首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Social Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Correction. 更正。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2024.2315697
{"title":"Correction.","authors":"","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2024.2315697","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2024.2315697","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139703731","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychological dread and extreme persistent fear. 心理恐惧和极端持久的恐惧。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-04 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2024.2310545
Robin M Kowalski, Madalynne Gagne, Grace Drolet, Kaitlyn Burzin, Hailey Carroll, Hannah Korson, Blake Rimmer, Emma Aurilio, Raquel Bunche, Gabriela Mochizuki, Natalie Cote, Morgan Dowd, Lyndsey Brewer, Kelly Evans, Aspen Ridder

Psychological dread is a phenomenon with which virtually everyone is familiar, whether dreading a medical appointment, a job interview, or the impending death of a loved one. Despite the prevalence of dread in most people's everyday lives, surprisingly little empirical research has explored the construct. The purpose of the current research was to examine psychological dread (Study 1 and Study 2) and to compare dread to extreme persistent fear (Study 2). Respondents across both studies completed surveys on which they described a dreaded experience (Studies 1 and 2) or an extremely feared event (Study 2) and answered questions about the event. Participants reported uncertainty and lack of control surrounding events associated with both dread and extreme persistent fear. They also anticipated that they would feel relief when these events were over, but this relief was greater with dread than fear. Implications for coping with dread and extreme persistent fears are discussed along with comparisons of the types of events most commonly associated with dread and extreme fear.

心理恐惧几乎是每个人都熟悉的现象,无论是害怕就医、工作面试,还是害怕亲人即将离世。尽管恐惧在大多数人的日常生活中普遍存在,但令人惊讶的是,很少有实证研究对这一概念进行探讨。当前研究的目的是考察心理恐惧(研究 1 和研究 2),并将恐惧与极端持续恐惧(研究 2)进行比较。两项研究的受访者都填写了调查问卷,他们在问卷中描述了一段恐惧经历(研究 1 和研究 2)或一件极度恐惧的事件(研究 2),并回答了有关该事件的问题。受调查者报告了与恐惧和极度持续恐惧相关事件的不确定性和缺乏控制。他们还预计,当这些事件结束时,他们会感到轻松,但这种轻松感在恐惧时比在害怕时更强烈。本文讨论了应对恐惧和极度持续恐惧的意义,并对最常与恐惧和极度恐惧相关的事件类型进行了比较。
{"title":"Psychological dread and extreme persistent fear.","authors":"Robin M Kowalski, Madalynne Gagne, Grace Drolet, Kaitlyn Burzin, Hailey Carroll, Hannah Korson, Blake Rimmer, Emma Aurilio, Raquel Bunche, Gabriela Mochizuki, Natalie Cote, Morgan Dowd, Lyndsey Brewer, Kelly Evans, Aspen Ridder","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2024.2310545","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2024.2310545","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Psychological dread is a phenomenon with which virtually everyone is familiar, whether dreading a medical appointment, a job interview, or the impending death of a loved one. Despite the prevalence of dread in most people's everyday lives, surprisingly little empirical research has explored the construct. The purpose of the current research was to examine psychological dread (Study 1 and Study 2) and to compare dread to extreme persistent fear (Study 2). Respondents across both studies completed surveys on which they described a dreaded experience (Studies 1 and 2) or an extremely feared event (Study 2) and answered questions about the event. Participants reported uncertainty and lack of control surrounding events associated with both dread and extreme persistent fear. They also anticipated that they would feel relief when these events were over, but this relief was greater with dread than fear. Implications for coping with dread and extreme persistent fears are discussed along with comparisons of the types of events most commonly associated with dread and extreme fear.</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139681755","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The broken trust: how exploitative leadership damages employee work passion. 破碎的信任:剥削型领导如何损害员工的工作热情。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-02 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2024.2311256
Saleh Bajaba, Ziyad Al-Judibi, Abdulrahman Basahal, Abdullah Alsabban

This study explores how exploitative leadership affects employees' work passion, a vital element for engagement, creativity, and productivity. It further delves into how trust in leaders mediates this relationship. By applying social exchange theory and conservation of resources theory and analyzing responses from 384 full-time employees through covariance-based structural equation modeling using SmartPLS, the findings confirm the negative effects of exploitative leadership on work passion. They also underscore the significant mediating role of trust in leaders. These insights underline the importance of addressing exploitative leadership in organizational policies and enhancing trust to improve work passion. The study not only provides valuable information for organizations but also lays the groundwork for future research on leadership styles, trust, and employee passion.

本研究探讨了剥削型领导如何影响员工的工作热情,而工作热情是员工参与、创造力和生产力的重要因素。研究还进一步探讨了对领导者的信任是如何调节这种关系的。研究运用社会交换理论和资源保护理论,通过基于协方差的结构方程模型(SmartPLS)对 384 名全职员工的回答进行分析,结果证实了剥削型领导对工作激情的负面影响。研究结果还强调了领导信任的重要中介作用。这些见解强调了在组织政策中解决剥削型领导问题和增强信任以提高工作热情的重要性。这项研究不仅为组织提供了有价值的信息,也为未来有关领导风格、信任和员工激情的研究奠定了基础。
{"title":"The broken trust: how exploitative leadership damages employee work passion.","authors":"Saleh Bajaba, Ziyad Al-Judibi, Abdulrahman Basahal, Abdullah Alsabban","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2024.2311256","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2024.2311256","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study explores how exploitative leadership affects employees' work passion, a vital element for engagement, creativity, and productivity. It further delves into how trust in leaders mediates this relationship. By applying social exchange theory and conservation of resources theory and analyzing responses from 384 full-time employees through covariance-based structural equation modeling using SmartPLS, the findings confirm the negative effects of exploitative leadership on work passion. They also underscore the significant mediating role of trust in leaders. These insights underline the importance of addressing exploitative leadership in organizational policies and enhancing trust to improve work passion. The study not only provides valuable information for organizations but also lays the groundwork for future research on leadership styles, trust, and employee passion.</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139673271","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dehumanization of stigmatized targets of ostracism. 对被污名化的排斥对象进行非人化处理。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-29 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2024.2307577
Maayan Dvir, Maayan Nagar

The study explored people's reactions to observing the ostracism of stigmatized targets. Participants (n = 198) who observed ostracism experienced need threat regardless of the target's identity. Participants regarded included addicts more positively than ostracized addicts, especially on traits that are considered unique to humans. As for dehumanization, subtle measures demonstrate that ostracized targets are perceived as less human. In contrast, our original measure of blatant dehumanization suggests that targets of ostracism are perceived as more human. The study stresses the inconsistency between dehumanization measurements and the need to specify what each measure taps into and how each contributes to the theory.

该研究探讨了人们在观察被污名化的对象受到排斥时的反应。观察到排斥行为的参与者(n = 198)无论目标对象的身份如何,都会感受到需求威胁。与被排斥的瘾君子相比,受试者更积极地看待被接纳的瘾君子,尤其是在被认为是人类独有的特征方面。至于 "非人化",微妙的测量结果表明,被排斥的目标被认为不那么像人。与此相反,我们最初对公然非人化的测量结果表明,被排斥的对象被认为更有人性。这项研究强调了非人性化测量之间的不一致性,并强调有必要明确说明每种测量方法所涉及的内容,以及每种方法对该理论的贡献。
{"title":"Dehumanization of stigmatized targets of ostracism.","authors":"Maayan Dvir, Maayan Nagar","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2024.2307577","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00224545.2024.2307577","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The study explored people's reactions to observing the ostracism of stigmatized targets. Participants (<i>n</i> = 198) who observed ostracism experienced need threat regardless of the target's identity. Participants regarded included addicts more positively than ostracized addicts, especially on traits that are considered unique to humans. As for dehumanization, subtle measures demonstrate that ostracized targets are perceived as less human. In contrast, our original measure of blatant dehumanization suggests that targets of ostracism are perceived as more human. The study stresses the inconsistency between dehumanization measurements and the need to specify what each measure taps into and how each contributes to the theory.</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139574856","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Blindfolding political trust: the palliative effect of trust-based ignorance of political performance problems. 蒙蔽政治信任:基于信任的对政治表现问题一无所知的缓和效应。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-09 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2023.2298886
Márton Hadarics

In this study, we gather empirical evidence supporting the assumption that political trust is related to the ignorance of political performance problems, which ignorance, in turn, has a palliative function as it protects subjective well-being. We hypothesized that political trust has a stronger positive effect on the evaluation of political performance (and indirectly on subjective well-being) if actual performance is more questionable. These hypotheses were tested by multilevel structural equation modeling. Our multilevel moderated mediation models supporting this hypothesis were based on the 2018 European Social Survey dataset (N = 49,519) extended with county-level indices indicating actual performance in healthcare, education, and economy. The results also imply that the bigger the actual problem is, the stronger ignorance is driven by political trust.

在本研究中,我们收集了支持以下假设的经验证据:政治信任与对政治表现问题的无知有关,而这种无知又具有保护主观幸福感的缓和功能。我们假设,如果实际表现更有问题,政治信任对政治表现的评价(以及间接对主观幸福感)会有更强的积极影响。我们通过多层次结构方程模型对这些假设进行了检验。我们支持这一假设的多层次调节中介模型基于2018年欧洲社会调查数据集(N = 49,519),并扩展了表明医疗保健、教育和经济实际绩效的县级指数。结果还意味着,实际问题越大,政治信任对无知的驱动力就越强。
{"title":"Blindfolding political trust: the palliative effect of trust-based ignorance of political performance problems.","authors":"Márton Hadarics","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2023.2298886","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2023.2298886","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this study, we gather empirical evidence supporting the assumption that political trust is related to the ignorance of political performance problems, which ignorance, in turn, has a palliative function as it protects subjective well-being. We hypothesized that political trust has a stronger positive effect on the evaluation of political performance (and indirectly on subjective well-being) if actual performance is more questionable. These hypotheses were tested by multilevel structural equation modeling. Our multilevel moderated mediation models supporting this hypothesis were based on the 2018 European Social Survey dataset (<i>N</i> = 49,519) extended with county-level indices indicating actual performance in healthcare, education, and economy. The results also imply that the bigger the actual problem is, the stronger ignorance is driven by political trust.</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139404752","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
I hope my partner can make me change: expected relational self-changes and relational outcomes. 我希望我的伴侣能让我改变:预期的关系自我改变和关系结果。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-02 Epub Date: 2021-12-01 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2021.2008853
Lijing Ma, Eddie M Clark

The Two-Dimensional Model of Relational Self-change (TDM) suggests four types of self-change that may be elicited by romantic relationships: self-expansion, self-adulteration, self-pruning, and self-contraction. Previous research has shown that these four types of relational self-change were associated with relational outcomes. Prior research has also shown that positive illusions and expectations in romantic relationships can lead to positive relational outcomes. The current study used a longitudinal design to investigate how the four types of expected relational self-change affect future relationship satisfaction and commitment. Results indicated that participants had higher expected self-changes than their actual self-changes after three months, but their expected self-changes and actual self-changes were correlated. At Time 1 of their relationship, participants' relationship satisfaction and commitment were related to their expected self-changes. However, controlling for Time 1 relationship satisfaction and commitment, expected self-changes predicted commitment, but not relationship satisfaction, three months later.

关系自我改变的二维模型(TDM)提出了恋爱关系可能引发的四种自我改变类型:自我扩张、自我陶醉、自我修剪和自我收缩。先前的研究表明,这四种类型的关系自我改变与关系结果相关。先前的研究还表明,恋爱关系中的积极幻想和期望能带来积极的恋爱结果。本研究采用纵向设计,调查四种预期关系自我改变如何影响未来的关系满意度和承诺。结果表明,三个月后,参与者的预期自我改变高于实际自我改变,但他们的预期自我改变和实际自我改变是相关的。在恋爱第一阶段,参与者的恋爱满意度和承诺与他们的预期自我改变有关。然而,在控制了第一阶段的关系满意度和承诺后,三个月后,预期的自我改变可以预测承诺,但不能预测关系满意度。
{"title":"I hope my partner can make me change: expected relational self-changes and relational outcomes.","authors":"Lijing Ma, Eddie M Clark","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2021.2008853","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00224545.2021.2008853","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Two-Dimensional Model of Relational Self-change (TDM) suggests four types of self-change that may be elicited by romantic relationships: self-expansion, self-adulteration, self-pruning, and self-contraction. Previous research has shown that these four types of relational self-change were associated with relational outcomes. Prior research has also shown that positive illusions and expectations in romantic relationships can lead to positive relational outcomes. The current study used a longitudinal design to investigate how the four types of expected relational self-change affect future relationship satisfaction and commitment. Results indicated that participants had higher expected self-changes than their actual self-changes after three months, but their expected self-changes and actual self-changes were correlated. At Time 1 of their relationship, participants' relationship satisfaction and commitment were related to their expected self-changes. However, controlling for Time 1 relationship satisfaction and commitment, expected self-changes predicted commitment, but not relationship satisfaction, three months later.</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39770461","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A cautionary note on interpreting research findings in the presence of statistical suppression. 在存在统计抑制的情况下解释研究结果的注意事项。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-02 Epub Date: 2023-04-02 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2023.2197775
Gordon Hodson, Elvira Prusaczyk

In regression analyses predictor variables can suppress the effects of other predictor variables, sometimes even resulting in "flipped" relations relative to their zero-order relations (i.e. negative suppression). Drawing on research examining the relations between religion and prejudice, and between ideology and desiring "tall poppies" (successful people) to fall, we highlight examples where researchers appear to have made inappropriate or confusing interpretations of their findings. We compare these examples to a best practice illustration involving associations between psychopathy and counter-productive work behavior. Finally, we provide practical guidelines for thinking about suppression effects in research programmes.

在回归分析中,预测变量可以抑制其他预测变量的影响,有时甚至会导致相对于零阶关系的 "翻转 "关系(即负抑制)。通过研究宗教与偏见之间的关系,以及意识形态与希望 "高高的罂粟花"(成功人士)倒下之间的关系,我们着重举例说明研究人员似乎对其研究结果做出了不恰当或混乱的解释。我们将这些例子与涉及心理变态与反工作行为之间关联的最佳实践说明进行比较。最后,我们为在研究计划中考虑抑制效应提供了实用指南。
{"title":"A cautionary note on interpreting research findings in the presence of statistical suppression.","authors":"Gordon Hodson, Elvira Prusaczyk","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2023.2197775","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00224545.2023.2197775","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In regression analyses predictor variables can suppress the effects of other predictor variables, sometimes even resulting in \"flipped\" relations relative to their zero-order relations (i.e. negative suppression). Drawing on research examining the relations between religion and prejudice, and between ideology and desiring \"tall poppies\" (successful people) to fall, we highlight examples where researchers appear to have made inappropriate or confusing interpretations of their findings. We compare these examples to a best practice illustration involving associations between psychopathy and counter-productive work behavior. Finally, we provide practical guidelines for thinking about suppression effects in research programmes.</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9227778","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
No evidence for modulation of facial mimicry by attachment tendencies in adulthood: an EMG investigation. 没有证据表明成年后依恋倾向会调节面部模仿:一项肌电图调查。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-02 Epub Date: 2021-09-30 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2021.1973946
Stefania V Vacaru, Johanna E van Schaik, Lukas Spiess, Sabine Hunnius

Mimicking another individual functions as a social glue: it smoothens the interaction and fosters affiliation. Here, we investigated whether the intrinsic motivation to affiliate with others, stemming from attachment relationships, modulates individuals' engagement in facial mimicry (FM). Participants (N = 100; MAge = 24.54 years, SDAge = 3.90 years) observed faces with happy, sad, and neutral expressions, while their facial muscle activity was recorded with electromyography. Attachment was measured with the Attachment Styles Questionnaire, which provides a multidimensional profile for preoccupied and dismissing styles. It was proposed that the preoccupied and dismissing styles are characterized by high and low intrinsic affiliation motivation, respectively, and these were hypothesized to manifest in enhanced and diminished FM. Participants showed happy and sad FM, yet attachment styles did not significantly predict FM. Bayes Factor analyses lend evidence favoring the null hypothesis, suggesting that adult attachment do not contribute to FM.

模仿他人是一种社会粘合剂:它能使互动更加顺畅,并促进从属关系。在此,我们研究了源自依恋关系的与他人建立联系的内在动机是否会调节个体的面部模仿(FM)行为。参与者(N = 100;MAge = 24.54 years, SDAge = 3.90 years)观察带有快乐、悲伤和中性表情的面孔,同时用肌电图记录他们的面部肌肉活动。依恋是通过依恋风格问卷来测量的,该问卷提供了先入为主风格和否定风格的多维特征。据推测,先入为主型和漠视型分别具有高和低内在依恋动机的特征,这两种类型会表现为调频的增强和减弱。受试者表现出快乐和悲伤的 FM,但依恋风格并不能显著预测 FM。贝叶斯因子分析提供了有利于零假设的证据,表明成人依恋不会导致FM。
{"title":"No evidence for modulation of facial mimicry by attachment tendencies in adulthood: an EMG investigation.","authors":"Stefania V Vacaru, Johanna E van Schaik, Lukas Spiess, Sabine Hunnius","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2021.1973946","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00224545.2021.1973946","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Mimicking another individual functions as a social glue: it smoothens the interaction and fosters affiliation. Here, we investigated whether the intrinsic motivation to affiliate with others, stemming from attachment relationships, modulates individuals' engagement in facial mimicry (FM). Participants (N = 100; <i>M</i><sub>Age</sub> = 24.54 years, <i>SD</i><sub>Age</sub> = 3.90 years) observed faces with happy, sad, and neutral expressions, while their facial muscle activity was recorded with electromyography. Attachment was measured with the Attachment Styles Questionnaire, which provides a multidimensional profile for preoccupied and dismissing styles. It was proposed that the preoccupied and dismissing styles are characterized by high and low intrinsic affiliation motivation, respectively, and these were hypothesized to manifest in enhanced and diminished FM. Participants showed happy and sad FM, yet attachment styles did not significantly predict FM. Bayes Factor analyses lend evidence favoring the null hypothesis, suggesting that adult attachment do not contribute to FM.</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39471175","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Power and the confrontation of sexism: the impact of measured and manipulated power on confronting behavior. 权力与性别歧视的对抗:衡量和操纵权力对对抗行为的影响。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-02 Epub Date: 2022-09-18 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2022.2122767
Nicholas P Alt, Jenniffer Wong Chavez, Cheryl L Dickter, Margaret J Shih

The confrontation of prejudicial acts and comments promotes multiple benefits, most notably the prevention of future prejudicial remarks and the reduction of stereotype use. Research, however, consistently shows low rates of confronting prejudice, particularly regarding sexism. Here, we examine whether personal sense of power, known to increase action and activate the behavioral approach system, increases the likelihood of confronting a sexist remark. In Study 1, we demonstrate that for both women and men, self-reported power is associated with a higher frequency of confronting sexism. In Study 2, we manipulate women's sense of power (i.e., high power, low power, or control) and subsequently present an opportunity to confront a sexist remark. Results show that women primed to feel powerful were more likely to confront the sexist remark and expressed greater disagreement with the comment, compared to women primed to feel powerless. Implications for the confronting literature and behavior are discussed.

正视偏见行为和评论可带来多种益处,其中最显著的是防止今后出现偏见言论,减少刻板印象的使用。然而,研究始终表明,面对偏见的比例很低,尤其是在性别歧视方面。在此,我们研究了个人的权力感(众所周知,权力感会增加行动并激活行为方法系统)是否会增加面对性别歧视言论的可能性。在研究 1 中,我们证明,对于女性和男性来说,自我报告的权力与更高的性别歧视对抗频率相关。在研究 2 中,我们操纵了女性的权力感(即高权力感、低权力感或控制感),并随后提供了一个对抗性别歧视言论的机会。结果表明,与感觉自己没有权力的女性相比,感觉自己有权力的女性更有可能面对性别歧视言论,并对该言论表达了更多的不同意见。本文讨论了对抗文学和行为的意义。
{"title":"Power and the confrontation of sexism: the impact of measured and manipulated power on confronting behavior.","authors":"Nicholas P Alt, Jenniffer Wong Chavez, Cheryl L Dickter, Margaret J Shih","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2022.2122767","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00224545.2022.2122767","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The confrontation of prejudicial acts and comments promotes multiple benefits, most notably the prevention of future prejudicial remarks and the reduction of stereotype use. Research, however, consistently shows low rates of confronting prejudice, particularly regarding sexism. Here, we examine whether personal sense of power, known to increase action and activate the behavioral approach system, increases the likelihood of confronting a sexist remark. In Study 1, we demonstrate that for both women and men, self-reported power is associated with a higher frequency of confronting sexism. In Study 2, we manipulate women's sense of power (i.e., high power, low power, or control) and subsequently present an opportunity to confront a sexist remark. Results show that women primed to feel powerful were more likely to confront the sexist remark and expressed greater disagreement with the comment, compared to women primed to feel powerless. Implications for the confronting literature and behavior are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40365805","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Natives with a need for cognitive closure can approve of immigrants' economic effect when they trust pro-immigrant epistemic authorities. 有认知封闭需求的本地人如果信任支持移民的认识论权威,就会认可移民的经济效应。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-02 Epub Date: 2021-11-12 DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2021.1988498
Conrad Baldner, Antonio Pierro, Alessandra Talamo, Arie Kruglanski

Previous research on the need for cognitive closure (NFC), or the desire for epistemic certainty, has consistently found that it is associated with negative attitudes toward immigrants, among other outgroups, potentially because they represent agents of change and/or due to a general preference for perceived stability and certainty associated with right-wing politics. However, as individuals with this need theoretically prefer stable and certain knowledge, independent of the specific content, it is also possible that these individuals could have positive attitudes toward immigrants when they are provided with a positive source of information to which they can metaphorically "close" upon. In two studies (n = 397), controlling for participants' political orientation, we found that individuals with an NFC were more likely to accept immigrants when their positive effect was endorsed by an epistemic authority (Study 1), but only when they trusted this source (Study 2).

以往关于认知封闭性需求(NFC)或对认识论确定性的渴望的研究一直发现,这种需求与人们对移民和其他外来群体的消极态度有关,这可能是因为移民代表着变革的推动者,和/或由于人们普遍偏好与右翼政治相关的可感知的稳定性和确定性。然而,从理论上讲,具有这种需求的人更喜欢稳定和确定的知识,而与具体内容无关,因此,当这些人获得积极的信息来源时,他们也有可能对移民持积极态度,因为他们可以隐喻地 "接近 "这些信息来源。在两项研究(n = 397)中,在控制了参与者的政治倾向后,我们发现,当移民的积极影响得到认识论权威的认可时,NFC个体更有可能接受移民(研究 1),但只有当他们信任这一信息来源时才会接受移民(研究 2)。
{"title":"Natives with a need for cognitive closure can approve of immigrants' economic effect when they trust pro-immigrant epistemic authorities.","authors":"Conrad Baldner, Antonio Pierro, Alessandra Talamo, Arie Kruglanski","doi":"10.1080/00224545.2021.1988498","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00224545.2021.1988498","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research on the need for cognitive closure (NFC), or the desire for epistemic certainty, has consistently found that it is associated with negative attitudes toward immigrants, among other outgroups, potentially because they represent agents of change and/or due to a general preference for perceived stability and certainty associated with right-wing politics. However, as individuals with this need theoretically prefer stable and certain knowledge, independent of the specific content, it is also possible that these individuals could have positive attitudes toward immigrants when they are provided with a positive source of information to which they can metaphorically \"close\" upon. In two studies (<i>n</i> = 397), controlling for participants' political orientation, we found that individuals with an NFC were more likely to accept immigrants when their positive effect was endorsed by an epistemic authority (Study 1), but only when they trusted this source (Study 2).</p>","PeriodicalId":48205,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39703160","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
Journal of Social Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1