首页 > 最新文献

Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung最新文献

英文 中文
Start of a new editor-in-chief. 新主编的开始。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-18 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02072-2
Tilo Strobach
{"title":"Start of a new editor-in-chief.","authors":"Tilo Strobach","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02072-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02072-2","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"45"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11742733/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143014232","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The benefit of extrinsic motivation on effortful cognitive control is influenced by need for cognition. 外在动机对努力性认知控制的益处受认知需要的影响。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-18 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02074-0
Qian Yang, Ruoke Xu, Lijie Zhang, Lei Qiao

Extrinsic motivation can foster effortful cognitive control. Moreover, the selective coupling of extrinsic motivation on low- versus high-control demands tasks would exert an additional impact. However, to what extent their influences are further modulated by the level of Need for Cognition (NFC) remains unclear. Thus, the current study sought to address this question. To this end, we conducted two behavioral experiments wherein cognitive control was triggered by the confound-minimized Stroop task and the NFC questionnaire was administered. Two different forms of extrinsic motivation were manipulated at the block level. In Experiment 1, extrinsic motivation was triggered by evaluative feedback. In Experiment 2, extrinsic motivation was triggered by reward incentives, while evaluative feedback was selectively coupled with low (congruent)- or high (incongruent)- control demands trials. The results indicated that two forms of extrinsic motivation (evaluative feedback vs. reward incentives) presented distinctive effects on effortful cognitive control; while their benefits on overall performance were further influenced by NFC. Interestingly, when incongruent rather than congruent trials were selectively coupled with reward incentives, not only conflict processing, but also overall performance for low-NFC participants only, benefited from this scenario. Taken together, the current study shows that extrinsic motivation can boost cognitive control, which gain was further reduced by high NFC.

外在动机可以促进努力的认知控制。此外,外在动机对低控制要求任务和高控制要求任务的选择性耦合会产生额外的影响。然而,它们的影响在多大程度上受到认知需要水平的进一步调节尚不清楚。因此,目前的研究试图解决这个问题。为此,我们进行了两个行为实验,其中认知控制由混淆最小化Stroop任务触发,NFC问卷管理。在块水平上操纵两种不同形式的外在动机。在实验1中,外在动机由评价反馈触发。在实验2中,外在动机由奖励激励触发,而评价反馈则选择性地与低(一致)或高(不一致)控制需求试验相结合。结果表明,两种外在动机(评价反馈和奖励激励)对努力认知控制有显著的影响;而它们对整体性能的好处进一步受到NFC的影响。有趣的是,当不一致而不是一致的试验选择性地与奖励激励相结合时,不仅冲突处理,而且低nfc参与者的整体表现也从这种情况中受益。综上所述,目前的研究表明,外在动机可以促进认知控制,而高NFC会进一步降低认知控制的增益。
{"title":"The benefit of extrinsic motivation on effortful cognitive control is influenced by need for cognition.","authors":"Qian Yang, Ruoke Xu, Lijie Zhang, Lei Qiao","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02074-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02074-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Extrinsic motivation can foster effortful cognitive control. Moreover, the selective coupling of extrinsic motivation on low- versus high-control demands tasks would exert an additional impact. However, to what extent their influences are further modulated by the level of Need for Cognition (NFC) remains unclear. Thus, the current study sought to address this question. To this end, we conducted two behavioral experiments wherein cognitive control was triggered by the confound-minimized Stroop task and the NFC questionnaire was administered. Two different forms of extrinsic motivation were manipulated at the block level. In Experiment 1, extrinsic motivation was triggered by evaluative feedback. In Experiment 2, extrinsic motivation was triggered by reward incentives, while evaluative feedback was selectively coupled with low (congruent)- or high (incongruent)- control demands trials. The results indicated that two forms of extrinsic motivation (evaluative feedback vs. reward incentives) presented distinctive effects on effortful cognitive control; while their benefits on overall performance were further influenced by NFC. Interestingly, when incongruent rather than congruent trials were selectively coupled with reward incentives, not only conflict processing, but also overall performance for low-NFC participants only, benefited from this scenario. Taken together, the current study shows that extrinsic motivation can boost cognitive control, which gain was further reduced by high NFC.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"46"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143014237","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Binding a stimulus after, but not before, response execution: examining the temporal binding window of event files. 在响应执行之后而不是之前绑定刺激:检查事件文件的临时绑定窗口。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-09 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-025-02077-5
Sihan He, Jay Pratt

Efficient and flexible responses are essential for successfully interacting with the environment. These interactions require an instantaneous integration of visual stimuli and responses, known as 'stimulus-response binding' (SR binding). SR binding is considered part of a holistic temporary representation, the event file, that integrates the stimulus, the response, and the action effect produced by this response. It is commonly assumed that an event file (or at least the SR binding) would end with the execution of the response or its action effect. This, however, has never been directly tested. Here, we tested whether the SR binding can be formed between another temporally close stimulus that people didn't respond to by implementing a sequence of two stimuli where participants needed to respond only to the first or second stimulus. Our results indicate that a binding between the response and this temporally close stimulus can occur, but only when it's placed after, and not before, the response execution. This finding suggests that the event file might operate a temporal binding window that is not decisively terminated by the response execution. Further, this insight into the temporal dynamics of the event file highlights the temporal flexibility of the SR binding and, thus, the need for careful consideration of its sub-structures and durability.

有效和灵活的反应对于成功地与环境互动至关重要。这些相互作用需要视觉刺激和反应的瞬间整合,称为“刺激-反应结合”(SR结合)。SR绑定被认为是整体临时表示的一部分,即事件文件,它集成了刺激、响应和由该响应产生的动作效果。通常假设事件文件(或至少SR绑定)将随着响应或其操作效果的执行而结束。然而,这一点从未被直接验证过。在这里,我们测试了SR绑定是否可以在另一个暂时关闭的刺激之间形成,人们没有反应,通过实施两个刺激序列,参与者只需要对第一个或第二个刺激做出反应。我们的研究结果表明,在反应和这个暂时关闭的刺激之间可以发生绑定,但只有当它被放置在反应执行之后,而不是之前。这一发现表明,事件文件可能操作一个临时绑定窗口,该窗口没有被响应执行果断地终止。此外,这种对事件文件时间动态的洞察突出了SR绑定的时间灵活性,因此需要仔细考虑其子结构和持久性。
{"title":"Binding a stimulus after, but not before, response execution: examining the temporal binding window of event files.","authors":"Sihan He, Jay Pratt","doi":"10.1007/s00426-025-02077-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-025-02077-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Efficient and flexible responses are essential for successfully interacting with the environment. These interactions require an instantaneous integration of visual stimuli and responses, known as 'stimulus-response binding' (SR binding). SR binding is considered part of a holistic temporary representation, the event file, that integrates the stimulus, the response, and the action effect produced by this response. It is commonly assumed that an event file (or at least the SR binding) would end with the execution of the response or its action effect. This, however, has never been directly tested. Here, we tested whether the SR binding can be formed between another temporally close stimulus that people didn't respond to by implementing a sequence of two stimuli where participants needed to respond only to the first or second stimulus. Our results indicate that a binding between the response and this temporally close stimulus can occur, but only when it's placed after, and not before, the response execution. This finding suggests that the event file might operate a temporal binding window that is not decisively terminated by the response execution. Further, this insight into the temporal dynamics of the event file highlights the temporal flexibility of the SR binding and, thus, the need for careful consideration of its sub-structures and durability.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"44"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142956671","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The power of music: impact on EEG signals. 音乐的力量:对脑电图信号的影响。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-06 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02060-6
Basma Bahgat El Sayed, Mye Ali Basheer, Marwa Safwat Shalaby, Hala Rashad El Habashy, Saly Hasan Elkholy

Introduction: Music is known to impact attentional state without conscious awareness. Listening to music encourages the brain to secrete neurotransmitters improving cognition and emotion.

Aim of work: Analysis of QEEG band width while listening to two music types, identifying different cortical areas activated and which genre has a similar effect to relaxed EEG.

Methods: This is a cross-section interventional analytic study including 76 normal subjects, 55 of them are females (72.37%). Participants listened to 10 min of a single audio track during EEG recording, consisting of (1 min of silence, 3 min of Egyptian folk music, 3 min of silence, then 3 min of Egyptian instrumental classic music (without any lyrics). We analyzed QEEG bands at each brain region during different tracks. The power ratio index (PRI) was calculated for each region, and then the interhemispheric difference was compared.

Results: The participants' ages ranged from 15 to 26 with a mean 16.73 ± 2.37 years. PRI showed a significant increase in the frontal and occipital regions during listening to folk music compared to the silent epoch, where p < 0.001 and p = 0.023, respectively. In the frontal and temporal regions, the classic music epoch evoked the highest PRI interhemispheric difference compared to the folk music epoch, where p = 0.004 and p < 0.001, respectively.

Conclusion: Egyptian folk music has significantly slowed the brain rhythm, particularly in the frontal region, compared to classic music, supporting the hypothesis of a momentary reduction of cognitive capacities by the noise. Classic music was evidently associated with a relaxed state EEG.

导读:众所周知,音乐可以在无意识的情况下影响注意力状态。听音乐可以促进大脑分泌神经递质,改善认知和情绪。工作目的:分析听两种类型音乐时QEEG的频带宽度,识别不同的皮层区域被激活,以及哪种类型的音乐与放松时的EEG效果相似。方法:对76例正常受试者进行横断面介入分析研究,其中女性55例(72.37%)。在脑电图记录过程中,参与者听了10分钟的单一音轨,包括(1分钟的沉默,3分钟的埃及民间音乐,3分钟的沉默,然后3分钟的埃及器乐古典音乐(没有任何歌词)。我们分析了不同轨迹下各脑区QEEG波段。计算各区域的功率比指数(PRI),比较各区域的脑间差异。结果:参与者年龄15 ~ 26岁,平均年龄16.73±2.37岁。结论:与古典音乐相比,埃及民间音乐显著减缓了大脑节奏,尤其是在额叶区域,这支持了噪音会导致认知能力瞬间降低的假设。古典音乐与放松状态的脑电图明显相关。
{"title":"The power of music: impact on EEG signals.","authors":"Basma Bahgat El Sayed, Mye Ali Basheer, Marwa Safwat Shalaby, Hala Rashad El Habashy, Saly Hasan Elkholy","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02060-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02060-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Music is known to impact attentional state without conscious awareness. Listening to music encourages the brain to secrete neurotransmitters improving cognition and emotion.</p><p><strong>Aim of work: </strong>Analysis of QEEG band width while listening to two music types, identifying different cortical areas activated and which genre has a similar effect to relaxed EEG.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>This is a cross-section interventional analytic study including 76 normal subjects, 55 of them are females (72.37%). Participants listened to 10 min of a single audio track during EEG recording, consisting of (1 min of silence, 3 min of Egyptian folk music, 3 min of silence, then 3 min of Egyptian instrumental classic music (without any lyrics). We analyzed QEEG bands at each brain region during different tracks. The power ratio index (PRI) was calculated for each region, and then the interhemispheric difference was compared.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The participants' ages ranged from 15 to 26 with a mean 16.73 ± 2.37 years. PRI showed a significant increase in the frontal and occipital regions during listening to folk music compared to the silent epoch, where p < 0.001 and p = 0.023, respectively. In the frontal and temporal regions, the classic music epoch evoked the highest PRI interhemispheric difference compared to the folk music epoch, where p = 0.004 and p < 0.001, respectively.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Egyptian folk music has significantly slowed the brain rhythm, particularly in the frontal region, compared to classic music, supporting the hypothesis of a momentary reduction of cognitive capacities by the noise. Classic music was evidently associated with a relaxed state EEG.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"42"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11703926/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142933065","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of inhibition in the processing of peripheral cues. 抑制在外周线索加工中的作用。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-06 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02073-1
Christian Büsel, Stephan F Dahm, Pierre Sachse, Ulrich Ansorge

The present study investigated the role of inhibition in peripheral cueing by nonpredictive cues. Based on past findings, we investigated the possibility that inhibition of learned irrelevant cue colors is typical of short cue-target intervals, with more competition for attention capture between cue versus target. In line with the expectation, in a modified contingent-capture protocol, with short cue-target intervals, we found same-location costs (SLCs) - that is, disadvantages for validly cued targets (cue = target position) compared to invalidly cued targets (cue ≠ target position) with consistently colored non-matching cues. In contrast, no such effects for inconsistently colored non-matching cues were observed with short intervals. In a control condition, with longer intervals, the differences between consistently and inconsistently colored cues were no longer observed. We argue that this effect is due to participants proactively inhibiting consistently colored non-matching cues with short intervals but not with long intervals, but that inhibition failed with inconsistently colored non-matching cues that could take on different possible colors. Alternative explanations in terms of object-updating costs or masking were ruled out. We conclude that the currently found type of inhibition of peripheral cues most likely reflected the limitation of proactively established control structures that could be used at the same time.

本研究探讨了非预测性线索对外周线索的抑制作用。基于过去的研究结果,我们研究了学习到的无关线索颜色的抑制是短线索-目标间隔的典型现象,线索和目标之间在注意力捕获方面的竞争更激烈。与预期一致的是,在一个改进的偶然捕获协议中,在短的线索-目标间隔下,我们发现了相同位置成本(SLCs),即有效线索目标(线索=目标位置)与无效线索目标(线索≠目标位置)相比,具有一致颜色的非匹配线索。相比之下,对于颜色不一致的非匹配线索,在短时间间隔内没有观察到这种影响。在控制条件下,在较长的间隔时间内,颜色一致和不一致的线索之间的差异不再被观察到。我们认为,这种效应是由于参与者在短时间间隔内主动抑制颜色一致的非匹配线索,而不是在长时间间隔内,但在颜色不一致的非匹配线索可能呈现不同颜色时,抑制失败。在对象更新成本或遮蔽方面的其他解释被排除在外。我们的结论是,目前发现的外周线索的抑制类型很可能反映了可以同时使用的主动建立的控制结构的局限性。
{"title":"The role of inhibition in the processing of peripheral cues.","authors":"Christian Büsel, Stephan F Dahm, Pierre Sachse, Ulrich Ansorge","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02073-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02073-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present study investigated the role of inhibition in peripheral cueing by nonpredictive cues. Based on past findings, we investigated the possibility that inhibition of learned irrelevant cue colors is typical of short cue-target intervals, with more competition for attention capture between cue versus target. In line with the expectation, in a modified contingent-capture protocol, with short cue-target intervals, we found same-location costs (SLCs) - that is, disadvantages for validly cued targets (cue = target position) compared to invalidly cued targets (cue ≠ target position) with consistently colored non-matching cues. In contrast, no such effects for inconsistently colored non-matching cues were observed with short intervals. In a control condition, with longer intervals, the differences between consistently and inconsistently colored cues were no longer observed. We argue that this effect is due to participants proactively inhibiting consistently colored non-matching cues with short intervals but not with long intervals, but that inhibition failed with inconsistently colored non-matching cues that could take on different possible colors. Alternative explanations in terms of object-updating costs or masking were ruled out. We conclude that the currently found type of inhibition of peripheral cues most likely reflected the limitation of proactively established control structures that could be used at the same time.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"43"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11703931/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142933115","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distinct serial dependence between small and large numerosity processing. 小数和大数处理之间具有明显的序列依赖性。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-31 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02071-3
Yue Huang, Haokun Li, Shiming Qiu, Xianfeng Ding, Min Li, Wangjuan Liu, Zhao Fan, Xiaorong Cheng

The serial dependence effect (SDE) is a perceptual bias where current stimuli are perceived as more similar to recently seen stimuli, possibly enhancing the stability and continuity of visual perception. Although SDE has been observed across many visual features, it remains unclear whether humans rely on a single mechanism of SDE to support numerosity processing across two distinct numerical ranges: subitizing (i.e., small numerosity processing, likely related to early object recognition) and estimation (i.e., large numerosity processing, likely related to ensemble numerosity extraction). Here, we show that subitizing and estimation exhibit distinct SDE patterns. Subitizing is characterized by an asymmetric SDE, whereas estimation demonstrates a symmetric SDE. Specifically, in subitizing, the SDE occurs only when the current magnitude is smaller than the previous magnitude but not when it is larger. In contrast, the SDE in estimation is present in both scenarios. We propose that these differences arise from distinct underlying mechanisms. A perceptual mechanism-namely, a 'temporal hysteresis' account, can explain the asymmetrical SDE in subitizing since object individuation resources are easily activated but resistant to deactivation. Conversely, a combination of perceptual and post-perceptual mechanisms can account for the SDEs in estimation, as both perceptual and post-perceptual interference can reduce the SDEs. Critically, a novel type of SDE characterized by reduced processing precision is found in subitizing only, implying that the continuity and stability of numerical processing can be dissociable in dynamic situations where numerical information is integrated over time. Our findings reveal the multifaceted nature of SDE mechanisms and suggest their engagement with cognitive modules likely subserving different functionalities.

序列依赖效应(SDE)是一种感知偏差,即当前的刺激被认为与最近看到的刺激更相似,可能会增强视觉感知的稳定性和连续性。尽管SDE已经在许多视觉特征中被观察到,但尚不清楚人类是否依赖于SDE的单一机制来支持两个不同数值范围的数字处理:细分(即小数字处理,可能与早期目标识别有关)和估计(即大数字处理,可能与集合数字提取有关)。在这里,我们展示了细分和估计表现出不同的SDE模式。细分的特点是不对称SDE,而估计的特点是对称SDE。具体来说,在细分中,SDE仅在当前量级小于前一个量级时发生,而在当前量级大于前一个量级时不发生。相反,估计中的SDE在两种情况下都存在。我们认为这些差异是由不同的潜在机制引起的。一种感知机制,即“时间滞后”解释,可以解释不对称的SDE,因为对象个性化资源很容易被激活,但不易被停用。相反,知觉和后知觉机制的结合可以解释估计中的sde,因为知觉和后知觉干扰都可以降低sde。关键的是,一种以加工精度降低为特征的新型SDE仅在细分中被发现,这意味着数值处理的连续性和稳定性在数值信息随时间集成的动态情况下可以分离。我们的研究结果揭示了SDE机制的多面性,并表明它们与认知模块的参与可能服务于不同的功能。
{"title":"Distinct serial dependence between small and large numerosity processing.","authors":"Yue Huang, Haokun Li, Shiming Qiu, Xianfeng Ding, Min Li, Wangjuan Liu, Zhao Fan, Xiaorong Cheng","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02071-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02071-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The serial dependence effect (SDE) is a perceptual bias where current stimuli are perceived as more similar to recently seen stimuli, possibly enhancing the stability and continuity of visual perception. Although SDE has been observed across many visual features, it remains unclear whether humans rely on a single mechanism of SDE to support numerosity processing across two distinct numerical ranges: subitizing (i.e., small numerosity processing, likely related to early object recognition) and estimation (i.e., large numerosity processing, likely related to ensemble numerosity extraction). Here, we show that subitizing and estimation exhibit distinct SDE patterns. Subitizing is characterized by an asymmetric SDE, whereas estimation demonstrates a symmetric SDE. Specifically, in subitizing, the SDE occurs only when the current magnitude is smaller than the previous magnitude but not when it is larger. In contrast, the SDE in estimation is present in both scenarios. We propose that these differences arise from distinct underlying mechanisms. A perceptual mechanism-namely, a 'temporal hysteresis' account, can explain the asymmetrical SDE in subitizing since object individuation resources are easily activated but resistant to deactivation. Conversely, a combination of perceptual and post-perceptual mechanisms can account for the SDEs in estimation, as both perceptual and post-perceptual interference can reduce the SDEs. Critically, a novel type of SDE characterized by reduced processing precision is found in subitizing only, implying that the continuity and stability of numerical processing can be dissociable in dynamic situations where numerical information is integrated over time. Our findings reveal the multifaceted nature of SDE mechanisms and suggest their engagement with cognitive modules likely subserving different functionalities.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"41"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142910998","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The CASE of brand names during sentence reading. 品牌名称在句子阅读中的案例分析。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-30 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02070-4
Melanie Labusch, Manuel Perea

Brand names typically maintain a distinctive letter case (e.g., IKEA, Google). This element is essential for theoretical (word recognition models) and practical (brand design) reasons. In abstractionist models, letter case is considered irrelevant, whereas instance-based models use surface information like letter case during lexical retrieval. Previous brand identification tasks reported faster responses to brands in their characteristic letter case (e.g., IKEA and Google faster than ikea and GOOGLE), favoring instance-based models. We examined whether this pattern can be generalized to normal sentence reading: Participants read sentences in which well-known brand names were presented intact (e.g., IKEA, Google) or with a modified letter case (e.g., Ikea, GOOGLE). Results showed a cost for brands written in uppercase, independently of their characteristic letter case, in early eye fixation measures (probability of first-fixation, first-fixation duration). However, for later measures (gaze duration and total times), fixation times were longer when the brand's letter case was modified, restricted to those brands typically written in lowercase (e.g., GOOGLE > Google, whereas Ikea ≲ IKEA). Thus, during sentence reading, both the actual letter case and the typical letter case of brand names interact dynamically, posing problems for abstractionist models of reading.

品牌名称通常保持独特的字母大小写(例如,IKEA, b谷歌)。这个元素对于理论(单词识别模型)和实践(品牌设计)来说都是必不可少的。在抽象主义模型中,字母大小写被认为是不相关的,而基于实例的模型在词汇检索过程中使用字母大小写等表面信息。先前的品牌识别任务报告了对其特征字母案例的品牌的更快反应(例如,IKEA和谷歌比IKEA和谷歌快),更倾向于基于实例的模型。我们研究了这种模式是否可以推广到正常的句子阅读:参与者阅读的句子中,知名品牌名称是完整的(例如,IKEA,谷歌),或者是修改了的字母大小写(例如,IKEA,谷歌)。结果显示,在早期眼睛注视测量(首次注视的概率,首次注视的持续时间)中,用大写字母书写的品牌的成本与它们的特征字母大小写无关。然而,对于后来的测量(凝视持续时间和总时间),当品牌的字母大小写被修改时,注视时间更长,仅限于那些通常以小写书写的品牌(例如,谷歌>谷歌,而Ikea > Ikea)。因此,在句子阅读过程中,品牌名称的实际字母格和典型字母格是动态互动的,这给抽象主义的阅读模式带来了问题。
{"title":"The CASE of brand names during sentence reading.","authors":"Melanie Labusch, Manuel Perea","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02070-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02070-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Brand names typically maintain a distinctive letter case (e.g., IKEA, Google). This element is essential for theoretical (word recognition models) and practical (brand design) reasons. In abstractionist models, letter case is considered irrelevant, whereas instance-based models use surface information like letter case during lexical retrieval. Previous brand identification tasks reported faster responses to brands in their characteristic letter case (e.g., IKEA and Google faster than ikea and GOOGLE), favoring instance-based models. We examined whether this pattern can be generalized to normal sentence reading: Participants read sentences in which well-known brand names were presented intact (e.g., IKEA, Google) or with a modified letter case (e.g., Ikea, GOOGLE). Results showed a cost for brands written in uppercase, independently of their characteristic letter case, in early eye fixation measures (probability of first-fixation, first-fixation duration). However, for later measures (gaze duration and total times), fixation times were longer when the brand's letter case was modified, restricted to those brands typically written in lowercase (e.g., GOOGLE > Google, whereas Ikea ≲ IKEA). Thus, during sentence reading, both the actual letter case and the typical letter case of brand names interact dynamically, posing problems for abstractionist models of reading.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"40"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142910999","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are three zebras more than three frogs: examining conceptual and physical congruency in numerosity judgements of familiar objects. 三只斑马比三只青蛙多吗:在熟悉物体的数量判断中检验概念和物理一致性。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-28 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02044-6
Mila Marinova, Bert Reynvoet

Researchers in numerical cognition have extensively studied the number sense-the innate human ability to extract numerical information from the environment quickly and effortlessly. Much of this research, however, uses abstract stimuli (e.g., dot configurations) that are also strictly controlled for their low-level visual confounds, such as size. Nonetheless, individuals rarely extract numerical information from abstract stimuli in everyday life. Yet, numerical judgments of familiar objects remain poorly understood and understudied. In the current study, we examined the cognitive mechanisms underlying the numerical decisions of familiar objects. In two experiments, we asked adult participants (Experiment 1) and two groups of children (aged 7-9 years and 11-12 years, Experiment 2) to perform an animal numerosity task (i.e., "Which animal is more numerous?"), while the conceptual congruency (i.e., the congruency between an object's real-life size and its numerosity) and physical congruency (the congruency between the number of items and the total space they occupy on the screen) were manipulated. Results showed that the conceptual congruency effect (i.e., better performance when the animal with a larger size in real life is more numerous) and a physical congruency effect (i.e., better performance when the physically larger animal is more numerous) were present in adults and children. However, the effects differed across the age groups and were also a subject of developmental change. To our knowledge, this study is the first one to demonstrate that conceptual knowledge can interfere with numerosity judgements in a top-down manner. This interference effect is distinct from the bottom-up interference effect, which comes from the physical properties of the set. Our results imply that the number sense is not a standalone core system for numbers but is embedded in a more extensive network where both low-level and higher-order influences are possible. We encourage numerical cognition researchers to consider employing not only abstract but also familiar objects when examining numerosity judgements across the lifespan.

数字认知领域的研究人员对数字感进行了广泛的研究,数字感是人类从环境中快速、轻松地提取数字信息的天生能力。然而,大部分研究使用的是抽象的刺激(例如,点的结构),这些刺激也严格控制了它们的低级视觉混淆,例如大小。然而,在日常生活中,个体很少从抽象的刺激中提取数字信息。然而,对熟悉物体的数字判断仍然知之甚少,研究不足。在当前的研究中,我们研究了熟悉物体的数字决策的认知机制。在两个实验中,我们要求成人参与者(实验1)和两组儿童(7-9岁和11-12岁,实验2)执行一个动物数量任务(即“哪个动物的数量更多?”),同时操纵概念一致性(即物体的实际尺寸与其数量之间的一致性)和物理一致性(即物体的数量与其在屏幕上占据的总空间之间的一致性)。结果表明,成人和儿童均存在概念一致性效应(即现实生活中体型较大的动物数量越多,表现越好)和物理一致性效应(即体型较大的动物数量越多,表现越好)。然而,不同年龄组的影响不同,这也是一个发展变化的主题。据我们所知,这项研究是第一个证明概念性知识可以自上而下地干扰数量判断的研究。这种干扰效应不同于自下而上的干扰效应,自下而上的干扰效应来自于集合的物理性质。我们的研究结果表明,数字感觉不是一个独立的数字核心系统,而是嵌入在一个更广泛的网络中,在这个网络中,低级和高阶的影响都是可能的。我们鼓励数值认知研究人员在研究生命周期中的数值判断时,不仅要考虑使用抽象的对象,还要考虑使用熟悉的对象。
{"title":"Are three zebras more than three frogs: examining conceptual and physical congruency in numerosity judgements of familiar objects.","authors":"Mila Marinova, Bert Reynvoet","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02044-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02044-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Researchers in numerical cognition have extensively studied the number sense-the innate human ability to extract numerical information from the environment quickly and effortlessly. Much of this research, however, uses abstract stimuli (e.g., dot configurations) that are also strictly controlled for their low-level visual confounds, such as size. Nonetheless, individuals rarely extract numerical information from abstract stimuli in everyday life. Yet, numerical judgments of familiar objects remain poorly understood and understudied. In the current study, we examined the cognitive mechanisms underlying the numerical decisions of familiar objects. In two experiments, we asked adult participants (Experiment 1) and two groups of children (aged 7-9 years and 11-12 years, Experiment 2) to perform an animal numerosity task (i.e., \"Which animal is more numerous?\"), while the conceptual congruency (i.e., the congruency between an object's real-life size and its numerosity) and physical congruency (the congruency between the number of items and the total space they occupy on the screen) were manipulated. Results showed that the conceptual congruency effect (i.e., better performance when the animal with a larger size in real life is more numerous) and a physical congruency effect (i.e., better performance when the physically larger animal is more numerous) were present in adults and children. However, the effects differed across the age groups and were also a subject of developmental change. To our knowledge, this study is the first one to demonstrate that conceptual knowledge can interfere with numerosity judgements in a top-down manner. This interference effect is distinct from the bottom-up interference effect, which comes from the physical properties of the set. Our results imply that the number sense is not a standalone core system for numbers but is embedded in a more extensive network where both low-level and higher-order influences are possible. We encourage numerical cognition researchers to consider employing not only abstract but also familiar objects when examining numerosity judgements across the lifespan.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"39"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142899171","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
One direction? Cultural aspects of the mental number line beyond reading direction. 一个方向吗?文化方面的心理数列超越了阅读方向。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-23 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02038-4
Merve Bulut, Lilly Roth, Narjes Bahreini, Krzysztof Cipora, Ulf Dietrich Reips, Hans-Christoph Nuerk

Spatial-Numerical Associations (SNAs) refer to the demonstrations of spatial processing of numbers. The Mental Number Line (MNL) is a representation model describing numbers as aligning left-to-right (LR) and was suggested to account for directional biases in participants' responses during numerical tasks. One common behavioral demonstration of this is the Spatial-Numerical Associations of Response Codes (SNARC) effect, which describes faster left-/right-hand responses to smaller/larger numbers, respectively. The MNL, and, consequently, directional SNAs, show variabilities across different cultures. Reading direction is considered to be the main factor in explaining these differences. In line with this, individuals with right-to-left (RL) reading habits show a weaker or even reverse SNARC effect. In the present study, we investigated whether SNAs are influenced not only by reading direction, but also by cultural directional preferences such as drawing lines, arranging objects, imagining objects (i.e., rightward or leftward facing), or representing events in time (i.e., mentally representing the past/future on the left/right, respectively). To test this hypothesis, we measured the cultural directional preferences and the SNARC effect across three cultures in an online setup; German, Turkish, and Iranian. LR preferences in the Cultural Directional Preferences Questionnaire were most prominent in German participants, intermediate in Turkish participants, and least prominent in Iranian participants. In line with this, the LR SNARC effect was strongest in German, intermediate in Turkish, and weakest (but not RL) in Iranian culture. These findings suggest that cultural directional preferences are involved in the emergence of adult SNAs in addition to the reading direction.

空间-数值联想(sna)是对数字的空间处理的一种展示。心理数字线(MNL)是一种描述数字从左到右排列的表征模型,被认为可以解释参与者在数字任务中反应的方向性偏差。一个常见的行为证明是反应代码的空间-数字关联效应(SNARC),它分别描述了对较小/较大数字的更快的左/右反应。MNL,因此,定向sna,在不同的文化中表现出多样性。阅读方向被认为是解释这些差异的主要因素。与此相一致的是,具有从右向左阅读习惯的个体表现出较弱甚至相反的SNARC效应。在本研究中,我们调查了sna是否不仅受到阅读方向的影响,还受到文化方向偏好的影响,如绘制线条、排列物体、想象物体(即朝右或朝左)或在时间上代表事件(即分别在左/右心理上代表过去/未来)。为了验证这一假设,我们在一个在线设置中测量了三种文化的文化定向偏好和SNARC效应;德语、土耳其语和伊朗语。文化方向偏好问卷中的LR偏好在德国参与者中最突出,在土耳其参与者中居中,在伊朗参与者中最不突出。与此一致,LR SNARC效应在德语中最强,在土耳其语中居中,在伊朗文化中最弱(但不是RL)。这些发现表明,除了阅读方向外,文化方向偏好也参与了成人sna的出现。
{"title":"One direction? Cultural aspects of the mental number line beyond reading direction.","authors":"Merve Bulut, Lilly Roth, Narjes Bahreini, Krzysztof Cipora, Ulf Dietrich Reips, Hans-Christoph Nuerk","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02038-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02038-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Spatial-Numerical Associations (SNAs) refer to the demonstrations of spatial processing of numbers. The Mental Number Line (MNL) is a representation model describing numbers as aligning left-to-right (LR) and was suggested to account for directional biases in participants' responses during numerical tasks. One common behavioral demonstration of this is the Spatial-Numerical Associations of Response Codes (SNARC) effect, which describes faster left-/right-hand responses to smaller/larger numbers, respectively. The MNL, and, consequently, directional SNAs, show variabilities across different cultures. Reading direction is considered to be the main factor in explaining these differences. In line with this, individuals with right-to-left (RL) reading habits show a weaker or even reverse SNARC effect. In the present study, we investigated whether SNAs are influenced not only by reading direction, but also by cultural directional preferences such as drawing lines, arranging objects, imagining objects (i.e., rightward or leftward facing), or representing events in time (i.e., mentally representing the past/future on the left/right, respectively). To test this hypothesis, we measured the cultural directional preferences and the SNARC effect across three cultures in an online setup; German, Turkish, and Iranian. LR preferences in the Cultural Directional Preferences Questionnaire were most prominent in German participants, intermediate in Turkish participants, and least prominent in Iranian participants. In line with this, the LR SNARC effect was strongest in German, intermediate in Turkish, and weakest (but not RL) in Iranian culture. These findings suggest that cultural directional preferences are involved in the emergence of adult SNAs in addition to the reading direction.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"37"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11663824/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142878199","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The differential outcomes procedure for improving the recognition of dynamic facial expressions of emotion in people with autism spectrum disorders and first-degree relatives. 提高自闭症谱系障碍患者和一级亲属对动态面部情绪表情识别的差异结果程序。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-23 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-02067-z
Ángel García-Pérez, Antonio González-Rodríguez, Marta Godoy-Giménez, Pablo Sayans-Jiménez, Fernando Cañadas, Angeles F Estévez

Previous research highlights impairments in the recognition of facial expression of emotion in individuals diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Relatives of people with ASD may exhibit similar, albeit subtler, impairments, referred to as the Broad Autism Phenotype (BAP). Recently, the Differential outcomes procedure (DOP) has been shown to enhance this ability in young adults using dynamic stimuli, with fewer intensity levels required to identify fear and surprise. The present study aimed to extend these findings to adults diagnosed with ASD (ASD group), and relatives of people diagnosed with ASD (BAP group). A Bayesian Generalized Linear Model was employed for statistical inference. The results indicated that the ASD DOP group performed worse than the BAP DOP group in fear trials. The social dimension of autism negatively impacted performance in some conditions, while positive relationships were found between the repetitive behavior dimension and performance for the ASD group. The opposite pattern was observed in the BAP group. These results suggest the importance of considering different dimensions of autism when conducting research on its relationship with other variables. Finally, participants in both ASD and BAP groups required less intensity to identify certain emotions when the DOP was applied, highlighting its potential utility for improving dynamic facial emotion recognition.

先前的研究强调了自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)患者在识别面部情绪表情方面的障碍。ASD患者的亲属可能表现出类似的,尽管更微妙的缺陷,被称为广泛自闭症表型(BAP)。最近,差分结果程序(DOP)已被证明可以通过使用动态刺激来增强年轻人的这种能力,而识别恐惧和惊讶所需的强度水平更低。本研究旨在将这些发现扩展到诊断为ASD的成年人(ASD组)和诊断为ASD的人的亲属(BAP组)。采用贝叶斯广义线性模型进行统计推断。结果表明,ASD DOP组在恐惧试验中的表现比BAP DOP组差。自闭症的社会维度在某些情况下对表现有负向影响,而重复行为维度对ASD组的表现有正向影响。在BAP组中观察到相反的模式。这些结果表明,在研究自闭症与其他变量的关系时,考虑自闭症的不同维度是很重要的。最后,当应用DOP时,ASD组和BAP组的参与者需要更少的强度来识别某些情绪,这突出了它在改善动态面部情绪识别方面的潜在效用。
{"title":"The differential outcomes procedure for improving the recognition of dynamic facial expressions of emotion in people with autism spectrum disorders and first-degree relatives.","authors":"Ángel García-Pérez, Antonio González-Rodríguez, Marta Godoy-Giménez, Pablo Sayans-Jiménez, Fernando Cañadas, Angeles F Estévez","doi":"10.1007/s00426-024-02067-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00426-024-02067-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research highlights impairments in the recognition of facial expression of emotion in individuals diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Relatives of people with ASD may exhibit similar, albeit subtler, impairments, referred to as the Broad Autism Phenotype (BAP). Recently, the Differential outcomes procedure (DOP) has been shown to enhance this ability in young adults using dynamic stimuli, with fewer intensity levels required to identify fear and surprise. The present study aimed to extend these findings to adults diagnosed with ASD (ASD group), and relatives of people diagnosed with ASD (BAP group). A Bayesian Generalized Linear Model was employed for statistical inference. The results indicated that the ASD DOP group performed worse than the BAP DOP group in fear trials. The social dimension of autism negatively impacted performance in some conditions, while positive relationships were found between the repetitive behavior dimension and performance for the ASD group. The opposite pattern was observed in the BAP group. These results suggest the importance of considering different dimensions of autism when conducting research on its relationship with other variables. Finally, participants in both ASD and BAP groups required less intensity to identify certain emotions when the DOP was applied, highlighting its potential utility for improving dynamic facial emotion recognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":48184,"journal":{"name":"Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung","volume":"89 1","pages":"38"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142878202","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1