首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Social Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
The association between victims' vulnerable and grandiose narcissism and grudge holding. 受害者的脆弱和浮夸的自恋与怨恨之间的联系。
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2025-01-02 Epub Date: 2023-11-25 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2023.2286587
Jingyuan Li, C Ward Struthers, Dmytro O Rebrov, Ariel Shoikhedbrod, Joshua R Guilfoyle

Two nonexperimental studies were conducted to test how and why transgression victims' narcissism influences their grudge holding, using undergraduate students and a community sample of adults, respectively. Study 1 tested the association between victims' vulnerable narcissism and grudge holding, including emotional persistence, perceived longevity, and disdain toward the transgressor. It also tested the extent to which victims' grandiose narcissism moderated the association. Study 2 was conducted to replicate Study 1 and test whether victims' rumination about the transgression mediated the moderated association. Overall, those with higher degrees of grandiosity showed a positive relation between vulnerable narcissism and reported emotional persistence (Studies 1 and 2) and perceived longevity (Study 2). Finally, rumination explained the moderated relation (Study 2).

研究人员进行了两项非实验研究,分别以大学生和社区成年人为样本,来测试侵犯受害者的自恋如何以及为什么会影响他们的怨恨。研究1测试了受害者的脆弱自恋和怨恨之间的关系,包括情感持久性、感知寿命和对犯罪者的蔑视。它还测试了受害者的浮夸自恋在多大程度上缓和了这种联系。研究2重复研究1,检验受害者对越轨行为的反思是否介导了这种被调节的关联。总体而言,那些自大程度较高的人在脆弱自恋与报告的情绪持久性(研究1和2)和感知寿命(研究2)之间表现出正相关。最后,反刍解释了调节关系(研究2)。
{"title":"The association between victims' vulnerable and grandiose narcissism and grudge holding.","authors":"Jingyuan Li, C Ward Struthers, Dmytro O Rebrov, Ariel Shoikhedbrod, Joshua R Guilfoyle","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2023.2286587","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00224545.2023.2286587","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Two nonexperimental studies were conducted to test how and why transgression victims' narcissism influences their grudge holding, using undergraduate students and a community sample of adults, respectively. Study 1 tested the association between victims' vulnerable narcissism and grudge holding, including emotional persistence, perceived longevity, and disdain toward the transgressor. It also tested the extent to which victims' grandiose narcissism moderated the association. Study 2 was conducted to replicate Study 1 and test whether victims' rumination about the transgression mediated the moderated association. Overall, those with higher degrees of grandiosity showed a positive relation between vulnerable narcissism and reported emotional persistence (Studies 1 and 2) and perceived longevity (Study 2). Finally, rumination explained the moderated relation (Study 2).</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"21-36"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2025-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138441493","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The ways to avoid abusive supervision: the moderating effects of the characteristics of supervisors and subordinates on abusive supervision. 避免滥用监督的途径:主管和下属特征对滥用监督的调节作用。
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2025-01-02 Epub Date: 2023-11-25 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2023.2283481
Jungmin Jamie Seo, Haesang Park, Ju-Won Han

Following the recent emphasis on supervisory interactions in abusive supervision, this study explains why and how supervisors' job insecurity and authoritarianism are related to abusive supervision and how subordinates' characteristics, agreeableness and negotiating resistance interact with the effects of supervisors' characteristics. We conducted a field study with 261 supervisor and subordinate dyads in South Korea, and the study findings confirmed that supervisors' authoritarianism is positively related to abusive supervision and that the effect is enhanced when subordinates are highly agreeable and display resistant behaviors. The study contributes to the leadership literature, particularly on abusive supervision and personality. Moreover, our findings have practical implications for employees to manage their work relationships with their supervisors or subordinates.

随着最近对滥用监管中监管互动的重视,本研究解释了管理者的工作不安全感和威权主义与滥用监管的关系,以及下属的特征、亲和性和谈判阻力如何与管理者特征的影响相互作用。我们对韩国261名上下级人员进行了实地研究,研究结果证实,主管的威权主义与滥用监督呈正相关,并且当下属表现出高度顺从和抵抗行为时,这种影响会增强。该研究对领导力的研究有贡献,特别是对虐待性监督和人格的研究。此外,我们的研究结果对员工管理与上级或下属的工作关系具有实际意义。
{"title":"The ways to avoid abusive supervision: the moderating effects of the characteristics of supervisors and subordinates on abusive supervision.","authors":"Jungmin Jamie Seo, Haesang Park, Ju-Won Han","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2023.2283481","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00224545.2023.2283481","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Following the recent emphasis on supervisory interactions in abusive supervision, this study explains why and how supervisors' job insecurity and authoritarianism are related to abusive supervision and how subordinates' characteristics, agreeableness and negotiating resistance interact with the effects of supervisors' characteristics. We conducted a field study with 261 supervisor and subordinate dyads in South Korea, and the study findings confirmed that supervisors' authoritarianism is positively related to abusive supervision and that the effect is enhanced when subordinates are highly agreeable and display resistant behaviors. The study contributes to the leadership literature, particularly on abusive supervision and personality. Moreover, our findings have practical implications for employees to manage their work relationships with their supervisors or subordinates.</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-20"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2025-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138441494","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychological entitlement and conspiracy beliefs: evidence from the COVID-19 pandemic. 心理权利与阴谋信念:来自 COVID-19 大流行病的证据。
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2025-01-02 Epub Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2023.2292626
Lukas Neville, Glenda M Fisk, Katarina Ens

Psychological entitlement describes the dispositional tendency to claim excessive and unearned rewards and resources, and to demand undeserved special treatment. In one experiment, one cross-sectional survey, and one time-separated survey (total n = 721), we show that psychological entitlement is associated with general conspiracy theory endorsement, COVID-19 specific conspiracy theory endorsement, and conspiracy theorizing as an overarching cognitive style. We find those high in entitlement are more likely to report having made discretionary visits to non-essential venues and services (e.g. buffets, spas, casinos) during the pandemic, and that these risky public health behaviors are mediated through beliefs in conspiracy theories. We identify consequences for public health behavior and conclude with a research agenda for better understanding the underlying mechanisms linking entitlement and conspiracy beliefs.

心理权利描述了一种要求过多的、不应得的奖励和资源,以及要求不应得的特殊待遇的倾向。在一项实验、一项横截面调查和一项时间分离调查(总人数 = 721)中,我们表明心理权利与一般阴谋论认可、COVID-19 特定阴谋论认可以及作为一种总体认知风格的阴谋论相关。我们发现,在大流行病期间,那些高权利感的人更有可能报告曾随意光顾非必要的场所和服务(如自助餐、水疗、赌场),而这些高风险的公共卫生行为是通过对阴谋论的信仰而中介的。我们确定了公共卫生行为的后果,最后提出了一个研究议程,以更好地理解将权利和阴谋论信念联系起来的内在机制。
{"title":"Psychological entitlement and conspiracy beliefs: evidence from the COVID-19 pandemic.","authors":"Lukas Neville, Glenda M Fisk, Katarina Ens","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2023.2292626","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00224545.2023.2292626","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Psychological entitlement describes the dispositional tendency to claim excessive and unearned rewards and resources, and to demand undeserved special treatment. In one experiment, one cross-sectional survey, and one time-separated survey (total <i>n</i> = 721), we show that psychological entitlement is associated with general conspiracy theory endorsement, COVID-19 specific conspiracy theory endorsement, and conspiracy theorizing as an overarching cognitive style. We find those high in entitlement are more likely to report having made discretionary visits to non-essential venues and services (e.g. buffets, spas, casinos) during the pandemic, and that these risky public health behaviors are mediated through beliefs in conspiracy theories. We identify consequences for public health behavior and conclude with a research agenda for better understanding the underlying mechanisms linking entitlement and conspiracy beliefs.</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"65-87"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2025-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139075556","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Leveraging regulatory fit to enhance engagement: a public-sector study in South Korea. 利用监管契合度提高参与度:韩国公共部门研究。
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-12-30 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2024.2448439
Seon Ah Lee, Jae Hyeung Kang, Shannon Flumerfelt

This study extends the Job Demands-Resources (JD-R) model by incorporating regulatory focus (promotion vs. prevention) as a moderator, examining its impact on job engagement among public sector employees (n = 306). The results show that employees with a promotion focus are more likely to experience increased engagement when facing challenge stressors, as they perceive such stressors as growth opportunities. Conversely, employees with a prevention focus show reduced negative effects from hindrance stressors, as they seek stability and are more focused on avoiding risks. The findings highlight the importance of regulatory fit in job design, suggesting that aligning job demands with individual motivational tendencies can improve engagement. Promotion-focused employees benefit from opportunities for leadership and innovation, while prevention-focused employees thrive in structured environments prioritizing risk management. Tailored training programs in areas like stress management and communication can further support these employees, enhancing engagement and overall organizational performance.

本研究扩展了工作需求-资源(JD-R)模型,将监管焦点(促进与预防)作为调节因素,考察其对公共部门员工工作投入的影响(n = 306)。结果表明,关注晋升的员工在面对挑战压力源时更有可能体验到更高的敬业度,因为他们将这些压力源视为成长机会。相反,关注预防的员工会减少来自障碍压力源的负面影响,因为他们寻求稳定,更注重避免风险。研究结果强调了工作设计中监管契合度的重要性,表明将工作需求与个人动机倾向相结合可以提高敬业度。注重提升的员工受益于领导和创新的机会,而注重预防的员工则在优先考虑风险管理的结构化环境中茁壮成长。在压力管理和沟通等领域量身定制的培训项目可以进一步支持这些员工,提高参与度和整体组织绩效。
{"title":"Leveraging regulatory fit to enhance engagement: a public-sector study in South Korea.","authors":"Seon Ah Lee, Jae Hyeung Kang, Shannon Flumerfelt","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2024.2448439","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2024.2448439","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study extends the Job Demands-Resources (JD-R) model by incorporating regulatory focus (promotion vs. prevention) as a moderator, examining its impact on job engagement among public sector employees (<i>n</i> = 306). The results show that employees with a promotion focus are more likely to experience increased engagement when facing challenge stressors, as they perceive such stressors as growth opportunities. Conversely, employees with a prevention focus show reduced negative effects from hindrance stressors, as they seek stability and are more focused on avoiding risks. The findings highlight the importance of regulatory fit in job design, suggesting that aligning job demands with individual motivational tendencies can improve engagement. Promotion-focused employees benefit from opportunities for leadership and innovation, while prevention-focused employees thrive in structured environments prioritizing risk management. Tailored training programs in areas like stress management and communication can further support these employees, enhancing engagement and overall organizational performance.</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-18"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143014169","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ideological religious nationalism: measurement, construct validity, and cross-cultural comparisons. 意识形态宗教民族主义:测量、构建有效性和跨文化比较。
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-12-29 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2024.2441312
Joseph A Wagoner, Sucharita Belavadi, Antonis Gardikiotis, Barbara Barbieri, Matteo Antonini

Ideological religious nationalism (IRN) is a worldview that advocates the integration of religious beliefs with national policy and laws and the religious moralization of politics. However, the psychological mechanisms, individual differences, and socio-political consequences related to IRN are unclear. Across five studies (NTotal = 1,349), we established construct validity for a novel scale assessing IRN that is adaptable across different contexts. Results showed that stronger IRN relates to distinct psychological motives, domains of religiosity, and views of one's nation. Results also showed that stronger IRN relates to supporting policies that promote religious-national integration and support of political violence. Lastly, results show that this novel IRN scale can be used across different contexts and has incremental validity beyond similar but distinct measures of religious nationalism. Overall, results showed that IRN can successfully capture people's integration of their religious beliefs with their views about their nation's identity.

意识形态宗教民族主义(IRN)是一种主张将宗教信仰与国家政策、法律、政治的宗教道德化相结合的世界观。然而,与IRN相关的心理机制、个体差异和社会政治后果尚不清楚。在五项研究中(NTotal = 1349),我们建立了一种评估IRN的新型量表的结构效度,该量表可适应不同的背景。结果表明,较强的IRN与不同的心理动机、宗教信仰领域和民族观点有关。结果还表明,更强的IRN与促进宗教-民族融合和支持政治暴力的支持政策有关。最后,结果表明,这种新的IRN量表可以在不同的背景下使用,并且比类似但不同的宗教民族主义测量具有递增的效度。总体而言,结果表明,IRN可以成功地捕捉到人们将其宗教信仰与他们对国家认同的看法相结合的情况。
{"title":"Ideological religious nationalism: measurement, construct validity, and cross-cultural comparisons.","authors":"Joseph A Wagoner, Sucharita Belavadi, Antonis Gardikiotis, Barbara Barbieri, Matteo Antonini","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2024.2441312","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00224545.2024.2441312","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Ideological religious nationalism (IRN) is a worldview that advocates the integration of religious beliefs with national policy and laws and the religious moralization of politics. However, the psychological mechanisms, individual differences, and socio-political consequences related to IRN are unclear. Across five studies (<i>N</i><sub>Total</sub> = 1,349), we established construct validity for a novel scale assessing IRN that is adaptable across different contexts. Results showed that stronger IRN relates to distinct psychological motives, domains of religiosity, and views of one's nation. Results also showed that stronger IRN relates to supporting policies that promote religious-national integration and support of political violence. Lastly, results show that this novel IRN scale can be used across different contexts and has incremental validity beyond similar but distinct measures of religious nationalism. Overall, results showed that IRN can successfully capture people's integration of their religious beliefs with their views about their nation's identity.</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-33"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142903925","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Fighting the beat and winning: stereotype threat and White people's rhythmic performance. 对抗节奏与胜利:刻板印象威胁与白人的节奏表现。
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-12-25 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2024.2442029
Simon Howard, Alex M Borgella

A commonly held stereotype about White people in the United States is that they do not have rhythm. Stereotype threat posits that targets in stereotyped domains run the risk of confirming stereotypes in contexts in which they may be evaluated. We examined whether White people experience stereotype threat in domains diagnostic of rhythmic ability. We predicted White people under stereotype threat would perform worse on a rhythm task and have higher domain disengagement relative to White participants in the control condition. White Americans (N =118, 81 women, Mage = 18.81, SD = 1.06) were either told a rhythmic video game task was diagnostic of their rhythmic ability or told the game was to help future game development (i.e. non-diagnostic of ability). We found that White people in the stereotype threat condition performed more poorly on the game than those in the control condition. Furthermore, they also had higher domain disengagement than those in control. Stereotype threat may impede White individuals' ability to perform actions requiring rhythmic ability (e.g. clapping on beat, dancing).

在美国,人们普遍认为白人没有节奏感。刻板印象威胁假设刻板印象领域中的目标在可能对其进行评估的环境中有确认刻板印象的风险。研究了白人在节奏能力诊断领域是否存在刻板印象威胁。我们预测在刻板印象威胁下的白人在节奏任务中的表现较差,并且相对于控制条件下的白人有更高的领域脱离。美国白人(N =118,女性81,法师= 18.81,SD = 1.06)要么被告知节奏性电子游戏任务是对他们节奏能力的诊断,要么被告知游戏有助于未来的游戏开发(即非能力诊断)。我们发现白人在刻板印象威胁条件下的表现比对照组的表现更差。此外,他们也比控制者有更高的领域脱离。刻板印象威胁可能会阻碍白人执行需要节奏能力的动作(如拍手、跳舞)的能力。
{"title":"Fighting the beat and winning: stereotype threat and White people's rhythmic performance.","authors":"Simon Howard, Alex M Borgella","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2024.2442029","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2024.2442029","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A commonly held stereotype about White people in the United States is that they do not have rhythm. Stereotype threat posits that targets in stereotyped domains run the risk of confirming stereotypes in contexts in which they may be evaluated. We examined whether White people experience stereotype threat in domains diagnostic of rhythmic ability. We predicted White people under stereotype threat would perform worse on a rhythm task and have higher domain disengagement relative to White participants in the control condition. White Americans (<i>N</i> =118, 81 women, <i>M</i><sub><i>age</i></sub> = 18.81, <i>SD</i> = 1.06) were either told a rhythmic video game task was diagnostic of their rhythmic ability or told the game was to help future game development (i.e. non-diagnostic of ability). We found that White people in the stereotype threat condition performed more poorly on the game than those in the control condition. Furthermore, they also had higher domain disengagement than those in control. Stereotype threat may impede White individuals' ability to perform actions requiring rhythmic ability (e.g. clapping on beat, dancing).</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-10"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142899172","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bidirectional embodied association between debt and physical burden. 债务与身体负担的双向体现关联。
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-12-15 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2024.2441313
Zi-Han Wei, Xing-Lan Yang, Xiang-Qin Liu, Hong-Zhi Liu

There is a Chinese proverb saying that "when the debts are paid, the body feels light." From the perspective of embodied cognition, there may be a connection between indebtedness and the sensation of physical burden. However, the relationship between the two aspects has not been fully examined. The present research investigated the bidirectionality between indebtedness and physical burden through two studies. In Study 1, we examined the effect of the manipulation of indebtedness on the judgment of a hill slant, which varies by physical burden. Results revealed that participants in the indebted condition judged the hill as steeper than those in the control condition, while repaying the debt eliminated this effect. In Study 2, we found that physical burden enhanced the participants' perception of debt. Consistent with an embodied perspective on cognition, findings suggested the bidirectionality between indebtedness and physical burden and supported embodied simulation theory.

中国有句成语叫 "欠债还钱,一身轻"。从具身认知的角度来看,负债与身体负担感之间可能存在联系。然而,这两方面之间的关系尚未得到充分研究。本研究通过两项研究探讨了负债与身体负担之间的双向性。在研究 1 中,我们考察了负债操纵对山坡斜度判断的影响,而山坡斜度因身体负担而异。结果显示,负债条件下的参与者比对照条件下的参与者判断山坡更陡,而偿还债务则消除了这种影响。在研究 2 中,我们发现身体负担增强了参与者对债务的感知。研究结果表明,负债与身体负担之间存在双向性,这与认知的具身视角相一致,并支持具身模拟理论。
{"title":"Bidirectional embodied association between debt and physical burden.","authors":"Zi-Han Wei, Xing-Lan Yang, Xiang-Qin Liu, Hong-Zhi Liu","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2024.2441313","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2024.2441313","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>There is a Chinese proverb saying that \"when the debts are paid, the body feels light.\" From the perspective of embodied cognition, there may be a connection between indebtedness and the sensation of physical burden. However, the relationship between the two aspects has not been fully examined. The present research investigated the bidirectionality between indebtedness and physical burden through two studies. In Study 1, we examined the effect of the manipulation of indebtedness on the judgment of a hill slant, which varies by physical burden. Results revealed that participants in the indebted condition judged the hill as steeper than those in the control condition, while repaying the debt eliminated this effect. In Study 2, we found that physical burden enhanced the participants' perception of debt. Consistent with an embodied perspective on cognition, findings suggested the bidirectionality between indebtedness and physical burden and supported embodied simulation theory.</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-11"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142830407","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does forming an implementation intention lead individuals to spontaneously use visual mental imagery? 形成实施意向是否会导致个体自发使用视觉心理想象?
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-12-14 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2024.2439945
Léonie Messmer, Fabien Fenouillet, Eve Legrand

Planning through implementation intention involves forming mental representations of a goal-relevant situation and a goal-directed behavior. The main objective of this study was to determine whether mental representations during conscious planning with if-then plan were spontaneously visualized. The sample included 205 participants, asked to perform an easy vs. difficult pro-environmental behavior. They formed an if-then plan vs. a goal intention and were asked whether they spontaneously used mental imagery. Seven days after, 109 participants reported the number of behaviors they performed. The perceived difficulty of the behavior did not differ significantly, this variable was excluded from analyses. A logistic regression was performed and showed that participants who formed an if-then plan used significantly more spontaneous mental imagery compared to goal intention participants. ANCOVAs also revealed that they reported more behaviors than participants who formed a goal intention. However, participants who spontaneously visualized their if-then plan did not report more goal attainment than other participants. This result, which suggests a distinction between spontaneous and instructed visualization (i.e. explicitly requiring participants to visualize their plan), is discussed.

通过实施意图进行规划涉及形成与目标相关的情境和目标导向行为的心理表征。本研究的主要目的是确定在有意识的“如果-那么”计划中心理表征是否自发地可视化。样本包括205名参与者,他们被要求执行一项简单和困难的环保行为。他们形成了一个“如果-那么”计划和一个目标意图,并被问及他们是否自发地使用了心理意象。七天后,109名参与者报告了他们所做行为的数量。行为的感知难度没有显著差异,这一变量被排除在分析之外。逻辑回归分析表明,与目标意向的参与者相比,形成“如果-那么”计划的参与者使用了更多的自发心理意象。ANCOVAs还显示,他们比形成目标意向的参与者报告了更多的行为。然而,自发想象“如果-那么”计划的参与者并没有比其他参与者报告更多的目标实现。这一结果表明,自发和指示可视化(即明确要求参与者可视化他们的计划)之间的区别,进行了讨论。
{"title":"Does forming an implementation intention lead individuals to spontaneously use visual mental imagery?","authors":"Léonie Messmer, Fabien Fenouillet, Eve Legrand","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2024.2439945","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2024.2439945","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Planning through implementation intention involves forming mental representations of a goal-relevant situation and a goal-directed behavior. The main objective of this study was to determine whether mental representations during conscious planning with if-then plan were spontaneously visualized. The sample included 205 participants, asked to perform an easy vs. difficult pro-environmental behavior. They formed an if-then plan vs. a goal intention and were asked whether they spontaneously used mental imagery. Seven days after, 109 participants reported the number of behaviors they performed. The perceived difficulty of the behavior did not differ significantly, this variable was excluded from analyses. A logistic regression was performed and showed that participants who formed an if-then plan used significantly more spontaneous mental imagery compared to goal intention participants. ANCOVAs also revealed that they reported more behaviors than participants who formed a goal intention. However, participants who spontaneously visualized their if-then plan did not report more goal attainment than other participants. This result, which suggests a distinction between spontaneous and instructed visualization (i.e. explicitly requiring participants to visualize their plan), is discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-20"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142824536","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Gender differences in receptivity to sexual invitations: two naturalistic replication studies. 接受性邀请的性别差异:两个自然复制研究。
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-12-11 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2024.2439950
Sascha Kunz, Tobias Greitemeyer

In a seminal study, Clark and Hatfield (1989) found that men were more willing than women to accept casual sexual invitations, whereas no significant gender differences were observed in responses to propositions for non-committal social activities. The present research comprises two preregistered naturalistic replication studies (total N = 240). Study 1 serves as a direct replication, while Study 2 additionally tests whether differences in sociosexuality account for why men are more willing than women to accept casual sexual offers. In both studies, men more readily than women accepted a sexual invitation from a stranger of the opposite gender. In contrast to the original study, the gender difference was independent of the type of proposition. Individual differences in sociosexuality did not account for the observed gender differences. In summary, gender differences in the willingness to accept casual sexual invitations persist to this day, over 40 years after the initial Clark and Hatfield study.

在一项开创性的研究中,Clark和Hatfield(1989)发现,男性比女性更愿意接受随意的性邀请,而对于非承诺的社交活动的回应则没有明显的性别差异。本研究包括两个预注册的自然重复研究(总N = 240)。研究1是直接的复制,而研究2则进一步测试了社会性别的差异是否解释了为什么男性比女性更愿意接受随意性行为。在这两项研究中,男性比女性更容易接受异性陌生人的性邀请。与原始研究相反,性别差异与命题类型无关。社会性取向的个体差异并不能解释观察到的性别差异。总之,在克拉克和哈特菲尔德最初的研究之后的40多年里,性别差异在接受随意性邀请的意愿上一直存在。
{"title":"Gender differences in receptivity to sexual invitations: two naturalistic replication studies.","authors":"Sascha Kunz, Tobias Greitemeyer","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2024.2439950","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2024.2439950","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In a seminal study, Clark and Hatfield (1989) found that men were more willing than women to accept casual sexual invitations, whereas no significant gender differences were observed in responses to propositions for non-committal social activities. The present research comprises two preregistered naturalistic replication studies (total <i>N</i> = 240). Study 1 serves as a direct replication, while Study 2 additionally tests whether differences in sociosexuality account for why men are more willing than women to accept casual sexual offers. In both studies, men more readily than women accepted a sexual invitation from a stranger of the opposite gender. In contrast to the original study, the gender difference was independent of the type of proposition. Individual differences in sociosexuality did not account for the observed gender differences. In summary, gender differences in the willingness to accept casual sexual invitations persist to this day, over 40 years after the initial Clark and Hatfield study.</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-14"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142808173","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Similarity-attraction across ethnic, religious, and political groups: does celebrating differences or similarities make a difference? 不同种族、宗教和政治群体之间的相似性-吸引力:庆祝差异还是相似性会产生影响?
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-11-25 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2024.2427834
Raimundo Salas-Schweikart, Margaret J Hendricks, Melanie Boychuck, Fathali M Moghaddam

Extensive research supports a positive association between similarity and attraction at the inter-personal level; the very limited research at the intergroup level is also supportive. In the context of increasing diversity in major societies, alternative diversity management approaches give priority to celebrating differences versus celebrating similarities. We tested to see if similarity-attraction at the intergroup level remains robust in conditions of celebrating differences versus similarities in four studies with ethnic (Study 1, N = 231; Study 2, N = 823), religious (Study 3, N = 1,004), and political (Study 4, N = 606) groups. Study 1 confirmed that participants wanted closer contact with others who they see as more similar. Studies 2, 3, and 4 largely replicated this pattern and found no differences across conditions celebrating differences or similarities between groups. In line with similarity-attraction theory, most group members preferred contact with similar others, both when intergroup differences and similarities were celebrated. The findings are discussed in the context of debates about diversity management policies.

大量研究表明,在人与人之间,相似性与吸引力之间存在正相关;在群体与群体之间,非常有限的研究也证明了这一点。在主要社会日益多元化的背景下,其他多元化管理方法优先考虑的是赞美差异而非赞美相似。我们通过四项研究,分别对种族群体(研究 1,人数 = 231;研究 2,人数 = 823)、宗教群体(研究 3,人数 = 1,004)和政治群体(研究 4,人数 = 606)进行了测试,以了解在赞美差异与赞美相似的条件下,群体间的相似性吸引力是否依然强大。研究 1 证实,参与者希望与他们认为更相似的人建立更密切的联系。研究 2、研究 3 和研究 4 在很大程度上复制了这一模式,并发现在不同条件下,群体间的差异或相似性并不存在。与相似性吸引理论相一致的是,无论是在庆祝群体间差异还是相似性的情况下,大多数群体成员都更愿意与相似的人接触。本文结合有关多样性管理政策的讨论,对研究结果进行了讨论。
{"title":"Similarity-attraction across ethnic, religious, and political groups: does celebrating differences or similarities make a difference?","authors":"Raimundo Salas-Schweikart, Margaret J Hendricks, Melanie Boychuck, Fathali M Moghaddam","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2024.2427834","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2024.2427834","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Extensive research supports a positive association between similarity and attraction at the inter-personal level; the very limited research at the intergroup level is also supportive. In the context of increasing diversity in major societies, alternative diversity management approaches give priority to celebrating differences versus celebrating similarities. We tested to see if similarity-attraction at the intergroup level remains robust in conditions of celebrating differences versus similarities in four studies with ethnic (Study 1, <i>N</i> = 231; Study 2, <i>N</i> = 823), religious (Study 3, <i>N</i> = 1,004), and political (Study 4, <i>N</i> = 606) groups. Study 1 confirmed that participants wanted closer contact with others who they see as more similar. Studies 2, 3, and 4 largely replicated this pattern and found no differences across conditions celebrating differences or similarities between groups. In line with similarity-attraction theory, most group members preferred contact with similar others, both when intergroup differences and similarities were celebrated. The findings are discussed in the context of debates about diversity management policies.</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-20"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142710828","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Social Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1