首页 > 最新文献

Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung最新文献

英文 中文
Distinct serial dependence between small and large numerosity processing. 小数和大数处理之间具有明显的序列依赖性。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-31 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02071-3
Yue Huang, Haokun Li, Shiming Qiu, Xianfeng Ding, Min Li, Wangjuan Liu, Zhao Fan, Xiaorong Cheng

The serial dependence effect (SDE) is a perceptual bias where current stimuli are perceived as more similar to recently seen stimuli, possibly enhancing the stability and continuity of visual perception. Although SDE has been observed across many visual features, it remains unclear whether humans rely on a single mechanism of SDE to support numerosity processing across two distinct numerical ranges: subitizing (i.e., small numerosity processing, likely related to early object recognition) and estimation (i.e., large numerosity processing, likely related to ensemble numerosity extraction). Here, we show that subitizing and estimation exhibit distinct SDE patterns. Subitizing is characterized by an asymmetric SDE, whereas estimation demonstrates a symmetric SDE. Specifically, in subitizing, the SDE occurs only when the current magnitude is smaller than the previous magnitude but not when it is larger. In contrast, the SDE in estimation is present in both scenarios. We propose that these differences arise from distinct underlying mechanisms. A perceptual mechanism-namely, a 'temporal hysteresis' account, can explain the asymmetrical SDE in subitizing since object individuation resources are easily activated but resistant to deactivation. Conversely, a combination of perceptual and post-perceptual mechanisms can account for the SDEs in estimation, as both perceptual and post-perceptual interference can reduce the SDEs. Critically, a novel type of SDE characterized by reduced processing precision is found in subitizing only, implying that the continuity and stability of numerical processing can be dissociable in dynamic situations where numerical information is integrated over time. Our findings reveal the multifaceted nature of SDE mechanisms and suggest their engagement with cognitive modules likely subserving different functionalities.

序列依赖效应(SDE)是一种感知偏差,即当前的刺激被认为与最近看到的刺激更相似,可能会增强视觉感知的稳定性和连续性。尽管SDE已经在许多视觉特征中被观察到,但尚不清楚人类是否依赖于SDE的单一机制来支持两个不同数值范围的数字处理:细分(即小数字处理,可能与早期目标识别有关)和估计(即大数字处理,可能与集合数字提取有关)。在这里,我们展示了细分和估计表现出不同的SDE模式。细分的特点是不对称SDE,而估计的特点是对称SDE。具体来说,在细分中,SDE仅在当前量级小于前一个量级时发生,而在当前量级大于前一个量级时不发生。相反,估计中的SDE在两种情况下都存在。我们认为这些差异是由不同的潜在机制引起的。一种感知机制,即“时间滞后”解释,可以解释不对称的SDE,因为对象个性化资源很容易被激活,但不易被停用。相反,知觉和后知觉机制的结合可以解释估计中的sde,因为知觉和后知觉干扰都可以降低sde。关键的是,一种以加工精度降低为特征的新型SDE仅在细分中被发现,这意味着数值处理的连续性和稳定性在数值信息随时间集成的动态情况下可以分离。我们的研究结果揭示了SDE机制的多面性,并表明它们与认知模块的参与可能服务于不同的功能。
{"title":"Distinct serial dependence between small and large numerosity processing.","authors":"Yue Huang, Haokun Li, Shiming Qiu, Xianfeng Ding, Min Li, Wangjuan Liu, Zhao Fan, Xiaorong Cheng","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02071-3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-024-02071-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The serial dependence effect (SDE) is a perceptual bias where current stimuli are perceived as more similar to recently seen stimuli, possibly enhancing the stability and continuity of visual perception. Although SDE has been observed across many visual features, it remains unclear whether humans rely on a single mechanism of SDE to support numerosity processing across two distinct numerical ranges: subitizing (i.e., small numerosity processing, likely related to early object recognition) and estimation (i.e., large numerosity processing, likely related to ensemble numerosity extraction). Here, we show that subitizing and estimation exhibit distinct SDE patterns. Subitizing is characterized by an asymmetric SDE, whereas estimation demonstrates a symmetric SDE. Specifically, in subitizing, the SDE occurs only when the current magnitude is smaller than the previous magnitude but not when it is larger. In contrast, the SDE in estimation is present in both scenarios. We propose that these differences arise from distinct underlying mechanisms. A perceptual mechanism-namely, a 'temporal hysteresis' account, can explain the asymmetrical SDE in subitizing since object individuation resources are easily activated but resistant to deactivation. Conversely, a combination of perceptual and post-perceptual mechanisms can account for the SDEs in estimation, as both perceptual and post-perceptual interference can reduce the SDEs. Critically, a novel type of SDE characterized by reduced processing precision is found in subitizing only, implying that the continuity and stability of numerical processing can be dissociable in dynamic situations where numerical information is integrated over time. Our findings reveal the multifaceted nature of SDE mechanisms and suggest their engagement with cognitive modules likely subserving different functionalities.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"41"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142910998","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The CASE of brand names during sentence reading. 品牌名称在句子阅读中的案例分析。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-30 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02070-4
Melanie Labusch, Manuel Perea

Brand names typically maintain a distinctive letter case (e.g., IKEA, Google). This element is essential for theoretical (word recognition models) and practical (brand design) reasons. In abstractionist models, letter case is considered irrelevant, whereas instance-based models use surface information like letter case during lexical retrieval. Previous brand identification tasks reported faster responses to brands in their characteristic letter case (e.g., IKEA and Google faster than ikea and GOOGLE), favoring instance-based models. We examined whether this pattern can be generalized to normal sentence reading: Participants read sentences in which well-known brand names were presented intact (e.g., IKEA, Google) or with a modified letter case (e.g., Ikea, GOOGLE). Results showed a cost for brands written in uppercase, independently of their characteristic letter case, in early eye fixation measures (probability of first-fixation, first-fixation duration). However, for later measures (gaze duration and total times), fixation times were longer when the brand's letter case was modified, restricted to those brands typically written in lowercase (e.g., GOOGLE > Google, whereas Ikea ≲ IKEA). Thus, during sentence reading, both the actual letter case and the typical letter case of brand names interact dynamically, posing problems for abstractionist models of reading.

品牌名称通常保持独特的字母大小写(例如,IKEA, b谷歌)。这个元素对于理论(单词识别模型)和实践(品牌设计)来说都是必不可少的。在抽象主义模型中,字母大小写被认为是不相关的,而基于实例的模型在词汇检索过程中使用字母大小写等表面信息。先前的品牌识别任务报告了对其特征字母案例的品牌的更快反应(例如,IKEA和谷歌比IKEA和谷歌快),更倾向于基于实例的模型。我们研究了这种模式是否可以推广到正常的句子阅读:参与者阅读的句子中,知名品牌名称是完整的(例如,IKEA,谷歌),或者是修改了的字母大小写(例如,IKEA,谷歌)。结果显示,在早期眼睛注视测量(首次注视的概率,首次注视的持续时间)中,用大写字母书写的品牌的成本与它们的特征字母大小写无关。然而,对于后来的测量(凝视持续时间和总时间),当品牌的字母大小写被修改时,注视时间更长,仅限于那些通常以小写书写的品牌(例如,谷歌>谷歌,而Ikea > Ikea)。因此,在句子阅读过程中,品牌名称的实际字母格和典型字母格是动态互动的,这给抽象主义的阅读模式带来了问题。
{"title":"The CASE of brand names during sentence reading.","authors":"Melanie Labusch, Manuel Perea","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02070-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-024-02070-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Brand names typically maintain a distinctive letter case (e.g., IKEA, Google). This element is essential for theoretical (word recognition models) and practical (brand design) reasons. In abstractionist models, letter case is considered irrelevant, whereas instance-based models use surface information like letter case during lexical retrieval. Previous brand identification tasks reported faster responses to brands in their characteristic letter case (e.g., IKEA and Google faster than ikea and GOOGLE), favoring instance-based models. We examined whether this pattern can be generalized to normal sentence reading: Participants read sentences in which well-known brand names were presented intact (e.g., IKEA, Google) or with a modified letter case (e.g., Ikea, GOOGLE). Results showed a cost for brands written in uppercase, independently of their characteristic letter case, in early eye fixation measures (probability of first-fixation, first-fixation duration). However, for later measures (gaze duration and total times), fixation times were longer when the brand's letter case was modified, restricted to those brands typically written in lowercase (e.g., GOOGLE > Google, whereas Ikea ≲ IKEA). Thus, during sentence reading, both the actual letter case and the typical letter case of brand names interact dynamically, posing problems for abstractionist models of reading.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"40"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142910999","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are three zebras more than three frogs: examining conceptual and physical congruency in numerosity judgements of familiar objects. 三只斑马比三只青蛙多吗:在熟悉物体的数量判断中检验概念和物理一致性。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-28 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02044-6
Mila Marinova, Bert Reynvoet

Researchers in numerical cognition have extensively studied the number sense-the innate human ability to extract numerical information from the environment quickly and effortlessly. Much of this research, however, uses abstract stimuli (e.g., dot configurations) that are also strictly controlled for their low-level visual confounds, such as size. Nonetheless, individuals rarely extract numerical information from abstract stimuli in everyday life. Yet, numerical judgments of familiar objects remain poorly understood and understudied. In the current study, we examined the cognitive mechanisms underlying the numerical decisions of familiar objects. In two experiments, we asked adult participants (Experiment 1) and two groups of children (aged 7-9 years and 11-12 years, Experiment 2) to perform an animal numerosity task (i.e., "Which animal is more numerous?"), while the conceptual congruency (i.e., the congruency between an object's real-life size and its numerosity) and physical congruency (the congruency between the number of items and the total space they occupy on the screen) were manipulated. Results showed that the conceptual congruency effect (i.e., better performance when the animal with a larger size in real life is more numerous) and a physical congruency effect (i.e., better performance when the physically larger animal is more numerous) were present in adults and children. However, the effects differed across the age groups and were also a subject of developmental change. To our knowledge, this study is the first one to demonstrate that conceptual knowledge can interfere with numerosity judgements in a top-down manner. This interference effect is distinct from the bottom-up interference effect, which comes from the physical properties of the set. Our results imply that the number sense is not a standalone core system for numbers but is embedded in a more extensive network where both low-level and higher-order influences are possible. We encourage numerical cognition researchers to consider employing not only abstract but also familiar objects when examining numerosity judgements across the lifespan.

数字认知领域的研究人员对数字感进行了广泛的研究,数字感是人类从环境中快速、轻松地提取数字信息的天生能力。然而,大部分研究使用的是抽象的刺激(例如,点的结构),这些刺激也严格控制了它们的低级视觉混淆,例如大小。然而,在日常生活中,个体很少从抽象的刺激中提取数字信息。然而,对熟悉物体的数字判断仍然知之甚少,研究不足。在当前的研究中,我们研究了熟悉物体的数字决策的认知机制。在两个实验中,我们要求成人参与者(实验1)和两组儿童(7-9岁和11-12岁,实验2)执行一个动物数量任务(即“哪个动物的数量更多?”),同时操纵概念一致性(即物体的实际尺寸与其数量之间的一致性)和物理一致性(即物体的数量与其在屏幕上占据的总空间之间的一致性)。结果表明,成人和儿童均存在概念一致性效应(即现实生活中体型较大的动物数量越多,表现越好)和物理一致性效应(即体型较大的动物数量越多,表现越好)。然而,不同年龄组的影响不同,这也是一个发展变化的主题。据我们所知,这项研究是第一个证明概念性知识可以自上而下地干扰数量判断的研究。这种干扰效应不同于自下而上的干扰效应,自下而上的干扰效应来自于集合的物理性质。我们的研究结果表明,数字感觉不是一个独立的数字核心系统,而是嵌入在一个更广泛的网络中,在这个网络中,低级和高阶的影响都是可能的。我们鼓励数值认知研究人员在研究生命周期中的数值判断时,不仅要考虑使用抽象的对象,还要考虑使用熟悉的对象。
{"title":"Are three zebras more than three frogs: examining conceptual and physical congruency in numerosity judgements of familiar objects.","authors":"Mila Marinova, Bert Reynvoet","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02044-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-024-02044-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Researchers in numerical cognition have extensively studied the number sense-the innate human ability to extract numerical information from the environment quickly and effortlessly. Much of this research, however, uses abstract stimuli (e.g., dot configurations) that are also strictly controlled for their low-level visual confounds, such as size. Nonetheless, individuals rarely extract numerical information from abstract stimuli in everyday life. Yet, numerical judgments of familiar objects remain poorly understood and understudied. In the current study, we examined the cognitive mechanisms underlying the numerical decisions of familiar objects. In two experiments, we asked adult participants (Experiment 1) and two groups of children (aged 7-9 years and 11-12 years, Experiment 2) to perform an animal numerosity task (i.e., \"Which animal is more numerous?\"), while the conceptual congruency (i.e., the congruency between an object's real-life size and its numerosity) and physical congruency (the congruency between the number of items and the total space they occupy on the screen) were manipulated. Results showed that the conceptual congruency effect (i.e., better performance when the animal with a larger size in real life is more numerous) and a physical congruency effect (i.e., better performance when the physically larger animal is more numerous) were present in adults and children. However, the effects differed across the age groups and were also a subject of developmental change. To our knowledge, this study is the first one to demonstrate that conceptual knowledge can interfere with numerosity judgements in a top-down manner. This interference effect is distinct from the bottom-up interference effect, which comes from the physical properties of the set. Our results imply that the number sense is not a standalone core system for numbers but is embedded in a more extensive network where both low-level and higher-order influences are possible. We encourage numerical cognition researchers to consider employing not only abstract but also familiar objects when examining numerosity judgements across the lifespan.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"39"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142899171","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
One direction? Cultural aspects of the mental number line beyond reading direction. 一个方向吗?文化方面的心理数列超越了阅读方向。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-23 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02038-4
Merve Bulut, Lilly Roth, Narjes Bahreini, Krzysztof Cipora, Ulf Dietrich Reips, Hans-Christoph Nuerk

Spatial-Numerical Associations (SNAs) refer to the demonstrations of spatial processing of numbers. The Mental Number Line (MNL) is a representation model describing numbers as aligning left-to-right (LR) and was suggested to account for directional biases in participants' responses during numerical tasks. One common behavioral demonstration of this is the Spatial-Numerical Associations of Response Codes (SNARC) effect, which describes faster left-/right-hand responses to smaller/larger numbers, respectively. The MNL, and, consequently, directional SNAs, show variabilities across different cultures. Reading direction is considered to be the main factor in explaining these differences. In line with this, individuals with right-to-left (RL) reading habits show a weaker or even reverse SNARC effect. In the present study, we investigated whether SNAs are influenced not only by reading direction, but also by cultural directional preferences such as drawing lines, arranging objects, imagining objects (i.e., rightward or leftward facing), or representing events in time (i.e., mentally representing the past/future on the left/right, respectively). To test this hypothesis, we measured the cultural directional preferences and the SNARC effect across three cultures in an online setup; German, Turkish, and Iranian. LR preferences in the Cultural Directional Preferences Questionnaire were most prominent in German participants, intermediate in Turkish participants, and least prominent in Iranian participants. In line with this, the LR SNARC effect was strongest in German, intermediate in Turkish, and weakest (but not RL) in Iranian culture. These findings suggest that cultural directional preferences are involved in the emergence of adult SNAs in addition to the reading direction.

空间-数值联想(sna)是对数字的空间处理的一种展示。心理数字线(MNL)是一种描述数字从左到右排列的表征模型,被认为可以解释参与者在数字任务中反应的方向性偏差。一个常见的行为证明是反应代码的空间-数字关联效应(SNARC),它分别描述了对较小/较大数字的更快的左/右反应。MNL,因此,定向sna,在不同的文化中表现出多样性。阅读方向被认为是解释这些差异的主要因素。与此相一致的是,具有从右向左阅读习惯的个体表现出较弱甚至相反的SNARC效应。在本研究中,我们调查了sna是否不仅受到阅读方向的影响,还受到文化方向偏好的影响,如绘制线条、排列物体、想象物体(即朝右或朝左)或在时间上代表事件(即分别在左/右心理上代表过去/未来)。为了验证这一假设,我们在一个在线设置中测量了三种文化的文化定向偏好和SNARC效应;德语、土耳其语和伊朗语。文化方向偏好问卷中的LR偏好在德国参与者中最突出,在土耳其参与者中居中,在伊朗参与者中最不突出。与此一致,LR SNARC效应在德语中最强,在土耳其语中居中,在伊朗文化中最弱(但不是RL)。这些发现表明,除了阅读方向外,文化方向偏好也参与了成人sna的出现。
{"title":"One direction? Cultural aspects of the mental number line beyond reading direction.","authors":"Merve Bulut, Lilly Roth, Narjes Bahreini, Krzysztof Cipora, Ulf Dietrich Reips, Hans-Christoph Nuerk","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02038-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02038-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Spatial-Numerical Associations (SNAs) refer to the demonstrations of spatial processing of numbers. The Mental Number Line (MNL) is a representation model describing numbers as aligning left-to-right (LR) and was suggested to account for directional biases in participants' responses during numerical tasks. One common behavioral demonstration of this is the Spatial-Numerical Associations of Response Codes (SNARC) effect, which describes faster left-/right-hand responses to smaller/larger numbers, respectively. The MNL, and, consequently, directional SNAs, show variabilities across different cultures. Reading direction is considered to be the main factor in explaining these differences. In line with this, individuals with right-to-left (RL) reading habits show a weaker or even reverse SNARC effect. In the present study, we investigated whether SNAs are influenced not only by reading direction, but also by cultural directional preferences such as drawing lines, arranging objects, imagining objects (i.e., rightward or leftward facing), or representing events in time (i.e., mentally representing the past/future on the left/right, respectively). To test this hypothesis, we measured the cultural directional preferences and the SNARC effect across three cultures in an online setup; German, Turkish, and Iranian. LR preferences in the Cultural Directional Preferences Questionnaire were most prominent in German participants, intermediate in Turkish participants, and least prominent in Iranian participants. In line with this, the LR SNARC effect was strongest in German, intermediate in Turkish, and weakest (but not RL) in Iranian culture. These findings suggest that cultural directional preferences are involved in the emergence of adult SNAs in addition to the reading direction.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"37"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11663824/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142878199","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The differential outcomes procedure for improving the recognition of dynamic facial expressions of emotion in people with autism spectrum disorders and first-degree relatives. 提高自闭症谱系障碍患者和一级亲属对动态面部情绪表情识别的差异结果程序。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-23 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02067-z
Ángel García-Pérez, Antonio González-Rodríguez, Marta Godoy-Giménez, Pablo Sayans-Jiménez, Fernando Cañadas, Angeles F Estévez

Previous research highlights impairments in the recognition of facial expression of emotion in individuals diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Relatives of people with ASD may exhibit similar, albeit subtler, impairments, referred to as the Broad Autism Phenotype (BAP). Recently, the Differential outcomes procedure (DOP) has been shown to enhance this ability in young adults using dynamic stimuli, with fewer intensity levels required to identify fear and surprise. The present study aimed to extend these findings to adults diagnosed with ASD (ASD group), and relatives of people diagnosed with ASD (BAP group). A Bayesian Generalized Linear Model was employed for statistical inference. The results indicated that the ASD DOP group performed worse than the BAP DOP group in fear trials. The social dimension of autism negatively impacted performance in some conditions, while positive relationships were found between the repetitive behavior dimension and performance for the ASD group. The opposite pattern was observed in the BAP group. These results suggest the importance of considering different dimensions of autism when conducting research on its relationship with other variables. Finally, participants in both ASD and BAP groups required less intensity to identify certain emotions when the DOP was applied, highlighting its potential utility for improving dynamic facial emotion recognition.

先前的研究强调了自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)患者在识别面部情绪表情方面的障碍。ASD患者的亲属可能表现出类似的,尽管更微妙的缺陷,被称为广泛自闭症表型(BAP)。最近,差分结果程序(DOP)已被证明可以通过使用动态刺激来增强年轻人的这种能力,而识别恐惧和惊讶所需的强度水平更低。本研究旨在将这些发现扩展到诊断为ASD的成年人(ASD组)和诊断为ASD的人的亲属(BAP组)。采用贝叶斯广义线性模型进行统计推断。结果表明,ASD DOP组在恐惧试验中的表现比BAP DOP组差。自闭症的社会维度在某些情况下对表现有负向影响,而重复行为维度对ASD组的表现有正向影响。在BAP组中观察到相反的模式。这些结果表明,在研究自闭症与其他变量的关系时,考虑自闭症的不同维度是很重要的。最后,当应用DOP时,ASD组和BAP组的参与者需要更少的强度来识别某些情绪,这突出了它在改善动态面部情绪识别方面的潜在效用。
{"title":"The differential outcomes procedure for improving the recognition of dynamic facial expressions of emotion in people with autism spectrum disorders and first-degree relatives.","authors":"Ángel García-Pérez, Antonio González-Rodríguez, Marta Godoy-Giménez, Pablo Sayans-Jiménez, Fernando Cañadas, Angeles F Estévez","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02067-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-024-02067-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research highlights impairments in the recognition of facial expression of emotion in individuals diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Relatives of people with ASD may exhibit similar, albeit subtler, impairments, referred to as the Broad Autism Phenotype (BAP). Recently, the Differential outcomes procedure (DOP) has been shown to enhance this ability in young adults using dynamic stimuli, with fewer intensity levels required to identify fear and surprise. The present study aimed to extend these findings to adults diagnosed with ASD (ASD group), and relatives of people diagnosed with ASD (BAP group). A Bayesian Generalized Linear Model was employed for statistical inference. The results indicated that the ASD DOP group performed worse than the BAP DOP group in fear trials. The social dimension of autism negatively impacted performance in some conditions, while positive relationships were found between the repetitive behavior dimension and performance for the ASD group. The opposite pattern was observed in the BAP group. These results suggest the importance of considering different dimensions of autism when conducting research on its relationship with other variables. Finally, participants in both ASD and BAP groups required less intensity to identify certain emotions when the DOP was applied, highlighting its potential utility for improving dynamic facial emotion recognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"38"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142878202","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Naturalistic multimodal emotion data with deep learning can advance the theoretical understanding of emotion. 基于深度学习的自然多模态情感数据可以促进对情感的理论理解。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-21 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02068-y
Thanakorn Angkasirisan

What are emotions? Despite being a century-old question, emotion scientists have yet to agree on what emotions exactly are. Emotions are diversely conceptualised as innate responses (evolutionary view), mental constructs (constructivist view), cognitive evaluations (appraisal view), or self-organising states (dynamical systems view). This enduring fragmentation likely stems from the limitations of traditional research methods, which often adopt narrow methodological approaches. Methods from artificial intelligence (AI), particularly those leveraging big data and deep learning, offer promising approaches for overcoming these limitations. By integrating data from multimodal markers of emotion, including subjective experiences, contextual factors, brain-bodily physiological signals and expressive behaviours, deep learning algorithms can uncover and map their complex relationships within multidimensional spaces. This multimodal emotion framework has the potential to provide novel, nuanced insights into long-standing questions, such as whether emotion categories are innate or learned and whether emotions exhibit coherence or degeneracy, thereby refining emotion theories. Significant challenges remain, particularly in obtaining comprehensive naturalistic multimodal emotion data, highlighting the need for advances in synchronous measurement of naturalistic multimodal emotion.

什么是情绪?尽管这是一个有百年历史的问题,但情感科学家们尚未就情感到底是什么达成一致。情绪被不同地概念化为先天反应(进化观点)、心理构念(建构主义观点)、认知评价(评价观点)或自组织状态(动力系统观点)。这种持续的分裂可能源于传统研究方法的局限性,传统研究方法通常采用狭隘的方法方法。人工智能(AI)的方法,特别是利用大数据和深度学习的方法,为克服这些限制提供了有希望的方法。通过整合来自多模态情感标记的数据,包括主观体验、情境因素、脑-身体生理信号和表达行为,深度学习算法可以在多维空间中发现并绘制它们之间的复杂关系。这种多模态情绪框架有可能为长期存在的问题提供新颖、细致的见解,例如情绪类别是天生的还是后天的,以及情绪是否表现出连贯性或退化,从而完善情绪理论。重大的挑战仍然存在,特别是在获得全面的自然多模态情绪数据方面,突出了在同步测量自然多模态情绪方面取得进展的必要性。
{"title":"Naturalistic multimodal emotion data with deep learning can advance the theoretical understanding of emotion.","authors":"Thanakorn Angkasirisan","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02068-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02068-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>What are emotions? Despite being a century-old question, emotion scientists have yet to agree on what emotions exactly are. Emotions are diversely conceptualised as innate responses (evolutionary view), mental constructs (constructivist view), cognitive evaluations (appraisal view), or self-organising states (dynamical systems view). This enduring fragmentation likely stems from the limitations of traditional research methods, which often adopt narrow methodological approaches. Methods from artificial intelligence (AI), particularly those leveraging big data and deep learning, offer promising approaches for overcoming these limitations. By integrating data from multimodal markers of emotion, including subjective experiences, contextual factors, brain-bodily physiological signals and expressive behaviours, deep learning algorithms can uncover and map their complex relationships within multidimensional spaces. This multimodal emotion framework has the potential to provide novel, nuanced insights into long-standing questions, such as whether emotion categories are innate or learned and whether emotions exhibit coherence or degeneracy, thereby refining emotion theories. Significant challenges remain, particularly in obtaining comprehensive naturalistic multimodal emotion data, highlighting the need for advances in synchronous measurement of naturalistic multimodal emotion.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"36"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11663169/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142873013","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does cognitive inhibition contribute to working memory and reasoning during childhood? 认知抑制是否有助于儿童时期的工作记忆和推理?
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-19 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02066-0
Yesica Sabina Aydmune, María Fernanda López-Ramón, Eliana Vanesa Zamora, Lorena Canet Juric, Isabel María Introzzi

Theoretical frameworks suggest that cognitive inhibition suppresses irrelevant information in working memory, preventing overload and promoting the processing of task-relevant information. Consequently, it may also contribute to more complex skills, such as abstract reasoning, by facilitating the retention and processing of patterns and relationships. However, empirical evidence does not consistently show these relationships in early elementary school years. This study aims to examine the validity of the following theoretical proposition: cognitive inhibition is a fundamental process that influences working memory, and both contribute to abstract reasoning in children aged 6-8 years. The final sample included 293 schoolchildren from 1st, 2nd, and 3rd grades, who completed tasks measuring cognitive inhibition, working memory, and reasoning. Age was also considered in the analyses. The main results indicate that age is associated with improvements in working memory and reasoning (explaining 19% of the variance), but not with cognitive inhibition performance. Additionally, cognitive inhibition directly contributes to working memory (explaining 19% of the variance), and working memory, but not cognitive inhibition, contributes to abstract reasoning (the model explains 23% of the variance). No indirect effects were found. We discuss the importance of incorporating specific relationships between cognitive skills at different developmental stages into theoretical and practical proposals.

理论框架认为,认知抑制抑制了工作记忆中的不相关信息,防止了过载,促进了任务相关信息的加工。因此,它也可能有助于更复杂的技能,如抽象推理,通过促进模式和关系的保留和处理。然而,经验证据并没有一致地表明这些关系在小学早期。本研究旨在检验以下理论命题的有效性:认知抑制是影响工作记忆的基本过程,两者都有助于6-8岁儿童的抽象推理。最后的样本包括293名来自一年级、二年级和三年级的学生,他们完成了测量认知抑制、工作记忆和推理的任务。年龄也在分析中被考虑。主要结果表明,年龄与工作记忆和推理能力的提高有关(解释了19%的差异),但与认知抑制表现无关。此外,认知抑制直接影响了工作记忆(解释了19%的方差),而工作记忆,而不是认知抑制,影响了抽象推理(该模型解释了23%的方差)。没有发现间接影响。我们讨论了将不同发展阶段的认知技能之间的特定关系纳入理论和实践建议的重要性。
{"title":"Does cognitive inhibition contribute to working memory and reasoning during childhood?","authors":"Yesica Sabina Aydmune, María Fernanda López-Ramón, Eliana Vanesa Zamora, Lorena Canet Juric, Isabel María Introzzi","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02066-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-024-02066-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Theoretical frameworks suggest that cognitive inhibition suppresses irrelevant information in working memory, preventing overload and promoting the processing of task-relevant information. Consequently, it may also contribute to more complex skills, such as abstract reasoning, by facilitating the retention and processing of patterns and relationships. However, empirical evidence does not consistently show these relationships in early elementary school years. This study aims to examine the validity of the following theoretical proposition: cognitive inhibition is a fundamental process that influences working memory, and both contribute to abstract reasoning in children aged 6-8 years. The final sample included 293 schoolchildren from 1st, 2nd, and 3rd grades, who completed tasks measuring cognitive inhibition, working memory, and reasoning. Age was also considered in the analyses. The main results indicate that age is associated with improvements in working memory and reasoning (explaining 19% of the variance), but not with cognitive inhibition performance. Additionally, cognitive inhibition directly contributes to working memory (explaining 19% of the variance), and working memory, but not cognitive inhibition, contributes to abstract reasoning (the model explains 23% of the variance). No indirect effects were found. We discuss the importance of incorporating specific relationships between cognitive skills at different developmental stages into theoretical and practical proposals.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"35"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142856095","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Implications of neural integration of math and spatial experiences for math ability and math anxiety. 数学和空间体验的神经整合对数学能力和数学焦虑的影响。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-10 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02063-3
Raeanne N Martell, Richard J Daker, H Moriah Sokolowski, Daniel Ansari, Ian M Lyons

Mathematical and spatial abilities are positively related at both the behavioral and neural levels. Much of the evidence illuminating this relationship comes from classic laboratory-based experimental methods focused on cognitive performance despite most individuals also experiencing math and space in other contexts, such as in conversations or lectures. To broaden our understanding of math-space integration in these more commonplace situations, we used an auditory memory-encoding task with stimuli whose content evoked a range of educational and everyday settings related to math or spatial thinking. We used a multivariate approach to directly assess the extent of neural similarity between activity patterns elicited by these math and spatial stimuli. Results from whole-brain searchlight analysis revealed a highly specific positive relation between math and spatial activity patterns in bilateral anterior hippocampi. Examining individual variation in math-space similarity, we found that greater math-space similarity in bilateral anterior hippocampi was associated with poorer math skills and higher anxiety about math. Integration of neural responses to mathematical and spatial content may not always portend positive outcomes. We suggest that episodic simulation of quotidian contexts may link everyday experiences with math and spatial thinking-and the strength of this link is predictive of math in a manner that diverges from math-space associations derived from more lab-based tasks. On a methodological level, this work points to the value of considering a wider range of experimental paradigms, and of the value of combining multivariate fMRI analysis with behavioral data to better contextualize interpretations of brain data.

数学能力和空间能力在行为和神经水平上都是正相关的。阐明这种关系的许多证据来自经典的基于实验室的实验方法,这些实验方法专注于认知表现,尽管大多数人也在其他环境中体验数学和空间,比如在谈话或讲座中。为了在这些更常见的情况下扩大我们对数学空间整合的理解,我们使用了一个听觉记忆编码任务,其内容唤起了一系列与数学或空间思维相关的教育和日常环境。我们使用多元方法直接评估数学和空间刺激引发的活动模式之间的神经相似性程度。全脑探照灯分析结果显示,数学与双侧海马前部空间活动模式之间存在高度具体的正相关。在检查数学空间相似性的个体差异时,我们发现双侧海马前部的数学空间相似性越大,与较差的数学技能和较高的数学焦虑有关。对数学和空间内容的神经反应的整合可能并不总是预示着积极的结果。我们认为,日常情境的情景模拟可能将日常经历与数学和空间思维联系起来——这种联系的强度在某种程度上预示着数学的发展,这与更多基于实验室的任务产生的数学空间联想不同。在方法学层面上,这项工作指出了考虑更广泛的实验范式的价值,以及将多元功能磁共振成像分析与行为数据相结合的价值,以更好地将大脑数据的解释背景化。
{"title":"Implications of neural integration of math and spatial experiences for math ability and math anxiety.","authors":"Raeanne N Martell, Richard J Daker, H Moriah Sokolowski, Daniel Ansari, Ian M Lyons","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02063-3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-024-02063-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Mathematical and spatial abilities are positively related at both the behavioral and neural levels. Much of the evidence illuminating this relationship comes from classic laboratory-based experimental methods focused on cognitive performance despite most individuals also experiencing math and space in other contexts, such as in conversations or lectures. To broaden our understanding of math-space integration in these more commonplace situations, we used an auditory memory-encoding task with stimuli whose content evoked a range of educational and everyday settings related to math or spatial thinking. We used a multivariate approach to directly assess the extent of neural similarity between activity patterns elicited by these math and spatial stimuli. Results from whole-brain searchlight analysis revealed a highly specific positive relation between math and spatial activity patterns in bilateral anterior hippocampi. Examining individual variation in math-space similarity, we found that greater math-space similarity in bilateral anterior hippocampi was associated with poorer math skills and higher anxiety about math. Integration of neural responses to mathematical and spatial content may not always portend positive outcomes. We suggest that episodic simulation of quotidian contexts may link everyday experiences with math and spatial thinking-and the strength of this link is predictive of math in a manner that diverges from math-space associations derived from more lab-based tasks. On a methodological level, this work points to the value of considering a wider range of experimental paradigms, and of the value of combining multivariate fMRI analysis with behavioral data to better contextualize interpretations of brain data.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"34"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142802771","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An ERP study on multiplication and its relationship to phonological processing in children and adults. 儿童和成人乘法及其与语音加工关系的ERP研究。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-06 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02036-6
Heather G Simpson, Lisa M Henderson, Silke M Göbel

Associations between arithmetic and reading skills suggest that these important abilities may rely, at least in part, on shared neurocognitive processes. It has been argued that retrieval of arithmetic facts may rely on phonological processing; however, very few studies have explored this association using neural indices and whether it manifests similarly in children and adults. Here we examined event related potentials (ERPs) as an indirect neural correlate of arithmetic fact retrieval, and whether variability in ERP modulation is associated with individual differences in phonological processing (verbal working memory, rate of access, and phonological awareness). Arithmetic processing was examined in two samples with different levels of arithmetic expertise: (1) young adults (n = 24; Mage = 21.8 years); and (2) children (n = 25; Mage = 11.2 years). Participants were presented with simple multiplication equations that were correct or incorrect. Significant modulations of the ERPs by correctness were found at posterior electrodes in both samples, however, in different components. In adults a modulation of the P300 was observed, while for children the N400 response was modulated. For both children and adults, the size of the ERP modulation in posterior electrodes was associated with individual differences in verbal working memory. These results highlight an important distinction between behavioral outcomes and their underlying neurocognitive mechanisms. Additionally, they provide insight into how arithmetic processing evolves over the course of development.

算术和阅读技能之间的联系表明,这些重要的能力可能至少部分地依赖于共同的神经认知过程。有人认为,算术事实的检索可能依赖于语音加工;然而,很少有研究使用神经指数来探索这种关联,以及它是否在儿童和成人中表现相似。在这里,我们研究了事件相关电位(ERP)作为算术事实检索的间接神经关联,以及ERP调制的可变性是否与语音处理(言语工作记忆、访问率和语音意识)的个体差异有关。在两个不同算术专业水平的样本中检验了算术处理:(1)年轻人(n = 24;法师= 21.8岁);(2)儿童(n = 25;法师= 11.2年)。研究人员向参与者展示了正确或错误的简单乘法方程。然而,在两个样品中,在不同的成分中,在后电极上发现了ERPs的显著调节。在成人中,观察到P300的调节,而在儿童中,N400反应被调节。对于儿童和成人,后电极ERP调制的大小与言语工作记忆的个体差异有关。这些结果强调了行为结果与其潜在的神经认知机制之间的重要区别。此外,它们还提供了关于算术处理在开发过程中如何演变的见解。
{"title":"An ERP study on multiplication and its relationship to phonological processing in children and adults.","authors":"Heather G Simpson, Lisa M Henderson, Silke M Göbel","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02036-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02036-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Associations between arithmetic and reading skills suggest that these important abilities may rely, at least in part, on shared neurocognitive processes. It has been argued that retrieval of arithmetic facts may rely on phonological processing; however, very few studies have explored this association using neural indices and whether it manifests similarly in children and adults. Here we examined event related potentials (ERPs) as an indirect neural correlate of arithmetic fact retrieval, and whether variability in ERP modulation is associated with individual differences in phonological processing (verbal working memory, rate of access, and phonological awareness). Arithmetic processing was examined in two samples with different levels of arithmetic expertise: (1) young adults (n = 24; M<sub>age</sub> = 21.8 years); and (2) children (n = 25; M<sub>age</sub> = 11.2 years). Participants were presented with simple multiplication equations that were correct or incorrect. Significant modulations of the ERPs by correctness were found at posterior electrodes in both samples, however, in different components. In adults a modulation of the P300 was observed, while for children the N400 response was modulated. For both children and adults, the size of the ERP modulation in posterior electrodes was associated with individual differences in verbal working memory. These results highlight an important distinction between behavioral outcomes and their underlying neurocognitive mechanisms. Additionally, they provide insight into how arithmetic processing evolves over the course of development.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"33"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11621157/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142787292","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Number, size, and space associated in a common system by distinct mechanisms. 通过不同的机制在一个共同系统中关联的数量、大小和空间。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-05 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02052-6
Bihua Cao, Zhenwei Su, Fang Yi, Fuhong Li

The spatial numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect demonstrates that people respond faster to small numbers with their left hand and faster to large numbers with their right hand. The size congruity effect (SCE) refers to the fact that congruent trials between numerical values and physical sizes are faster than incongruent trials. Previous studies have found that the SNARC effect and SCE are independent when magnitudes or sizes are processed explicitly. This study aimed to explore whether number, size, and space are common and distinct mechanisms using an implicit parity judgment task. The results showed that the SNARC effect, SCE, and SNARC-like effect all co-existed. Furthermore, there was a significant interaction between the SNARC effect and SCE, in which the SNARC effect in the SCE-congruent condition was larger than in the SCE-incongruent condition, whereas SCE merely emerged in the SNARC-compatible trials. However, participants responded to small numbers in large size faster than to large numbers in small size with the left hand in SCE-incongruent trials, which reflected that number-space mapping (SNARC effect) was stronger than size-space mapping (SNARC-like effect). These findings provide new evidence for A Theory of Magnitude (ATOM), which suggests that number, size, and space are associated with a common generalized magnitude system through distinct mechanisms.

空间数字关联反应码(SNARC)效应表明,人们用左手对小数的反应更快,而用右手对大数的反应更快。尺寸一致性效应(size congruity effect, SCE)是指数值与物理尺寸之间的一致性试验比不一致性试验更快。以往的研究发现,当明确处理震级或大小时,SNARC效应和SCE是独立的。本研究旨在探讨是否数量,大小和空间是共同的和独特的机制使用隐式奇偶判断任务。结果表明,SNARC效应、SCE效应和SNARC样效应同时存在。此外,SNARC效应与SCE之间存在显著的相互作用,其中SCE一致条件下的SNARC效应大于SCE不一致条件下的SNARC效应,而SCE仅在SCE兼容的试验中出现。然而,在sce不一致实验中,被试用左手对大尺寸小数的反应速度要快于对小尺寸小数的反应速度,这反映了数字空间映射(SNARC效应)强于大小空间映射(SNARC样效应)。这些发现为数量级理论(ATOM)提供了新的证据,该理论认为,数量、大小和空间通过不同的机制与一个共同的广义数量级系统相关联。
{"title":"Number, size, and space associated in a common system by distinct mechanisms.","authors":"Bihua Cao, Zhenwei Su, Fang Yi, Fuhong Li","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02052-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-024-02052-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The spatial numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect demonstrates that people respond faster to small numbers with their left hand and faster to large numbers with their right hand. The size congruity effect (SCE) refers to the fact that congruent trials between numerical values and physical sizes are faster than incongruent trials. Previous studies have found that the SNARC effect and SCE are independent when magnitudes or sizes are processed explicitly. This study aimed to explore whether number, size, and space are common and distinct mechanisms using an implicit parity judgment task. The results showed that the SNARC effect, SCE, and SNARC-like effect all co-existed. Furthermore, there was a significant interaction between the SNARC effect and SCE, in which the SNARC effect in the SCE-congruent condition was larger than in the SCE-incongruent condition, whereas SCE merely emerged in the SNARC-compatible trials. However, participants responded to small numbers in large size faster than to large numbers in small size with the left hand in SCE-incongruent trials, which reflected that number-space mapping (SNARC effect) was stronger than size-space mapping (SNARC-like effect). These findings provide new evidence for A Theory of Magnitude (ATOM), which suggests that number, size, and space are associated with a common generalized magnitude system through distinct mechanisms.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"32"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142787299","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1