working memory performance. In older adults, physical, mental (hobbies), and social activities were associated with greater working memory performance. Leisure activities can enhance working memory discrimination and speed independent of individuals' age.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11449988/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141545347","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effect of additional tasks on the reaction time of braking responses in simulated car driving: beyond the PRP effect. 附加任务对模拟汽车驾驶中制动反应时间的影响:超越 PRP 效应。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-01988-z
Robert Stojan, Otmar Bock, Melanie Mack, Claudia Voelcker-Rehage

The presentation of one task increases the reaction time on a subsequent task, if stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between tasks is short. This psychological refractory period (PRP) effect is typically leveling off as SOA approaches 1 s, which has been documented both in classical laboratory paradigms and in simulated car driving. Here we report a more persistent effect on the subsequent task that goes well beyond the typical duration of the PRP effect. In a driving simulator, 120 healthy older participants followed a lead car that mostly drove at a constant speed. They had to maintain a regular distance from the lead car and had to brake when the lead car braked. Participants also engaged in several additional tasks during driving (two types of tasks: typing three-digit numbers, stating arguments on public issues). SOA between the braking task and the last preceding additional task was 11.49 s ± 1.99 (mean and standard deviation). In a control condition, the braking task was administered without additional tasks. Main performance outcome was Braking Reaction Time (RT, in s), as the interval between onset of brake lights of the lead car and the moment participants released the gas pedal. Additionally, foot movement time (MT, in s), i.e., the difference between gas pedal release and brake pedal onset, was considered for possible compensation behavior. Inter-vehicle distance to the lead car (in m) was taken into account as a moderator. We found that RT averaged 0.77 s without additional tasks, but averaged 1.45 s with additional tasks. This RT difference was less pronounced at smaller inter-vehicle distances, and was not compensated for by faster MT from the gas pedal to the brake pedal. We conclude that detrimental effects of additional tasks on subsequent braking responses can be more persistent than suggested by the PRP effect, possibly because of maintaining multiple task sets, requiring increased executive control. We further conclude that potential detrimental effects can be ameliorated at small inter-vehicle distances by mobilizing extra cognitive resources when response urgency is higher. As a practical implication of our study, distracting stimuli can have persisting detrimental effects on traffic safety.

如果任务之间的刺激起始不同步(SOA)时间较短,则一个任务的呈现会增加后续任务的反应时间。这种心理折射期(PRP)效应通常在 SOA 接近 1 秒时趋于平缓,这在经典的实验室范例和模拟汽车驾驶中都有记录。在这里,我们报告了一种对后续任务更持久的影响,这种影响远远超过了 PRP 效应的典型持续时间。在驾驶模拟器中,120 名健康的老年参与者跟随一辆主要以恒定速度行驶的领头车。他们必须与领头车保持一定距离,并在领头车刹车时制动。在驾驶过程中,参与者还需要完成几项额外的任务(两类任务:输入三位数的数字,就公共问题发表论点)。制动任务与最后一项附加任务之间的时间间隔为 11.49 秒 ± 1.99(平均值和标准偏差)。在对照组条件下,制动任务没有附加任务。主要成绩结果是制动反应时间(RT,单位为秒),即前车制动灯亮起与参与者松开油门踏板之间的时间间隔。此外,脚部移动时间(MT,秒),即松开油门踏板与踩下制动踏板之间的时间差,也被视为可能的补偿行为。车辆与前车之间的距离(单位:米)作为调节因素也被考虑在内。我们发现,在没有额外任务的情况下,RT 平均为 0.77 秒,但在有额外任务的情况下,RT 平均为 1.45 秒。在车距较小的情况下,这种 RT 差异并不明显,而且从油门踏板到制动踏板的 MT 速度更快也无法弥补这种 RT 差异。我们得出结论,额外任务对后续制动反应的不利影响可能比 PRP 效应所显示的更为持久,这可能是因为维持多个任务集需要更多的执行控制。我们进一步得出结论,在车辆间距离较小的情况下,当反应紧迫性较高时,通过调动额外的认知资源,可以改善潜在的不利影响。我们研究的一个实际意义是,分散注意力的刺激会对交通安全产生持续的不利影响。
{"title":"Effect of additional tasks on the reaction time of braking responses in simulated car driving: beyond the PRP effect.","authors":"Robert Stojan, Otmar Bock, Melanie Mack, Claudia Voelcker-Rehage","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-01988-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-01988-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The presentation of one task increases the reaction time on a subsequent task, if stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between tasks is short. This psychological refractory period (PRP) effect is typically leveling off as SOA approaches 1 s, which has been documented both in classical laboratory paradigms and in simulated car driving. Here we report a more persistent effect on the subsequent task that goes well beyond the typical duration of the PRP effect. In a driving simulator, 120 healthy older participants followed a lead car that mostly drove at a constant speed. They had to maintain a regular distance from the lead car and had to brake when the lead car braked. Participants also engaged in several additional tasks during driving (two types of tasks: typing three-digit numbers, stating arguments on public issues). SOA between the braking task and the last preceding additional task was 11.49 s ± 1.99 (mean and standard deviation). In a control condition, the braking task was administered without additional tasks. Main performance outcome was Braking Reaction Time (RT, in s), as the interval between onset of brake lights of the lead car and the moment participants released the gas pedal. Additionally, foot movement time (MT, in s), i.e., the difference between gas pedal release and brake pedal onset, was considered for possible compensation behavior. Inter-vehicle distance to the lead car (in m) was taken into account as a moderator. We found that RT averaged 0.77 s without additional tasks, but averaged 1.45 s with additional tasks. This RT difference was less pronounced at smaller inter-vehicle distances, and was not compensated for by faster MT from the gas pedal to the brake pedal. We conclude that detrimental effects of additional tasks on subsequent braking responses can be more persistent than suggested by the PRP effect, possibly because of maintaining multiple task sets, requiring increased executive control. We further conclude that potential detrimental effects can be ameliorated at small inter-vehicle distances by mobilizing extra cognitive resources when response urgency is higher. As a practical implication of our study, distracting stimuli can have persisting detrimental effects on traffic safety.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141447371","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Modeling body size information within weight labels using probability distributions. 利用概率分布对体重标签中的体型信息进行建模。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-22 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02006-y
Thomas Chazelle, Michel Guerraz, Richard Palluel-Germain

What images of bodies do we associate with thinness and fatness? Can our representations of weight-related words be described by simple probability distributions? To answer these questions, the present study examined participants' perceptions of a set of weight-related words using a pictural scale. 259 French women indicated the thinnest, fattest, and best-fitting figures for 13 words. We then used their responses to construct PERT probability distributions, simple skewed distributions allowing to visualize what body sizes were associated with each word. In particular, the variability of the distributions showed how different weight labels can have more or less precise meanings. We found some evidence that the lowest body mass index associated with a label shifted towards thinner figures as body dissatisfaction increased. Using the same method, we investigated the boundaries of what participants consider the ideal body, and showed that the inclusion of their own body in these boundaries predicted their levels of body dissatisfaction. We argue that PERT distributions can be a useful, easy-to-use tool in body image research for modeling the representations of weight labels in different populations.

我们将哪些身体形象与瘦和胖联系在一起?我们对体重相关词汇的表征可以用简单的概率分布来描述吗?为了回答这些问题,本研究使用图形量表考察了参与者对一组体重相关词汇的看法。259 名法国女性为 13 个单词标出了最瘦、最胖和最合适的数字。然后,我们利用她们的回答构建了 PERT 概率分布,通过这种简单的倾斜分布可以直观地看出每个词与哪些体型相关。特别是,分布的可变性显示了不同的体重标签可能具有或多或少的精确含义。我们发现有证据表明,随着对身体不满意度的增加,与标签相关的最低体重指数会向更瘦的体型转移。使用同样的方法,我们调查了参与者所认为的理想身材的界限,结果表明,将他们自己的身材纳入这些界限可以预测他们对身体的不满意程度。我们认为,在身体形象研究中,PERT 分布可以作为一种有用且易于使用的工具,用于模拟不同人群中体重标签的表现形式。
{"title":"Modeling body size information within weight labels using probability distributions.","authors":"Thomas Chazelle, Michel Guerraz, Richard Palluel-Germain","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02006-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02006-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>What images of bodies do we associate with thinness and fatness? Can our representations of weight-related words be described by simple probability distributions? To answer these questions, the present study examined participants' perceptions of a set of weight-related words using a pictural scale. 259 French women indicated the thinnest, fattest, and best-fitting figures for 13 words. We then used their responses to construct PERT probability distributions, simple skewed distributions allowing to visualize what body sizes were associated with each word. In particular, the variability of the distributions showed how different weight labels can have more or less precise meanings. We found some evidence that the lowest body mass index associated with a label shifted towards thinner figures as body dissatisfaction increased. Using the same method, we investigated the boundaries of what participants consider the ideal body, and showed that the inclusion of their own body in these boundaries predicted their levels of body dissatisfaction. We argue that PERT distributions can be a useful, easy-to-use tool in body image research for modeling the representations of weight labels in different populations.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141749352","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Impact of aging on crossmodal attention switching. 衰老对跨模态注意力转换的影响
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-26 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-01992-3
Ludivine A P Schils, Iring Koch, Pi-Chun Huang, Shulan Hsieh, Denise N Stephan

Previous studies on crossmodal visual-auditory attention switching using a spatial discrimination task showed performance costs when the target modality changed relative to when it repeated. The present study (n = 42 for each age group) examined age-related changes in crossmodal attention switching by asking young (age range 19 to 30 years old) and older (age range 64 to 80 years old) participants to respond to unimodal central cues and bimodal lateralized stimuli. The participants' task was to indicate the location of the target in the relevant modality using button presses. Results showed general attention switch costs. Additionally, we found no specific age-related increase of attention switch costs (no difference in performance between switch and repetition of target modality), but age-related increased mixing costs (decreased performance for repetition in modality-mixed condition compared to single target modality). Moreover, spatial distraction produced a crossmodal congruency effect, which was only slightly larger in older adults. Taken together, age-related increased mixing costs suggest a general difficulty with maintaining more than one task, but no specific age-related crossmodal impairment in crossmodal attention switching.

以往使用空间分辨任务对跨模态视觉-听觉注意力转换进行的研究表明,当目标模态发生变化时,相对于目标模态重复时的表现成本。本研究(每个年龄组 n = 42 人)通过让年轻(年龄范围为 19 至 30 岁)和年长(年龄范围为 64 至 80 岁)的参与者对单模态中心提示和双模态侧向刺激做出反应,考察了跨模态注意转换中与年龄相关的变化。参与者的任务是通过按下按钮来指出目标在相关模态中的位置。结果显示,注意力转换成本普遍较高。此外,我们还发现注意力转换成本的增加与年龄无关(转换和重复目标模态的表现没有差异),但混合成本的增加与年龄有关(与单一目标模态相比,混合模态条件下的重复表现下降)。此外,空间分心会产生跨模态一致性效应,而这种效应在老年人中仅略微增大。总而言之,与年龄相关的混合成本增加表明,老年人在维持一项以上任务时普遍存在困难,但在跨模态注意转换方面并不存在与年龄相关的特定跨模态障碍。
{"title":"Impact of aging on crossmodal attention switching.","authors":"Ludivine A P Schils, Iring Koch, Pi-Chun Huang, Shulan Hsieh, Denise N Stephan","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-01992-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-01992-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous studies on crossmodal visual-auditory attention switching using a spatial discrimination task showed performance costs when the target modality changed relative to when it repeated. The present study (n = 42 for each age group) examined age-related changes in crossmodal attention switching by asking young (age range 19 to 30 years old) and older (age range 64 to 80 years old) participants to respond to unimodal central cues and bimodal lateralized stimuli. The participants' task was to indicate the location of the target in the relevant modality using button presses. Results showed general attention switch costs. Additionally, we found no specific age-related increase of attention switch costs (no difference in performance between switch and repetition of target modality), but age-related increased mixing costs (decreased performance for repetition in modality-mixed condition compared to single target modality). Moreover, spatial distraction produced a crossmodal congruency effect, which was only slightly larger in older adults. Taken together, age-related increased mixing costs suggest a general difficulty with maintaining more than one task, but no specific age-related crossmodal impairment in crossmodal attention switching.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141451906","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Different grasping experiences affect mapping effects but not correspondence effects between stimulus size and response location. 不同的抓握体验会影响映射效应,但不会影响刺激大小和反应位置之间的对应效应。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-28 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-01990-5
Melanie Richter, Peter Wühr

The so-called spatial-size association of response codes (SSARC) effect denotes that humans respond faster and more accurately with a left response to physically small stimuli and a right response to physically large stimuli, as compared to the opposite mapping. According to an application of the CORE principle to the SSARC effect, the habit to grasp larger/heavier objects with one's dominant hand and smaller/lighter objects with one's non-dominant hand creates spatial-size associations. We investigated if grasping habits play a causal role in the formation of spatial-size associations by testing if the mapping of a preceding object-grasping task affects the size of the SSARC effect in subsequent choice-response tasks with keypress responses. In the object-grasping task, participants were instructed to grasp wooden cubes of variable size either according to a compatible (small-left; large-right) or according to an incompatible (small-right; large-left) mapping. In the choice-response tasks, participants responded with left or right keypresses to the size or color of a small or large stimulus. The results showed that participants with the compatible mapping in the object-grasping task showed a larger SSARC effect in the size discrimination task, but not in the color discrimination task, than participants with the incompatible mapping in the object-grasping task. Results suggest that a short period of practice with different size-location mappings can modulate size-location links used for controlled S-R translation, but not links underlying automatic S-R translation. In general, the results support the hypothesis that grasping habits play a causal role in the formation of spatial-size associations.

所谓反应代码的空间大小关联效应(SSARC)是指,与相反的映射相比,人类对物理上较小的刺激做出左侧反应,对物理上较大的刺激做出右侧反应,反应速度更快、更准确。根据对 SSARC 效应的 CORE 原则的应用,用惯用手抓取较大/较重的物体和用非惯用手抓取较小/较轻的物体的习惯会产生空间大小关联。我们通过测试前一个物体抓取任务的映射是否会影响后续按键选择反应任务中 SSARC 效应的大小,来研究抓取习惯是否在空间大小联想的形成过程中起到了因果作用。在物体抓取任务中,受试者被要求根据相容(左小;右大)或不相容(右小;左大)的映射来抓取不同大小的木质立方体。在选择-反应任务中,受试者用左键或右键对大小刺激物的大小或颜色做出反应。结果表明,在物体抓取任务中使用相容映射的参与者在尺寸辨别任务中表现出的 SSARC 效应要大于在物体抓取任务中使用不相容映射的参与者,但在颜色辨别任务中却没有表现出 SSARC 效应。结果表明,短时间的不同大小-位置映射练习可以调节用于控制 S-R 翻译的大小-位置联系,但不能调节自动 S-R 翻译的基础联系。总的来说,这些结果支持了这样一个假设,即抓握习惯在空间-大小关联的形成过程中起着因果作用。
{"title":"Different grasping experiences affect mapping effects but not correspondence effects between stimulus size and response location.","authors":"Melanie Richter, Peter Wühr","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-01990-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-01990-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The so-called spatial-size association of response codes (SSARC) effect denotes that humans respond faster and more accurately with a left response to physically small stimuli and a right response to physically large stimuli, as compared to the opposite mapping. According to an application of the CORE principle to the SSARC effect, the habit to grasp larger/heavier objects with one's dominant hand and smaller/lighter objects with one's non-dominant hand creates spatial-size associations. We investigated if grasping habits play a causal role in the formation of spatial-size associations by testing if the mapping of a preceding object-grasping task affects the size of the SSARC effect in subsequent choice-response tasks with keypress responses. In the object-grasping task, participants were instructed to grasp wooden cubes of variable size either according to a compatible (small-left; large-right) or according to an incompatible (small-right; large-left) mapping. In the choice-response tasks, participants responded with left or right keypresses to the size or color of a small or large stimulus. The results showed that participants with the compatible mapping in the object-grasping task showed a larger SSARC effect in the size discrimination task, but not in the color discrimination task, than participants with the incompatible mapping in the object-grasping task. Results suggest that a short period of practice with different size-location mappings can modulate size-location links used for controlled S-R translation, but not links underlying automatic S-R translation. In general, the results support the hypothesis that grasping habits play a causal role in the formation of spatial-size associations.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141471630","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to: A kinematically complex multi‑articular motor skill for investigating implicit motor learning. 更正:一种运动学上复杂的多关节运动技能,用于研究隐性运动学习。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-25 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02025-9
Jack P Solomon, Austin J Hurst, Sarah N Kraeutner, Tony G J Ingram, Shaun G Boe
{"title":"Correction to: A kinematically complex multi‑articular motor skill for investigating implicit motor learning.","authors":"Jack P Solomon, Austin J Hurst, Sarah N Kraeutner, Tony G J Ingram, Shaun G Boe","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02025-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02025-9","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337144","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Semantic-to-autobiographical memory priming: the role of stimulus processing. 从语义到自传的记忆引物:刺激加工的作用。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-09 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-01999-w
John H Mace, Sophia R Keller

It is now well established that semantic processing can cause the activation of memories in the autobiographical memory system. Studies have shown that semantic processing of words, sounds, objects, or pictures primes autobiographical memories on voluntary and involuntary autobiographical memory tasks (the Crovitz cue-word task and the vigilance task). Known as semantic-to-autobiographical memory priming, our goal in the current study was to demonstrate that this form of priming occurs under different forms of processing (i.e., shallow versus deep), and that some forms of processing (e.g., visual mental imagery) may enhance priming in this domain. In Experiment 1, equivalent semantic-to-autobiographical priming was obtained on the vigilance task following shallow (e-counting) and deep (meaning judgements) word processing. In Experiment 2, word meaning judgements were compared to visual imagery of word meanings, and visual imagery led to more semantic-to-autobiographical priming on the vigilance task than meaning judgements. The results of these experiments support the idea that semantic-to-autobiographical priming occurs under a wide range of processing conditions, supporting a ubiquity claim, with some conditions producing more priming than others, and they further support the idea that this form of may play an important role in the production of involuntary memories in everyday life.

语义加工可以激活自传体记忆系统中的记忆,这一点现已得到证实。研究表明,在自愿和非自愿的自传体记忆任务(克罗维茨提示词任务和警觉任务)中,对单词、声音、物体或图片的语义加工会启动自传体记忆。这项研究被称为语义自传体记忆引物,我们的目标是证明这种引物形式会在不同的加工形式(即浅层加工和深层加工)下发生,而且某些加工形式(如视觉心理想象)可能会增强这一领域的引物。在实验 1 中,经过浅层(电子计数)和深层(词义判断)词处理后,在警觉任务中获得了同等的语义自传引物。在实验 2 中,词义判断与词义的视觉意象进行了比较,在警觉性任务中,视觉意象比词义判断产生了更多的语义自传引物。这些实验结果支持这样一种观点,即语义自传引物会在各种加工条件下出现,支持无处不在的说法,有些条件会比其他条件产生更多的引物,这些实验结果进一步支持了这样一种观点,即这种形式的引物可能在日常生活中产生非自主记忆方面发挥重要作用。
{"title":"Semantic-to-autobiographical memory priming: the role of stimulus processing.","authors":"John H Mace, Sophia R Keller","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-01999-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-01999-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is now well established that semantic processing can cause the activation of memories in the autobiographical memory system. Studies have shown that semantic processing of words, sounds, objects, or pictures primes autobiographical memories on voluntary and involuntary autobiographical memory tasks (the Crovitz cue-word task and the vigilance task). Known as semantic-to-autobiographical memory priming, our goal in the current study was to demonstrate that this form of priming occurs under different forms of processing (i.e., shallow versus deep), and that some forms of processing (e.g., visual mental imagery) may enhance priming in this domain. In Experiment 1, equivalent semantic-to-autobiographical priming was obtained on the vigilance task following shallow (e-counting) and deep (meaning judgements) word processing. In Experiment 2, word meaning judgements were compared to visual imagery of word meanings, and visual imagery led to more semantic-to-autobiographical priming on the vigilance task than meaning judgements. The results of these experiments support the idea that semantic-to-autobiographical priming occurs under a wide range of processing conditions, supporting a ubiquity claim, with some conditions producing more priming than others, and they further support the idea that this form of may play an important role in the production of involuntary memories in everyday life.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141559989","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Inhibition of cued but not executed task sets depends on cue-task compatibility and practice. 对有提示但未执行的任务集的抑制取决于提示-任务的兼容性和练习。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-30 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02013-z
Alexander Berger, Iring Koch, Markus Kiefer

In task switching, processing a task cue is thought to activate the corresponding task representation ("task set"), thereby allowing for advance task preparation. However, the contribution of preparatory processes to the emergence of n-2 repetition costs as index of task set inhibition processes is debated. The present study investigated whether merely preparing for a task activates a corresponding task set, which needs to be inhibited in order to switch to a different task. To this end, we presented so-called task cue-only trials in trial n-2 and assessed subsequent n-2 repetition costs. The results revealed n-2 repetition costs following a task cue-only, but only for compatible cues with a transparent cue-task relation and only at the beginning of the experiment. In contrast, n-2 repetition costs following task execution in trial n-2 were absent. In a second experiment, we sought to rule out that the presence of n-2 repetition costs following a task cue-only and the corresponding absence following task execution were the consequence of a decay of task sets. This second experiment replicated the result pattern of the first experiment, with n-2 repetition costs following a task cue-only being present only at the beginning of the experiment and only for compatible cues. Hence, cue-induced task set inhibition effects depended on cue-task compatibility and practice. Furthermore, merely prepared task sets were more likely inhibited than executed task sets.

在任务转换过程中,对任务线索的处理被认为会激活相应的任务表征("任务集"),从而提前做好任务准备。然而,准备过程对作为任务集抑制过程指标的 n-2 重复成本的出现所起的作用还存在争议。本研究调查了仅仅为一项任务做准备是否会激活相应的任务集,而为了切换到不同的任务,需要对任务集进行抑制。为此,我们在第 n-2 次试验中呈现了所谓的纯任务线索试验,并对随后的 n-2 次重复成本进行了评估。结果显示,在仅任务线索试验后,重复成本为 n-2,但仅限于具有透明线索-任务关系的相容线索,而且仅限于试验开始时。与此相反,在第 n-2 次试验中执行任务后的 n-2 次重复成本并不存在。在第二个实验中,我们试图排除仅在任务提示后出现 n-2 重复成本和在任务执行后出现 n-2 重复成本是任务集衰减的结果。第二个实验复制了第一个实验的结果模式,即只有在实验开始时才会出现任务提示后的 n-2 重复成本,而且只有在兼容提示时才会出现。因此,线索引起的任务集抑制效应取决于线索与任务的兼容性和练习。此外,仅仅准备的任务集比执行的任务集更容易受到抑制。
{"title":"Inhibition of cued but not executed task sets depends on cue-task compatibility and practice.","authors":"Alexander Berger, Iring Koch, Markus Kiefer","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02013-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02013-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In task switching, processing a task cue is thought to activate the corresponding task representation (\"task set\"), thereby allowing for advance task preparation. However, the contribution of preparatory processes to the emergence of n-2 repetition costs as index of task set inhibition processes is debated. The present study investigated whether merely preparing for a task activates a corresponding task set, which needs to be inhibited in order to switch to a different task. To this end, we presented so-called task cue-only trials in trial n-2 and assessed subsequent n-2 repetition costs. The results revealed n-2 repetition costs following a task cue-only, but only for compatible cues with a transparent cue-task relation and only at the beginning of the experiment. In contrast, n-2 repetition costs following task execution in trial n-2 were absent. In a second experiment, we sought to rule out that the presence of n-2 repetition costs following a task cue-only and the corresponding absence following task execution were the consequence of a decay of task sets. This second experiment replicated the result pattern of the first experiment, with n-2 repetition costs following a task cue-only being present only at the beginning of the experiment and only for compatible cues. Hence, cue-induced task set inhibition effects depended on cue-task compatibility and practice. Furthermore, merely prepared task sets were more likely inhibited than executed task sets.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11450066/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141856839","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sequential and domain-specific processing mechanisms for dual cognitive-emotional conflict. 认知-情感双重冲突的顺序和特定领域处理机制。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-30 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02012-0
Ling Xiang, Yu Gao, Zhihan Xiong, Jingjing Hu, Baoxi Wang

This study aimed to investigate how the cognitive control system resolves conflicts when cognitive and emotional conflicts occur simultaneously, and how it performs. To achieve this, a factorial task-crossing design was employed, combining the spatial Simon task and the face-word emotional interference task, allowing cognitive and emotional conflicts to occur concurrently within a single trial. The results revealed that the Simon cognitive conflict was only associated with N2 and early SP, while it did not affect the amplitude of N450 and late SP. Conversely, the face-word emotional conflict affected the amplitude of N450 and late SP, but had no impact on N2 and early SP. These findings demonstrate the adaptive sequencing organization and domain specificity in cognitive-emotional dual conflict processing, which reflects the precise and flexible orchestration and strategic adjustments of the cognitive control system. The results contribute to a better understanding of the dynamic and temporal processes involved in the cognitive control of multiple conflicts.

本研究旨在探讨当认知冲突和情绪冲突同时发生时,认知控制系统是如何解决冲突的,以及它的表现如何。为此,我们采用了一个因子任务交叉设计,将空间西蒙任务和面字情绪干扰任务结合起来,让认知冲突和情绪冲突在一次试验中同时发生。结果发现,西蒙认知冲突只与 N2 和早期 SP 相关,而不影响 N450 和后期 SP 的振幅。相反,脸-词情绪冲突影响了N450和晚期SP的振幅,但对N2和早期SP没有影响。这些发现证明了认知-情绪双重冲突处理的适应性排序组织和领域特异性,反映了认知控制系统精确灵活的协调和策略调整。这些结果有助于更好地理解多重冲突的认知控制所涉及的动态和时间过程。
{"title":"Sequential and domain-specific processing mechanisms for dual cognitive-emotional conflict.","authors":"Ling Xiang, Yu Gao, Zhihan Xiong, Jingjing Hu, Baoxi Wang","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02012-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02012-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study aimed to investigate how the cognitive control system resolves conflicts when cognitive and emotional conflicts occur simultaneously, and how it performs. To achieve this, a factorial task-crossing design was employed, combining the spatial Simon task and the face-word emotional interference task, allowing cognitive and emotional conflicts to occur concurrently within a single trial. The results revealed that the Simon cognitive conflict was only associated with N2 and early SP, while it did not affect the amplitude of N450 and late SP. Conversely, the face-word emotional conflict affected the amplitude of N450 and late SP, but had no impact on N2 and early SP. These findings demonstrate the adaptive sequencing organization and domain specificity in cognitive-emotional dual conflict processing, which reflects the precise and flexible orchestration and strategic adjustments of the cognitive control system. The results contribute to a better understanding of the dynamic and temporal processes involved in the cognitive control of multiple conflicts.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141856840","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1