首页 > 最新文献

Law and Human Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Assessing psychopathic traits with the MMPI-3: Findings from correctional, university, and community samples. 用 MMPI-3 评估精神病患者特质:来自教养所、大学和社区样本的研究结果。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000552
Dustin B Wygant, Martin Sellbom

Objective: The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) instruments have a long history with respect to the assessment of psychopathic personality traits. The most recent version, the MMPI-3, should be in a good position to continue this tradition, and the aim of the current research was to evaluate its scales for this purpose. We examined, on the basis of previous research, how well conceptually relevant MMPI-3 scales mapped onto dominant contemporary psychopathy models: the traditional three-factor model and triarchic psychopathy model.

Hypotheses: We hypothesized that MMPI-3 markers of internalizing would be negatively correlated with boldness, whereas broad and specific markers of externalizing proclivities would be associated with disinhibition and antisociality. We also hypothesized that egocentricity and callousness would be associated with MMPI-3 scales measuring various features of externalizing, interpersonal aggression/antagonism, and grandiosity.

Method: We used archival samples of male prison inmates (n = 452), community members with externalizing proclivities (n = 205), and university students (n = 645). These participants completed the Expanded Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale and the Triarchic Psychopathy Measure.

Results: Zero-order correlation analyses indicated support for many of our hypotheses across samples, with notable exceptions. Regression and dominance analyses yielded information about the most potent MMPI-3 predictors of each psychopathy domain, with consistency across the three samples. Boldness was associated with low scores on Emotional/Internalizing Dysfunction, Low Positive Emotions, Shyness, and Negative Emotionality/Neuroticism and high scores on Self-Importance and Dominance. For meanness and disinhibition, we found substantial overlap with MMPI-3 scales (e.g., Behavioral/Externalizing Dysfunction, Antisocial Behavior). Meanness was indicated by high Aggression, Cynicism, Aggressiveness, and Disaffiliativeness; disinhibition/antisociality was primarily marked by high Antisocial Behavior, Hypomanic Activation, Impulsivity, and Disconstraint; and Anger Proneness, Aggression, and Cynicism were secondary indicators.

Conclusions: These findings provide support for using the MMPI-3 in clinical assessments to corroborate other sources of information regarding psychopathy as well as generate hypotheses for further consideration. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:明尼苏达多相人格量表(MMPI)工具在评估变态人格特质方面有着悠久的历史。最新版本的明尼苏达多相人格问卷(MMPI-3)应该能够很好地继承这一传统,本研究的目的就是为此对其量表进行评估。在以往研究的基础上,我们考察了与概念相关的 MMPI-3 量表与当代主流变态心理学模型(传统的三因素模型和三元变态心理学模型)的映射程度:我们假设,MMPI-3 内化标记与大胆呈负相关,而外化倾向的广泛和特定标记则与抑制和反社会性相关。我们还假设,自我中心和冷酷无情会与测量外化、人际攻击/对抗和自大等各种特征的 MMPI-3 量表相关:我们使用了男性囚犯(452 人)、有外化倾向的社区成员(205 人)和大学生(645 人)的档案样本。这些参与者填写了扩展的莱文森自我报告心理变态量表和三元心理变态测量:零阶相关分析表明,我们的许多假设在不同样本中都得到了支持,但也有明显的例外。回归分析和优势分析得出了有关 MMPI-3 对每个心理变态领域最有效的预测因素的信息,这在三个样本中是一致的。大胆与情绪/内化功能障碍、低积极情绪、害羞和消极情绪/神经质的低分以及自我重要性和支配性的高分有关。在刻薄和抑制方面,我们发现与 MMPI-3 量表(如行为/外化功能障碍、反社会行为)有很大的重叠。高攻击性、愤世嫉俗、攻击性和不合群性是卑鄙的表现;高反社会行为、躁狂激活、冲动和拘谨是抑制/反社会性的主要标志;愤怒倾向、攻击性和愤世嫉俗是次要指标:这些研究结果支持在临床评估中使用 MMPI-3,以证实有关精神变态的其他信息来源,并提出供进一步考虑的假设。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Assessing psychopathic traits with the MMPI-3: Findings from correctional, university, and community samples.","authors":"Dustin B Wygant, Martin Sellbom","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000552","DOIUrl":"10.1037/lhb0000552","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) instruments have a long history with respect to the assessment of psychopathic personality traits. The most recent version, the MMPI-3, should be in a good position to continue this tradition, and the aim of the current research was to evaluate its scales for this purpose. We examined, on the basis of previous research, how well conceptually relevant MMPI-3 scales mapped onto dominant contemporary psychopathy models: the traditional three-factor model and triarchic psychopathy model.</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>We hypothesized that MMPI-3 markers of internalizing would be negatively correlated with boldness, whereas broad and specific markers of externalizing proclivities would be associated with disinhibition and antisociality. We also hypothesized that egocentricity and callousness would be associated with MMPI-3 scales measuring various features of externalizing, interpersonal aggression/antagonism, and grandiosity.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We used archival samples of male prison inmates (n = 452), community members with externalizing proclivities (n = 205), and university students (n = 645). These participants completed the Expanded Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale and the Triarchic Psychopathy Measure.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Zero-order correlation analyses indicated support for many of our hypotheses across samples, with notable exceptions. Regression and dominance analyses yielded information about the most potent MMPI-3 predictors of each psychopathy domain, with consistency across the three samples. Boldness was associated with low scores on Emotional/Internalizing Dysfunction, Low Positive Emotions, Shyness, and Negative Emotionality/Neuroticism and high scores on Self-Importance and Dominance. For meanness and disinhibition, we found substantial overlap with MMPI-3 scales (e.g., Behavioral/Externalizing Dysfunction, Antisocial Behavior). Meanness was indicated by high Aggression, Cynicism, Aggressiveness, and Disaffiliativeness; disinhibition/antisociality was primarily marked by high Antisocial Behavior, Hypomanic Activation, Impulsivity, and Disconstraint; and Anger Proneness, Aggression, and Cynicism were secondary indicators.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>These findings provide support for using the MMPI-3 in clinical assessments to corroborate other sources of information regarding psychopathy as well as generate hypotheses for further consideration. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"48 1","pages":"1-12"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140855662","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reexamining predictors of trial outcomes in New York State's sex offender civil management process. 重新审视纽约州性犯罪者民事管理程序中的审判结果预测因素。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-22 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000550
Nathan S Kemper, Marie L Reilly, Naomi J Freeman, Jeffrey C Sandler

Objective: In 2007, New York enacted the Sex Offender Management and Treatment Act, empowering the state to civilly manage individuals who have committed sexual offenses (respondents) and are deemed to have a mental abnormality (MA) that predisposes them to sexually recidivate after serving their criminal sentences. We sought to replicate and extend a previous study (Lu et al., 2015) to identify factors predicting legal decisions.

Hypotheses: We predicted, on the basis of previous research, that clinical information (e.g., diagnosis) as well as empirically supported risk factors (e.g., sexual deviance) would predict trial outcomes.

Method: We analyzed multiple pieces of demographic, criminogenic, and clinical data on three nested subsamples of respondents on the basis of the legal process: MA consent (n = 713), MA trial (n = 316), and disposition hearing (n = 643). The binary outcomes of interest were as follows: For the MA consent subsample, it was whether the respondent waived their MA trial; for the MA trial subsample, it was whether the respondent was found at trial to have an MA; and for the disposition hearing, it was whether the respondent was ordered to inpatient or outpatient civil management.

Results: The strongest predictor of waiving the trial was geographic location; respondents outside New York City and Long Island were more likely to waive their trials (ORs = 2.38-3.37). The strongest predictors of MA trial and disposition hearing outcomes were Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders diagnoses; pedophilia (ORs = 4.05-7.22) and sexual sadism (ORs = 2.68-7.03) diagnoses increased the likelihood of an MA finding and confinement order.

Conclusions: Judges and juries give significant weight to clinical information, particularly pedophilia diagnoses, when making civil management legal decisions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目 的 :2007 年,纽约州颁布了《性犯罪者管理与治疗法案》,授权州政府对犯有性犯罪(受访者)并被认为存在精神异常(MA)的个人进行民事管理,这种精神异常使他们在服刑期满后容易在性方面再次犯罪。我们试图复制并扩展之前的一项研究(Lu 等人,2015 年),以确定预测法律决定的因素:我们根据以往的研究预测,临床信息(如诊断)以及经验支持的风险因素(如性偏差)将预测审判结果:我们根据法律程序分析了三个嵌套子样本受访者的人口统计学、犯罪原因和临床数据:我们对三个嵌套子样本的人口、犯罪原因和临床数据进行了分析,这三个子样本的法律程序分别是:MA 同意(n = 713)、MA 审判(n = 316)和处置听证(n = 643)。相关的二元结果如下:对于精神病鉴定同意书子样本,是指受访者是否放弃精神病鉴定审判;对于精神病鉴定审判子样本,是指受访者是否在审判中被认定患有精神病鉴定;对于处置听证会,是指受访者是否被下令接受住院或门诊民事管理:放弃审判的最强预测因素是地理位置;纽约市和长岛以外的受访者更有可能放弃审判(ORs = 2.38-3.37)。精神障碍诊断与统计手册》中的精神障碍诊断是预测精神障碍审判和处置听证会结果的最有力因素;恋童癖(ORs = 4.05-7.22)和性虐待狂(ORs = 2.68-7.03)诊断增加了精神障碍鉴定结论和禁闭令的可能性:结论:法官和陪审团在做出民事管理法律裁决时,会非常重视临床信息,尤其是恋童癖诊断。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Reexamining predictors of trial outcomes in New York State's sex offender civil management process.","authors":"Nathan S Kemper, Marie L Reilly, Naomi J Freeman, Jeffrey C Sandler","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000550","DOIUrl":"10.1037/lhb0000550","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>In 2007, New York enacted the Sex Offender Management and Treatment Act, empowering the state to civilly manage individuals who have committed sexual offenses (respondents) and are deemed to have a mental abnormality (MA) that predisposes them to sexually recidivate after serving their criminal sentences. We sought to replicate and extend a previous study (Lu et al., 2015) to identify factors predicting legal decisions.</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>We predicted, on the basis of previous research, that clinical information (e.g., diagnosis) as well as empirically supported risk factors (e.g., sexual deviance) would predict trial outcomes.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We analyzed multiple pieces of demographic, criminogenic, and clinical data on three nested subsamples of respondents on the basis of the legal process: MA consent (<i>n</i> = 713), MA trial (<i>n</i> = 316), and disposition hearing (<i>n</i> = 643). The binary outcomes of interest were as follows: For the MA consent subsample, it was whether the respondent waived their MA trial; for the MA trial subsample, it was whether the respondent was found at trial to have an MA; and for the disposition hearing, it was whether the respondent was ordered to inpatient or outpatient civil management.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The strongest predictor of waiving the trial was geographic location; respondents outside New York City and Long Island were more likely to waive their trials (<i>OR</i>s = 2.38-3.37). The strongest predictors of MA trial and disposition hearing outcomes were <i>Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders</i> diagnoses; pedophilia (<i>OR</i>s = 4.05-7.22) and sexual sadism (<i>OR</i>s = 2.68-7.03) diagnoses increased the likelihood of an MA finding and confinement order.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Judges and juries give significant weight to clinical information, particularly pedophilia diagnoses, when making civil management legal decisions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"67-82"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139514042","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Advancing the Shift-of-Strategy approach: Shifting suspects' strategies in extended interviews. 推进策略转换方法:在扩展访谈中转变嫌疑人的策略。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000554
Lina Nyström, Timothy J Luke, Pär Anders Granhag, Aziz-Kaan Dönmez, Malin Ekelund, Pär D Stern

Objective: Interviewers often face the challenge of obtaining information from suspects who are willing to speak but are motivated to conceal incriminating information. The Shift-of-Strategy (SoS) approach is an interviewing technique designed to obtain new information from such suspects. This study provides a robust empirical test of the SoS approach using more complex crime events and longer interviews than previously tested as well as testing a new variation of the approach (SoS-Reinforcement) that included a strategic summary of the suspect's statement. We compared this new variation with a standard version of the approach (SoS-Standard) and an interviewing approach that involved no confrontation of discrepancies in the suspects' statements (Direct).

Hypotheses: We predicted that the two SoS versions would outperform the Direct condition in terms of participants' disclosure of previously unknown information. We also predicted that SoS-Reinforcement would outperform SoS-Standard. Finally, we expected that participants in the SoS conditions would not assess the interview or the interviewer more poorly than participants in Direct.

Method: A total of 300 participants completed an online mock crime procedure, and they were subsequently interviewed with one of the three interviewing techniques. Following the interview, participants provided assessments of their experiences being interviewed.

Results: Participants in both SoS-Standard (d = 0.49, 95% confidence interval [CI: 0.21, 0.78]) and SoS-Reinforcement (d = 0.59, 95% CI [0.30, 0.87]) disclosed more previously unknown information than participants in the Direct condition, but SoS-Reinforcement did not outperform SoS-Standard (d = 0.08, 95% CI [-0.20, 0.36]). Participants in SoS-Reinforcement assessed their experience more negatively than those in Direct. No such differences were observed in the remaining two-way comparisons.

Conclusions: The study provides support for the effectiveness of eliciting new information through the SoS approach and illuminates possible experiential downsides with being subjected to the SoS-Reinforcement approach. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:访谈者经常面临的挑战是,如何从愿意开口但有隐瞒罪证动机的疑犯口中获取信息。策略转换(SoS)方法是一种访谈技巧,旨在从这类嫌疑人那里获取新信息。本研究使用比以往更复杂的犯罪事件和更长的访谈时间,对策略转换法进行了有力的实证测试,并测试了策略转换法的新变体(策略转换-强化),其中包括对嫌疑人陈述的策略性总结。我们将这种新方法与标准版本的方法(SoS-标准)和不涉及嫌疑人陈述差异的面谈方法(直接)进行了比较:我们预测,就参与者披露先前未知信息的情况而言,两个 SoS 版本的效果将优于 "直接 "条件。我们还预测,"强化 "SoS 将优于 "标准 "SoS。最后,我们预计,SoS 条件下的参与者对访谈或访谈者的评价不会比直接条件下的参与者更差:共有 300 名参与者完成了在线模拟犯罪程序,随后他们使用三种面试技巧中的一种接受了面试。访谈结束后,参与者对自己的访谈经历进行了评估:结果:SoS-标准(d = 0.49,95% 置信区间[CI: 0.21, 0.78])和 SoS-强化(d = 0.59,95% 置信区间[0.30, 0.87])条件下的参与者比直接条件下的参与者披露了更多之前未知的信息,但 SoS-强化条件下的参与者披露的信息并没有超过 SoS-标准(d = 0.08,95% 置信区间[-0.20, 0.36])。SoS-Reinforcement参与实验者对其体验的评价比Direct参与实验者更消极。在其余的双向比较中没有观察到这种差异:本研究为通过 SoS 方法激发新信息的有效性提供了支持,并揭示了采用 SoS-强化方法可能带来的体验上的负面影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Advancing the Shift-of-Strategy approach: Shifting suspects' strategies in extended interviews.","authors":"Lina Nyström, Timothy J Luke, Pär Anders Granhag, Aziz-Kaan Dönmez, Malin Ekelund, Pär D Stern","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000554","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000554","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Interviewers often face the challenge of obtaining information from suspects who are willing to speak but are motivated to conceal incriminating information. The Shift-of-Strategy (SoS) approach is an interviewing technique designed to obtain new information from such suspects. This study provides a robust empirical test of the SoS approach using more complex crime events and longer interviews than previously tested as well as testing a new variation of the approach (SoS-Reinforcement) that included a strategic summary of the suspect's statement. We compared this new variation with a standard version of the approach (SoS-Standard) and an interviewing approach that involved no confrontation of discrepancies in the suspects' statements (Direct).</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>We predicted that the two SoS versions would outperform the Direct condition in terms of participants' disclosure of previously unknown information. We also predicted that SoS-Reinforcement would outperform SoS-Standard. Finally, we expected that participants in the SoS conditions would not assess the interview or the interviewer more poorly than participants in Direct.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>A total of 300 participants completed an online mock crime procedure, and they were subsequently interviewed with one of the three interviewing techniques. Following the interview, participants provided assessments of their experiences being interviewed.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Participants in both SoS-Standard (d = 0.49, 95% confidence interval [CI: 0.21, 0.78]) and SoS-Reinforcement (d = 0.59, 95% CI [0.30, 0.87]) disclosed more previously unknown information than participants in the Direct condition, but SoS-Reinforcement did not outperform SoS-Standard (d = 0.08, 95% CI [-0.20, 0.36]). Participants in SoS-Reinforcement assessed their experience more negatively than those in Direct. No such differences were observed in the remaining two-way comparisons.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The study provides support for the effectiveness of eliciting new information through the SoS approach and illuminates possible experiential downsides with being subjected to the SoS-Reinforcement approach. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"48 1","pages":"50-66"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140855661","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do inconclusive forensic decisions disadvantage the innocent? 不确定的法医裁决是否会使无辜者处于不利地位?
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000553
Stephanie Madon, Kayla A Burd, Max Guyll

Objective: Two experiments examined the potential for inconclusive forensic decisions to disadvantage the innocent.

Hypotheses: Both experiments tested the hypothesis that inconclusive decisions produce more incriminating legal judgments than do clearly exculpatory forensic decisions. Experiment 2 also examined whether this hypothesized effect conformed to a confirmation bias, a communication error, or perceptual accuracy.

Method: In Experiment 1 (N = 492), a forensic expert testified that physical evidence recovered from a crime scene either matched or did not match a suspect's evidence or produced an inconclusive result. In Experiment 2 (N = 1,002), a forensic expert testified that physical evidence recovered from a crime scene either matched or did not match a suspect's evidence, produced an inconclusive result, or was unsuitable for analysis. A fifth condition omitted the forensic evidence and expert testimony.

Results: The inconclusive decision produced less incriminating legal judgments than did the match forensic decision (|d|average = 0.96), more incriminating legal judgments than did the no-match forensic decision (|d|average = 0.62), and equivalent legal judgments to the unsuitable decision (|d|average = 0.12) and to legal judgments made in the absence of forensic evidence (|d|average = 0.07). These results suggest that participants interpreted the inconclusive decision to be forensically neutral, which is consistent with a communication error.

Conclusion: The findings provide preliminary support for the idea that inconclusive decisions can put the innocent at risk of wrongful conviction by depriving them of a clearly exculpatory forensic decision. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:两个实验研究了法医鉴定的不确定裁决可能对无辜者不利:两个实验都检验了这样一个假设,即与明显可以开脱罪责的法医裁决相比,不确定的裁决会产生更多有罪的法律判断。实验 2 还检验了这一假设效应是否与确认偏差、沟通错误或知觉准确性相一致:在实验 1(N = 492)中,法医专家证明从犯罪现场找到的物证与嫌疑人的证据相符或不相符,或得出不确定的结果。在实验 2(N = 1 002)中,法医专家证明从犯罪现场找到的物证与嫌疑人的物证相符或不相符,或得出不确定的结果,或不适合进行分析。第五种情况省略了法医证据和专家证词:结果:与法医证据匹配的结果相比,不确定的结果产生的有罪法律判断较少(|d|平均值=0.96),与法医证据不匹配的结果相比,不确定的结果产生的有罪法律判断较多(|d|平均值=0.62),与不适合的结果(|d|平均值=0.12)以及在没有法医证据的情况下产生的法律判断(|d|平均值=0.07)相当。这些结果表明,被试将不确定的决定解释为法医中性的,这与沟通错误是一致的:结论:研究结果初步支持了这样一种观点,即不确定的裁决可能会使无辜者因无法获得明确的开脱罪责的法医裁决而面临被错误定罪的风险。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Do inconclusive forensic decisions disadvantage the innocent?","authors":"Stephanie Madon, Kayla A Burd, Max Guyll","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000553","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000553","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Two experiments examined the potential for inconclusive forensic decisions to disadvantage the innocent.</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>Both experiments tested the hypothesis that inconclusive decisions produce more incriminating legal judgments than do clearly exculpatory forensic decisions. Experiment 2 also examined whether this hypothesized effect conformed to a confirmation bias, a communication error, or perceptual accuracy.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>In Experiment 1 (N = 492), a forensic expert testified that physical evidence recovered from a crime scene either matched or did not match a suspect's evidence or produced an inconclusive result. In Experiment 2 (N = 1,002), a forensic expert testified that physical evidence recovered from a crime scene either matched or did not match a suspect's evidence, produced an inconclusive result, or was unsuitable for analysis. A fifth condition omitted the forensic evidence and expert testimony.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The inconclusive decision produced less incriminating legal judgments than did the match forensic decision (|d|<sub>average</sub> = 0.96), more incriminating legal judgments than did the no-match forensic decision (|d|<sub>average</sub> = 0.62), and equivalent legal judgments to the unsuitable decision (|d|<sub>average</sub> = 0.12) and to legal judgments made in the absence of forensic evidence (|d|<sub>average</sub> = 0.07). These results suggest that participants interpreted the inconclusive decision to be forensically neutral, which is consistent with a communication error.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The findings provide preliminary support for the idea that inconclusive decisions can put the innocent at risk of wrongful conviction by depriving them of a clearly exculpatory forensic decision. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"48 1","pages":"33-49"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140870253","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An offer you cannot refuse: Plea offer size affects innocent but not guilty defendants' perceptions of voluntariness. 你无法拒绝的提议:认罪提议的大小会影响无罪而非有罪被告人对自愿性的认知。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2023-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000548
Melanie B Fessinger, Margaret Bull Kovera

Objective: We examined whether various plea outcomes-including sentence reduction size (smaller, larger), type (traditional guilty plea, Alford plea), and frame (plea discount, trial penalty)-differentially affected innocent and guilty defendants' perceptions of the voluntariness of their guilty pleas.

Hypotheses: We hypothesized (1) guilty defendants would rate guilty pleas as more voluntary than would innocent defendants; (2) defendants would rate larger sentence reductions either as more voluntary than smaller sentence reductions because they feel more fair or as less voluntary because they feel harder to reject; (3) defendants would rate guilty pleas as more voluntary when the plea offer was framed as a discount compared with a penalty; (4) penalty framing would differentially affect defendants offered large versus small sentence reductions; and (5) Alford pleas would differentially affect guilty versus innocent defendants.

Method: Adults from Qualtrics Research Panels (N = 1,518; Mage = 59.22 years; 52% male; 83% White, non-Hispanic) played the role of a defendant in a simulated plea decision-making process. They were either innocent or guilty of the accusation. The prosecutor offered them a plea deal that varied in sentence reduction size (smaller, versus larger), type (traditional versus Alford plea), and frame (plea discount versus trial penalty). Participants then decided how to plead and rated the voluntariness of the decision-making process.

Results: Plea outcomes affected innocent and guilty defendants in slightly different ways. Innocent and guilty defendants were less likely to plead guilty when the plea offer had a smaller compared with a larger sentence reduction. However, innocent defendants were less likely to plead guilty overall, required more prompting from their defense attorney to plead guilty, and rated the plea decision-making process as less voluntary than did guilty defendants. Innocent defendants also rated the plea decision-making process as less voluntary when offered a smaller compared with larger sentence reduction and when they were offered an Alford plea compared with a traditional guilty plea. Framing the plea offer as a discount or a penalty did not affect defendants' perceptions of voluntariness.

Conclusion: Variations in plea outcomes affect defendants' perceptions of voluntariness. Moreover, at least some courts' definitions of voluntariness do not align with how laypeople-and thus, possible defendants-view the same construct. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:我们研究了各种认罪结果--包括减刑幅度(较小、较大)、类型(传统认罪、阿尔弗德认罪)和框架(认罪折扣、审判处罚)--是否会不同程度地影响无辜被告和有罪被告对认罪自愿性的看法:我们假设:(1) 与无罪被告相比,有罪被告会认为认罪更自愿;(2) 与较小的减刑相比,被告会认为较大的减刑更自愿,因为他们感觉更公平,或者认为较小的减刑更不自愿,因为他们感觉更难拒绝;(3) 与刑罚相比,如果辩诉提议被设定为折扣,被告人会认为认罪更自愿;(4) 刑罚设定对减刑幅度大的被告人和减刑幅度小的被告人的影响不同;(5) Alford 辩诉对有罪被告人和无罪被告人的影响不同。研究方法:来自 Qualtrics 研究小组的成年人(人数 = 1,518;年龄 = 59.22 岁;52% 为男性;83% 为白人,非西班牙裔)在模拟认罪求情决策过程中扮演被告。他们要么是无辜的,要么是有罪的。检察官向他们提出不同减刑幅度(较小与较大)、类型(传统认罪与阿尔弗德认罪)和框架(认罪折扣与审判处罚)的认罪协议。然后,参与者决定如何认罪,并对决策过程的自愿性进行评分:认罪结果对无罪被告和有罪被告的影响略有不同。当辩诉提议的减刑幅度小于减刑幅度大时,无辜被告和有罪被告认罪的可能性都较小。然而,与有罪被告人相比,无罪被告人总体上认罪的可能性较低,需要辩护律师更多的提示才能认罪,并且对认罪决定过程的自愿性评价较低。与传统的认罪相比,无罪被告在获得较小减刑幅度时,与获得较大减刑幅度时相比,以及在获得阿尔弗德认罪协议时,对认罪求情决策过程的自愿性评价也较低。将认罪协议描述为减刑或处罚并不影响被告对自愿性的看法:抗辩结果的不同会影响被告对自愿性的认知。此外,至少有些法院对自愿性的定义与普通人--也就是可能的被告人--对同一概念的看法并不一致。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"An offer you cannot refuse: Plea offer size affects innocent but not guilty defendants' perceptions of voluntariness.","authors":"Melanie B Fessinger, Margaret Bull Kovera","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000548","DOIUrl":"10.1037/lhb0000548","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>We examined whether various plea outcomes-including sentence reduction size (smaller, larger), type (traditional guilty plea, Alford plea), and frame (plea discount, trial penalty)-differentially affected innocent and guilty defendants' perceptions of the voluntariness of their guilty pleas.</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>We hypothesized (1) guilty defendants would rate guilty pleas as more voluntary than would innocent defendants; (2) defendants would rate larger sentence reductions either as more voluntary than smaller sentence reductions because they feel more fair or as less voluntary because they feel harder to reject; (3) defendants would rate guilty pleas as more voluntary when the plea offer was framed as a discount compared with a penalty; (4) penalty framing would differentially affect defendants offered large versus small sentence reductions; and (5) Alford pleas would differentially affect guilty versus innocent defendants.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Adults from Qualtrics Research Panels (N = 1,518; M<sub>age</sub> = 59.22 years; 52% male; 83% White, non-Hispanic) played the role of a defendant in a simulated plea decision-making process. They were either innocent or guilty of the accusation. The prosecutor offered them a plea deal that varied in sentence reduction size (smaller, versus larger), type (traditional versus Alford plea), and frame (plea discount versus trial penalty). Participants then decided how to plead and rated the voluntariness of the decision-making process.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Plea outcomes affected innocent and guilty defendants in slightly different ways. Innocent and guilty defendants were less likely to plead guilty when the plea offer had a smaller compared with a larger sentence reduction. However, innocent defendants were less likely to plead guilty overall, required more prompting from their defense attorney to plead guilty, and rated the plea decision-making process as less voluntary than did guilty defendants. Innocent defendants also rated the plea decision-making process as less voluntary when offered a smaller compared with larger sentence reduction and when they were offered an Alford plea compared with a traditional guilty plea. Framing the plea offer as a discount or a penalty did not affect defendants' perceptions of voluntariness.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Variations in plea outcomes affect defendants' perceptions of voluntariness. Moreover, at least some courts' definitions of voluntariness do not align with how laypeople-and thus, possible defendants-view the same construct. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"47 6","pages":"619-633"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832285","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
More than race? Intragroup differences by gender and age in perceptions of police among street-identified Black men and women. 不仅仅是种族问题?街头黑人男女对警察的看法在性别和年龄上的组内差异。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2023-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000544
Brooklynn K Hitchens, Jeaneé C Miller, Yasser Arafat Payne, Ivan Y Sun, Isabella Castillo

Objective: Whereas studies have documented racial differences in attitudes toward police between White and Black Americans, relatively little is known about the intragroup, gender-based variations among urban Black residents involved in criminal activity (i.e., street-identified men and women).

Hypotheses: We hypothesized Black women would be more likely to believe in police legitimacy and positive intent than men (Hypothesis 1), especially among the younger segment of the sample (Hypothesis 2). We also expected this relationship to be moderated by contact with police (Hypothesis 3) and experiences with victimization (Hypothesis 4).

Method: Using survey data, this Street Participatory Action Research project examined the direct and interactional relationships between gender, age, involuntary police contact, personal victimization, and participants' perceptions of police legitimacy and positive intent. Participants included 515 street-identified Black men (40.4%; n = 208) and women (59.6%; n = 307), ages 18-35 years, from two high-crime neighborhoods in Wilmington, Delaware.

Results: Women had significantly higher perceptions of police legitimacy than did men (Hypothesis 1). While older participants tended to have lower perceptions that the police behave with positive intent, age did not moderate the relationship between gender and perceptions of police (Hypothesis 2). The relationship between gender and perceptions of positive police intent was moderated by involuntary police contact (Hypothesis 3) and experiences of victimization (Hypothesis 4).

Conclusions: Contrary to existing literature, prior involuntary police contact mattered more for street-identified Black women than men in predicting perceptions of police. Experiences of victimization were also more impactful for these perceptions for street-identified Black women than men. Men's perceptions of positive police intent were consistent, regardless of the frequency of police contact, whereas women's favorable perceptions declined with more police contact and victimization experiences, and they eventually became more critical of the police than their male counterparts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:尽管已有研究记录了美国白人和黑人在对待警察态度上的种族差异,但对于参与犯罪活动的城市黑人居民(即街头男性和女性)在群体内部、基于性别的差异却知之甚少:我们假设黑人女性比男性更有可能相信警察的合法性和积极意图(假设 1),尤其是在样本中的年轻群体中(假设 2)。我们还预计这种关系会受到与警察接触(假设 3)和受害经历(假设 4)的调节:本街头参与行动研究项目利用调查数据,研究了性别、年龄、非自愿与警察接触、个人受害经历以及参与者对警察合法性和积极意图的看法之间的直接关系和互动关系。参与者包括来自特拉华州威尔明顿两个高犯罪率社区的 515 名街头黑人男性(40.4%;n = 208)和女性(59.6%;n = 307),年龄在 18-35 岁之间:女性对警察合法性的看法明显高于男性(假设 1)。虽然年龄较大的参与者对警察行为的积极意图的感知往往较低,但年龄并不影响性别与对警察的感知之间的关系(假设 2)。非自愿接触警察(假设 3)和受害经历(假设 4)调节了性别与对警察积极意图的看法之间的关系:与现有文献相反,在预测对警察的看法时,曾非自愿接触过警察的街头黑人女性比男性更重要。与男性相比,受害经历对街头黑人女性的影响也更大。无论与警察接触的频率如何,男性对警察积极意图的看法都是一致的,而女性对警察的好感则随着与警察接触的增多和受害经历的增加而下降,最终她们对警察的批评比男性更多。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"More than race? Intragroup differences by gender and age in perceptions of police among street-identified Black men and women.","authors":"Brooklynn K Hitchens, Jeaneé C Miller, Yasser Arafat Payne, Ivan Y Sun, Isabella Castillo","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000544","DOIUrl":"10.1037/lhb0000544","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Whereas studies have documented racial differences in attitudes toward police between White and Black Americans, relatively little is known about the intragroup, gender-based variations among urban Black residents involved in criminal activity (i.e., street-identified men and women).</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>We hypothesized Black women would be more likely to believe in police legitimacy and positive intent than men (Hypothesis 1), especially among the younger segment of the sample (Hypothesis 2). We also expected this relationship to be moderated by contact with police (Hypothesis 3) and experiences with victimization (Hypothesis 4).</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Using survey data, this Street Participatory Action Research project examined the direct and interactional relationships between gender, age, involuntary police contact, personal victimization, and participants' perceptions of police legitimacy and positive intent. Participants included 515 street-identified Black men (40.4%; n = 208) and women (59.6%; n = 307), ages 18-35 years, from two high-crime neighborhoods in Wilmington, Delaware.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Women had significantly higher perceptions of police legitimacy than did men (Hypothesis 1). While older participants tended to have lower perceptions that the police behave with positive intent, age did not moderate the relationship between gender and perceptions of police (Hypothesis 2). The relationship between gender and perceptions of positive police intent was moderated by involuntary police contact (Hypothesis 3) and experiences of victimization (Hypothesis 4).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Contrary to existing literature, prior involuntary police contact mattered more for street-identified Black women than men in predicting perceptions of police. Experiences of victimization were also more impactful for these perceptions for street-identified Black women than men. Men's perceptions of positive police intent were consistent, regardless of the frequency of police contact, whereas women's favorable perceptions declined with more police contact and victimization experiences, and they eventually became more critical of the police than their male counterparts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"47 6","pages":"634-653"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832287","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Universal mandatory reporting policies show null effects in a statewide college sample. 在全州高校样本中,普遍强制报告政策显示效果为零。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2023-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000546
Tara N Richards, Kathryn J Holland, Allison E Cipriano, Alyssa Nystrom

Objective: It is widely assumed that universal mandatory reporting policies (MRPs) for sexual misconduct are important for campus safety, but there is little evidence to support these assumptions.

Hypotheses: Given the exploratory nature of this research, no formal hypotheses were tested. We did not expect universal MRPs to be significantly associated with increased reporting or postreporting outcomes.

Method: Data on MRPs and sexual misconduct reporting in annual security reports and to Title IX coordinators at institutions of higher education in New York (N = 188) were used to examine the prevalence of universal MRPs as well as the relationship between MRPs and reporting and postreporting outcomes.

Results: Descriptives showed that 44% of institutions of higher education have a universal MRP. Multivariate linear regression models indicated that universal MRPs were not significantly related to reporting in annual security reports; reports to Title IX coordinators, campus police, campus safety or security officers; or rates of referrals to additional services, no-contact orders, access to the judicial conduct process for sexual misconduct, or findings of student responsibility for sexual misconduct.

Conclusions: Our findings raise concerns about the widespread implementation of MRPs and highlight the need for future research on their impact on student-survivor reporting and access to remedies and resources. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

目的人们普遍认为,针对性不端行为的普遍强制报告政策(MRPs)对校园安全非常重要,但几乎没有证据支持这些假设:鉴于本研究的探索性质,我们没有测试正式的假设。我们并不期望普遍的 MRPs 会与更多的报告或报告后的结果有明显关联:方法:使用纽约高等教育机构(N = 188)年度安全报告中的 MRPs 和性不端行为报告数据以及向 Title IX 协调员报告的数据,研究普遍 MRPs 的普遍性以及 MRPs 与报告和报告后结果之间的关系:结果:描述性数据显示,44% 的高等院校具有普遍的 MRP。多变量线性回归模型表明,普遍的 MRP 与年度安全报告中的报告、向 Title IX 协调员、校警、校园安全或保安人员的报告、转介到额外服务的比率、禁止接触令、进入性不端行为司法程序或学生对性不端行为的责任认定没有明显关系:我们的研究结果引起了人们对广泛实施 MRPs 的担忧,并强调了未来研究 MRPs 对学生-幸存者报告以及获得补救和资源的影响的必要性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Universal mandatory reporting policies show null effects in a statewide college sample.","authors":"Tara N Richards, Kathryn J Holland, Allison E Cipriano, Alyssa Nystrom","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000546","DOIUrl":"10.1037/lhb0000546","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>It is widely assumed that universal mandatory reporting policies (MRPs) for sexual misconduct are important for campus safety, but there is little evidence to support these assumptions.</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>Given the exploratory nature of this research, no formal hypotheses were tested. We did not expect universal MRPs to be significantly associated with increased reporting or postreporting outcomes.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Data on MRPs and sexual misconduct reporting in annual security reports and to Title IX coordinators at institutions of higher education in New York (N = 188) were used to examine the prevalence of universal MRPs as well as the relationship between MRPs and reporting and postreporting outcomes.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Descriptives showed that 44% of institutions of higher education have a universal MRP. Multivariate linear regression models indicated that universal MRPs were not significantly related to reporting in annual security reports; reports to Title IX coordinators, campus police, campus safety or security officers; or rates of referrals to additional services, no-contact orders, access to the judicial conduct process for sexual misconduct, or findings of student responsibility for sexual misconduct.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Our findings raise concerns about the widespread implementation of MRPs and highlight the need for future research on their impact on student-survivor reporting and access to remedies and resources. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"47 6","pages":"686-699"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832288","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
White mock jurors' moral emotional responses to viewing female victim photographs depend on the victim's race. 白人模拟陪审员在观看女性受害者照片时的道德情感反应取决于受害者的种族。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2023-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000545
Hannah J Phalen, Jessica M Salerno, Madison Adamoli, Janice Nadler

Objective: Jurors often see both premortem photographs of female murder victims before death and postmortem photographs after death. Postmortem photographs are often probative but might prejudicially heighten jurors' other-condemning emotions, such as anger and disgust. Premortem photographs are often not probative and might prejudicially heighten jurors' other-suffering emotions, such as sympathy and empathy. We examined how victim race changes the impact of pre- and postmortem photographs on participants' moral emotions and, in turn, their verdicts.

Hypotheses: We hypothesized that seeing postmortem (vs. no) photographs would increase convictions through other-condemning emotions for White, but not Latina or Black, victims. We also hypothesized that seeing both pre- and postmortem (vs. only postmortem) photographs would further increase convictions through other-suffering emotions, again for White, but not Latina or Black, female victims.

Method: White participants (N = 1,261) watched a murder trial video. We manipulated the victim's race (White, Black, or Latina) and whether participants saw no victim photographs, premortem photographs of a female victim, postmortem photographs of a female victim, or both pre- and postmortem photographs. Participants reported the emotions they felt during the trial and chose a verdict.

Results: Seeing postmortem (vs. no) victim photographs increased White participants' guilty verdicts through other-condemning emotions when the female victim was White or Latina but not when she was Black. Seeing the combination of pre- and postmortem photographs increased White participants' convictions through other-suffering emotions when the victim was a White woman but not when she was Latina or Black.

Conclusions: Attorneys and judges should consider that jurors' emotional reactions to victim photographs are felt selectively depending on the victim's race and could exacerbate racial biases in jurors' judgments. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:陪 審 員 經 常 會 看 到 女 性 謀 殺 受 害 人 生 前 的 死 前 照 片 和 死 後 的 死 後 照 片 。死 後 的 照 片 通 常 可 以 提 供 證 據 , 但 可 能 會 偏 見 性 地 加 強 陪 審 員 的 其 他 譴 責 情 緒 , 例 如 憤 怒 和 厭 惡 。死前照片通常不具证明力,但可能会偏见性地增强陪审员的其他痛苦情绪,如同情和共鸣。我们研究了受害者种族如何改变死前和死后照片对参与者道德情绪的影响,进而影响他们的判决:我们假设,看到死后照片(与没有看到死后照片相比)会通过其他谴责情绪增加白人受害者的定罪率,但不会增加拉丁裔或黑人受害者的定罪率。我们还假设,同时看到死前和死后(与只看到死后)的照片会通过他者痛苦情绪进一步增加白人女性受害者的定罪率,但不会增加拉丁裔或黑人女性受害者的定罪率:方法:白人参与者(N = 1,261)观看谋杀案审判视频。我们对受害者的种族(白人、黑人或拉丁裔)以及参与者是否没有看到受害者照片、女性受害者死前照片、女性受害者死后照片或同时看到死前和死后照片进行了操纵。参与者报告了他们在审判过程中的感受,并选择了一项判决:结果:当女性受害者是白人或拉丁裔时,看到受害者尸体解剖后的照片(与没有看到尸体解剖后的照片相比)会通过其他谴责情绪增加白人参与者的有罪判决,但当她是黑人时则不会。当受害者是白人女性时,看到尸体解剖前和尸体解剖后照片的组合会增加白人参与者通过他者痛苦情绪做出的有罪判决,而当受害者是拉丁裔或黑人时则不会:律师和法官应该考虑到,陪审员对受害者照片的情绪反应会根据受害者的种族而有选择性地感受到,这可能会加剧陪审员判断中的种族偏见。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"White mock jurors' moral emotional responses to viewing female victim photographs depend on the victim's race.","authors":"Hannah J Phalen, Jessica M Salerno, Madison Adamoli, Janice Nadler","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000545","DOIUrl":"10.1037/lhb0000545","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Jurors often see both premortem photographs of female murder victims before death and postmortem photographs after death. Postmortem photographs are often probative but might prejudicially heighten jurors' other-condemning emotions, such as anger and disgust. Premortem photographs are often not probative and might prejudicially heighten jurors' other-suffering emotions, such as sympathy and empathy. We examined how victim race changes the impact of pre- and postmortem photographs on participants' moral emotions and, in turn, their verdicts.</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>We hypothesized that seeing postmortem (vs. no) photographs would increase convictions through other-condemning emotions for White, but not Latina or Black, victims. We also hypothesized that seeing both pre- and postmortem (vs. only postmortem) photographs would further increase convictions through other-suffering emotions, again for White, but not Latina or Black, female victims.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>White participants (N = 1,261) watched a murder trial video. We manipulated the victim's race (White, Black, or Latina) and whether participants saw no victim photographs, premortem photographs of a female victim, postmortem photographs of a female victim, or both pre- and postmortem photographs. Participants reported the emotions they felt during the trial and chose a verdict.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Seeing postmortem (vs. no) victim photographs increased White participants' guilty verdicts through other-condemning emotions when the female victim was White or Latina but not when she was Black. Seeing the combination of pre- and postmortem photographs increased White participants' convictions through other-suffering emotions when the victim was a White woman but not when she was Latina or Black.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Attorneys and judges should consider that jurors' emotional reactions to victim photographs are felt selectively depending on the victim's race and could exacerbate racial biases in jurors' judgments. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"47 6","pages":"666-685"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832289","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive-behavioral reciprocity: Testing the bidirectional relationship between antisocial cognition and delinquency. 认知行为互惠:测试反社会认知与犯罪之间的双向关系。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2023-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000549
Glenn D Walters

Objective: Although reciprocity between variables is a topic of interest in the field of criminology, we cannot simply assume that all or even most criminological relationships are bidirectional without testing them empirically. The objective of the current investigation was to test whether delinquency and antisocial cognition are reciprocally or bidirectionally related.

Hypotheses: The hypotheses evaluated as part of the present study proposed that antisocial cognition would predict delinquency, delinquency would predict antisocial cognition, and bidirectional models would display significantly better fit than the unidirectional models on which they are based.

Method: Using data from the Pathways to Desistance study (1,354 serious justice-involved youths), I explored whether antisocial cognition predicts delinquency and a change in delinquency and whether delinquency predicts antisocial cognition and a change in antisocial cognition. I paired two forms of antisocial cognition-moral neutralization and cognitive impulsivity-with delinquency to predict a single future outcome with a zero-order correlation and a lagged outcome or change with a partial correlation.

Results: Findings showed that 40 out of 40 prospective zero-order correlations and 36 out of 40 prospective partial correlations achieved significance, with moderate and small effect sizes, respectively. Structural equation modeling revealed that the bidirectional models linking moral neutralization to delinquency and cognitive impulsivity to delinquency using lagged outcome measures both achieved significantly better fit than the unidimensional models on which they were based.

Conclusion: The results of this study are congruent with the conclusion that the relationship between antisocial cognition and delinquency is reciprocal and that antisocial cognition is as much a predictor of delinquency as delinquency is a predictor of antisocial cognition. Thus, both patterns need to be taken into account for the purposes of theory integration in criminology, clinical practice in forensic psychology, and policy implementation in criminal justice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:尽管变量之间的互惠性是犯罪学领域中一个值得关注的话题,但我们不能简单地认为所有甚至大多数犯罪学关系都是双向的,而不对其进行实证检验。本次调查的目的是检验犯罪与反社会认知之间是互惠关系还是双向关系:假设:本研究评估的假设认为,反社会认知可以预测犯罪,犯罪可以预测反社会认知,双向模型的拟合效果明显优于其所依据的单向模型:我利用 "通往脱罪之路"(Pathways to Desistance)研究(1,354 名严重触犯法律的青少年)的数据,探讨了反社会认知是否能预测青少年犯罪和青少年犯罪的变化,以及青少年犯罪是否能预测反社会认知和反社会认知的变化。我将两种形式的反社会认知--道德中和与认知冲动--与犯罪配对,用零阶相关性预测未来的单一结果,用部分相关性预测滞后结果或变化:结果表明,40 个前瞻性零阶相关性中有 40 个达到了显著性,40 个前瞻性部分相关性中有 36 个达到了显著性,效应大小分别为中等和较小。结构方程模型显示,利用滞后结果测量将道德中和与犯罪和认知冲动与犯罪联系起来的双向模型的拟合度都明显优于其所基于的单维模型:本研究的结果与以下结论一致:反社会认知与犯罪之间的关系是相互的,反社会认知是犯罪的预测因素,而犯罪也是反社会认知的预测因素。因此,在犯罪学的理论整合、法医心理学的临床实践和刑事司法的政策实施中,这两种模式都需要加以考虑。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Cognitive-behavioral reciprocity: Testing the bidirectional relationship between antisocial cognition and delinquency.","authors":"Glenn D Walters","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000549","DOIUrl":"10.1037/lhb0000549","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Although reciprocity between variables is a topic of interest in the field of criminology, we cannot simply assume that all or even most criminological relationships are bidirectional without testing them empirically. The objective of the current investigation was to test whether delinquency and antisocial cognition are reciprocally or bidirectionally related.</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>The hypotheses evaluated as part of the present study proposed that antisocial cognition would predict delinquency, delinquency would predict antisocial cognition, and bidirectional models would display significantly better fit than the unidirectional models on which they are based.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Using data from the Pathways to Desistance study (1,354 serious justice-involved youths), I explored whether antisocial cognition predicts delinquency and a change in delinquency and whether delinquency predicts antisocial cognition and a change in antisocial cognition. I paired two forms of antisocial cognition-moral neutralization and cognitive impulsivity-with delinquency to predict a single future outcome with a zero-order correlation and a lagged outcome or change with a partial correlation.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Findings showed that 40 out of 40 prospective zero-order correlations and 36 out of 40 prospective partial correlations achieved significance, with moderate and small effect sizes, respectively. Structural equation modeling revealed that the bidirectional models linking moral neutralization to delinquency and cognitive impulsivity to delinquency using lagged outcome measures both achieved significantly better fit than the unidimensional models on which they were based.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The results of this study are congruent with the conclusion that the relationship between antisocial cognition and delinquency is reciprocal and that antisocial cognition is as much a predictor of delinquency as delinquency is a predictor of antisocial cognition. Thus, both patterns need to be taken into account for the purposes of theory integration in criminology, clinical practice in forensic psychology, and policy implementation in criminal justice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"47 6","pages":"654-665"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832286","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
ACUTE-2007 and STABLE-2007 predict recidivism for men adjudicated for child sexual exploitation material offending. ACUTE-2007和STABLE-2007预测了因儿童性剥削材料犯罪而被判刑的男性的累犯率。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-24 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000540
Kelly M Babchishin, Ségolène Dibayula, Chiara McCulloch, R Karl Hanson, L Maaike Helmus

Objective: Risk assessment is essential to effective correctional practice. For individuals with contact sexual offenses, many risk tools are available. There are fewer options, however, for individuals whose sexual offending exclusively involves child sexual exploitation materials (CSEM; legally referred to in Canada and the United States as child pornography).

Hypotheses: The present study examined the predictive validity of the ACUTE-2007 and STABLE-2007 sexual recidivism risk tools among men with CSEM offenses. We expected these tools to show moderate predictive validity across study groups.

Method: We compared the scales' discrimination and calibration across three groups: (a) 1,042 men with contact sexual offenses against children (baseline comparison), (b) 228 men with exclusive CSEM offending (no contact sexual offenses), and (c) 80 men with both contact sexual offenses and CSEM offenses.

Results: We found that the ACUTE-2007 and STABLE-2007 total scores and items had comparable (and often better) discrimination for men with CSEM offending compared with contact sexual offending against children in the prediction of any sexual recidivism, violent recidivism, and any recidivism. Calibration analyses indicated that the overall sexual recidivism rates for the median ACUTE-2007 and STABLE-2007 scores were similar for men with exclusive CSEM offenses compared with men with any contact offending against children. Almost all of the sexual recidivism for the CSEM-exclusive group involved further CSEM offenses.

Conclusions: This study supports the use of these tools to rank-order men with CSEM offending in terms of their risk of reoffending and to help direct treatment and management efforts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目标:风险评估对于有效的矫正实践至关重要。对于有接触性性犯罪的个人,有许多风险工具可用。然而,对于性犯罪完全涉及儿童性剥削材料(CSEM;在加拿大和美国法律上称为儿童色情制品)的个人来说,选择更少。假设:本研究检验了ACUTE-2007和STABLE-2007性再犯风险工具在CSEM犯罪男性中的预测有效性。我们期望这些工具在研究组中显示出适度的预测有效性。方法:我们比较了三组的量表的歧视和校准:(a)1042名对儿童有接触性性犯罪的男性(基线比较),(b)228名有完全性侵犯行为的男性(无接触性犯罪),以及(c)80名同时有接触性犯罪和性侵犯行为。结果:我们发现,在预测任何性累犯、暴力累犯和任何累犯方面,ACUTE-2007和STABLE-2007总分和项目对患有CSEM犯罪的男性与对儿童的接触性性犯罪具有可比性(通常更好)的歧视。校准分析表明,与任何针对儿童的接触性犯罪的男性相比,患有单纯CSEM犯罪的男性在ACUTE-2007和STABLE-2007中位得分的总体性再犯率相似。CSEM专属群体的几乎所有性累犯都涉及进一步的CSEM犯罪。结论:本研究支持使用这些工具根据CSEM罪犯再次犯罪的风险对其进行排名,并有助于指导治疗和管理工作。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"ACUTE-2007 and STABLE-2007 predict recidivism for men adjudicated for child sexual exploitation material offending.","authors":"Kelly M Babchishin, Ségolène Dibayula, Chiara McCulloch, R Karl Hanson, L Maaike Helmus","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000540","DOIUrl":"10.1037/lhb0000540","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Risk assessment is essential to effective correctional practice. For individuals with contact sexual offenses, many risk tools are available. There are fewer options, however, for individuals whose sexual offending exclusively involves child sexual exploitation materials (CSEM; legally referred to in Canada and the United States as child pornography).</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>The present study examined the predictive validity of the ACUTE-2007 and STABLE-2007 sexual recidivism risk tools among men with CSEM offenses. We expected these tools to show moderate predictive validity across study groups.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We compared the scales' discrimination and calibration across three groups: (a) 1,042 men with contact sexual offenses against children (baseline comparison), (b) 228 men with exclusive CSEM offending (no contact sexual offenses), and (c) 80 men with both contact sexual offenses and CSEM offenses.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>We found that the ACUTE-2007 and STABLE-2007 total scores and items had comparable (and often better) discrimination for men with CSEM offending compared with contact sexual offending against children in the prediction of any sexual recidivism, violent recidivism, and any recidivism. Calibration analyses indicated that the overall sexual recidivism rates for the median ACUTE-2007 and STABLE-2007 scores were similar for men with exclusive CSEM offenses compared with men with any contact offending against children. Almost all of the sexual recidivism for the CSEM-exclusive group involved further CSEM offenses.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>This study supports the use of these tools to rank-order men with CSEM offending in terms of their risk of reoffending and to help direct treatment and management efforts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"606-618"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10060115","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Law and Human Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1