首页 > 最新文献

Law and Human Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Rearrest is associated with heightened callous-unemotional traits: No moderating effect of maternal relationship quality. 再次被捕与冷酷无情的非情感特质增强有关:母亲关系质量没有调节作用。
IF 2.4 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000567
Emma Louise Rodgers, Jordan Beardslee, Elizabeth Cauffman, Paul J Frick, Laurence Steinberg

Objective: The presence of callous-unemotional (CU) traits in adolescence predisposes youth to negative behavioral and social outcomes and may be particularly damaging to youth involved in the justice system. Whereas research has shown that CU traits predict later arrest, it remains unknown whether rearrest predicts changes in CU traits and whether these associations may be modified by maternal relationship quality. The present study assessed whether being rearrested predicted changes in CU traits and whether these associations varied by maternal warmth and maternal hostility.

Hypotheses: We hypothesized that self-reported CU traits would increase at data collection time points following rearrest. Further, we hypothesized that maternal warmth would buffer the negative effects of rearrest, whereas maternal hostility would not have a significant moderating effect on the associations.

Method: Hypotheses were tested using a large, multisite longitudinal data set of 1,216 justice-involved male youth (Mage = 15.82 years at baseline; 47% Latino, 38% Black/African American, 15% White). Data from a series of nine interviews (across a 7-year period) were used to determine associations between rearrest at one-time point and CU traits at the subsequent time point.

Results: Rearrest is associated with a significant increase in CU traits. However, these associations are not moderated by either maternal warmth or maternal hostility.

Conclusions: Rearrest predicts increases in a known risk factor for healthy socioemotional development among justice-involved youths (CU traits). Moreover, the way rearrest is associated with CU traits does not change depending on maternal warmth; rearrest is associated with increases in CU traits irrespective of the quality of a youth's relationship with their mother. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:青少年时期出现的冷漠无情(CU)特质容易导致不良的行为和社会结果,对卷入司法系统的青少年的伤害尤为严重。研究表明,CU特质可预测青少年日后的被捕情况,但再次被捕是否可预测CU特质的变化,以及这些关联是否会因母子关系质量而改变,目前仍是未知数。本研究评估了再次被逮捕是否会预示 CU 特质的变化,以及这些关联是否会因母亲的温暖和敌意而有所不同:我们假设,在再次被捕后的数据收集时间点,自我报告的 CU 特质会增加。此外,我们还假设,母亲的温暖会缓冲再次被捕的负面影响,而母亲的敌意则不会对这种关联产生显著的调节作用:我们使用了一个大型、多站点纵向数据集来验证假设,该数据集包含 1216 名涉案男性青少年(基线年龄为 15.82 岁;47% 为拉丁裔,38% 为黑人/非裔美国人,15% 为白人)。通过九次系列访谈(为期七年)获得的数据来确定某一时间点的再逮捕与随后时间点的 CU 特征之间的关联:结果:再次被捕与 CU 特征的显著增加有关。然而,这些关联并不受母性温暖或母性敌意的影响:结论:再次被捕可预测涉案青少年社会情感健康发展的已知风险因素(CU 特质)的增加。此外,再逮捕与CU特质的关联方式并不因母亲的温暖程度而改变;无论青少年与母亲的关系如何,再逮捕都与CU特质的增加有关。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Rearrest is associated with heightened callous-unemotional traits: No moderating effect of maternal relationship quality.","authors":"Emma Louise Rodgers, Jordan Beardslee, Elizabeth Cauffman, Paul J Frick, Laurence Steinberg","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000567","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000567","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>The presence of callous-unemotional (CU) traits in adolescence predisposes youth to negative behavioral and social outcomes and may be particularly damaging to youth involved in the justice system. Whereas research has shown that CU traits predict later arrest, it remains unknown whether rearrest predicts changes in CU traits and whether these associations may be modified by maternal relationship quality. The present study assessed whether being rearrested predicted changes in CU traits and whether these associations varied by maternal warmth and maternal hostility.</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>We hypothesized that self-reported CU traits would increase at data collection time points following rearrest. Further, we hypothesized that maternal warmth would buffer the negative effects of rearrest, whereas maternal hostility would not have a significant moderating effect on the associations.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Hypotheses were tested using a large, multisite longitudinal data set of 1,216 justice-involved male youth (Mage = 15.82 years at baseline; 47% Latino, 38% Black/African American, 15% White). Data from a series of nine interviews (across a 7-year period) were used to determine associations between rearrest at one-time point and CU traits at the subsequent time point.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Rearrest is associated with a significant increase in CU traits. However, these associations are not moderated by either maternal warmth or maternal hostility.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Rearrest predicts increases in a known risk factor for healthy socioemotional development among justice-involved youths (CU traits). Moreover, the way rearrest is associated with CU traits does not change depending on maternal warmth; rearrest is associated with increases in CU traits irrespective of the quality of a youth's relationship with their mother. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"48 3","pages":"203-213"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141471673","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of confession evidence on jurors' verdict decisions: A systematic review and meta-analysis. 供认证据对陪审员判决决定的影响:系统回顾与荟萃分析。
IF 2.4 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000563
Amelia Mindthoff, Patricia A Ferreira, Christian A Meissner

Objective: Over the past 4 decades, discrepant research findings have emerged in the juror-confession literature, prompting the need for a systematic review and meta-analysis that assesses the effect of confession evidence (coerced or noncoerced) on conviction rates and the efficacy of trial safeguards.

Hypotheses: We did not predict any directional hypotheses. Some studies show increased convictions when a confession is present (vs. not), regardless of whether that confession was coerced; other studies demonstrate that jurors are able to discount coerced confessions. Studies have also demonstrated sensitivity effects (safeguards aided jurors in making appropriate decisions), skepticism effects (safeguards led jurors to indiscriminately disregard confession evidence), or null effects with regard to expert testimony and jury instructions.

Method: We identified 83 independent samples (N = 24,860) that met our meta-analytic inclusion criteria. Using extracted Hedges' g effect sizes, we conducted both network meta-analysis and metaregression to address key research questions.

Results: Coerced and noncoerced confessions (vs. no confession) increased convictions (network gs = 0.34 and 0.70, respectively), yet coerced (vs. noncoerced) confessions reduced convictions (network g = -0.36). When jury instructions were employed (vs. not), convictions in coerced confession cases were reduced (this difference did not emerge for noncoerced confessions; a sensitivity effect). Expert testimony, however, reduced conviction likelihood regardless of whether a confession was coerced (a skepticism effect).

Conclusion: Confession evidence is persuasive, and although jurors appear to recognize the detrimental effect of coercive interrogation methods on confession reliability, they do not fully discount unreliable confessions. Educational safeguards are therefore needed, but more research is encouraged to identify the most effective forms of jury instructions and expert testimony. One potential reform could be in the interrogation room itself, as science-based interviewing approaches could provide jurors with more reliable defendant statement evidence that assists them in reaching appropriate verdict decisions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目 的 :在过去40年中,陪审员供词文献中出现了一些不一致的研究结果,这促使我们有必要进行系统回顾和荟萃分析,以评估供词证据(胁迫或非胁迫)对定罪率的影响以及审判保障措施的效力:我们没有预测任何方向性假设。一些研究表明,在有供词(与没有供词)的情况下,无论供词是否为逼供,定罪率都会提高;另一些研究则表明,陪审员能够对逼供不予考虑。研究还显示了敏感效应(保障措施有助于陪审员做出适当的决定)、怀疑效应(保障措施导致陪审员不加区别地无视供词证据),或在专家证词和陪审团指令方面的无效效应:我们确定了符合荟萃分析纳入标准的 83 个独立样本(N = 24,860)。利用提取的赫奇斯 g效应大小,我们进行了网络荟萃分析和元回归,以解决关键的研究问题:结果:逼供和非逼供(与不逼供相比)增加了定罪率(网络 gs 分别为 0.34 和 0.70),但逼供(与非逼供相比)减少了定罪率(网络 g = -0.36)。当陪审团采用(与不采用)指示时,刑讯逼供案件的定罪率降低(非刑讯逼供没有出现这种差异;这是一种敏感性效应)。然而,无论供词是否受到胁迫,专家证词都会降低定罪的可能性(怀疑效应):供词证据具有说服力,尽管陪审员似乎认识到了胁迫性讯问方法对供词可靠性的不利影响,但他们并没有完全否定不可靠的供词。因 此 , 有 必 要 採 取 教 育 性 的 保 障 措 施 , 但 我 們 鼓 勵 進 行 更 多 的 研 究 , 以 確 定 陪 審 團 指 示 和 專 家 證 供 的 最 有 效 形 式 。一种潜在的改革可能是在审讯室本身,因为以科学为基础的面谈方法可以为陪审员提供更可靠的被告人供述证据,从而协助他们做出适当的判决决定。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"The effect of confession evidence on jurors' verdict decisions: A systematic review and meta-analysis.","authors":"Amelia Mindthoff, Patricia A Ferreira, Christian A Meissner","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000563","DOIUrl":"10.1037/lhb0000563","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Over the past 4 decades, discrepant research findings have emerged in the juror-confession literature, prompting the need for a systematic review and meta-analysis that assesses the effect of confession evidence (coerced or noncoerced) on conviction rates and the efficacy of trial safeguards.</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>We did not predict any directional hypotheses. Some studies show increased convictions when a confession is present (vs. not), regardless of whether that confession was coerced; other studies demonstrate that jurors are able to discount coerced confessions. Studies have also demonstrated sensitivity effects (safeguards aided jurors in making appropriate decisions), skepticism effects (safeguards led jurors to indiscriminately disregard confession evidence), or null effects with regard to expert testimony and jury instructions.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We identified 83 independent samples (N = 24,860) that met our meta-analytic inclusion criteria. Using extracted Hedges' g effect sizes, we conducted both network meta-analysis and metaregression to address key research questions.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Coerced and noncoerced confessions (vs. no confession) increased convictions (network gs = 0.34 and 0.70, respectively), yet coerced (vs. noncoerced) confessions reduced convictions (network g = -0.36). When jury instructions were employed (vs. not), convictions in coerced confession cases were reduced (this difference did not emerge for noncoerced confessions; a sensitivity effect). Expert testimony, however, reduced conviction likelihood regardless of whether a confession was coerced (a skepticism effect).</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Confession evidence is persuasive, and although jurors appear to recognize the detrimental effect of coercive interrogation methods on confession reliability, they do not fully discount unreliable confessions. Educational safeguards are therefore needed, but more research is encouraged to identify the most effective forms of jury instructions and expert testimony. One potential reform could be in the interrogation room itself, as science-based interviewing approaches could provide jurors with more reliable defendant statement evidence that assists them in reaching appropriate verdict decisions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"48 3","pages":"163-181"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141471674","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attorneys' experiences, perceptions, and plea recommendations in child sexual abuse cases. 儿童性虐待案件中律师的经验、看法和认罪建议。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000551
Melanie B Fessinger, Bradley D McAuliff, Eliana Aronson, Kelly McWilliams

Objective: We examined attorneys' experiences, perceptions, and decisions regarding plea recommendations in child sexual cases.

Hypotheses: We hypothesized that characteristics of the child (age, relationship to alleged perpetrator) and the report (timing of disclosure, consistency across reports) would affect attorneys' perceptions of evidence strength, likelihood of conviction, and plea recommendations.

Method: We collected data from a national sample of actively practicing prosecutors (n = 217) and defense attorneys (n = 251) who had experience with child abuse cases. They averaged 18 years of experience practicing law, were slightly more likely to be men (53%) than women, and primarily identified as White, non-Hispanic (86%). In Part 1, attorneys answered general questions about their experiences in child sexual abuse cases. In Part 2, they reviewed materials from a hypothetical case that varied the child's age (5 years, 11 years), the child's relationship to the alleged perpetrator (familial, nonfamilial), the timing of the child's initial disclosure (1 week, 6 months), and the consistency of the child's report (inconsistent, consistent). They rated the evidence strength, estimated the likelihood of conviction, and assessed whether they would recommend that the defendant accept a plea offer or proceed to trial.

Results: In Part 1, attorneys reported that they often have access to police reports, information about the alleged perpetrator, and evidence from the child when making plea recommendations. They said that it was important to know about prior allegations against the alleged perpetrator or by the child when assessing their credibility. They reported that the length of the sentence, sex offender registration requirement, and possibility of time served guided their plea recommendations. In Part 2, the consistency of the child's report influenced their decisions the most; they rated the evidence against the defendant as stronger when the child was consistent across reports than when the child was inconsistent. Additionally, their perceptions of evidence strength drove their recommendations. When the evidence against the defendant was stronger, attorneys thought that the defendant was more likely to be convicted at trial; thus, prosecutors were less willing and defense attorneys were more willing to recommend a plea.

Conclusion: Similar to other cases, evidence strength and the perceived likelihood of conviction drive attorneys' decisions to offer or recommend a plea to a defendant in a child sexual abuse case. The consistency of the child's report plays a major role in predicting perceptions of evidence strength. Future research is needed to determine which other factors in child sexual abuse cases may also predict attorneys' perceptions and plea recommendations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的我们研究了律师在儿童性犯罪案件中的经验、看法以及有关认罪建议的决定:我们假设,儿童的特征(年龄、与被控施害者的关系)和报告的特征(披露时间、不同报告的一致性)会影响律师对证据力度、定罪可能性和认罪建议的看法:我们收集了全国范围内有虐童案件经验的积极执业检察官(217 人)和辩护律师(251 人)的样本数据。他们的平均执业年限为 18 年,男性(53%)略多于女性,主要身份为非西班牙裔白人(86%)。在第 1 部分中,律师们回答了有关他们处理儿童性虐待案件经验的一般性问题。在第 2 部分中,他们审查了一个假定案件的材料,该假定案件中儿童的年龄(5 岁、11 岁)、儿童与被指控施暴者的关系(家庭关系、非家庭关系)、儿童首次披露的时间(1 周、6 个月)以及儿童报告的一致性(不一致、一致)各不相同。他们对证据的强度进行了评级,估计了定罪的可能性,并评估了他们是否会建议被告接受认罪协议或进行审判:在第 1 部分中,律师报告说,在提出认罪求情建议时,他们通常可以获得警方报告、被指控犯罪人的信息以及儿童提供的证据。他们说,在评估被指控的犯罪人或儿童的可信度时,了解他们之前的指控非常重要。他们报告说,刑期长短、性犯罪者登记要求以及服刑时间的可能性是他们提出认罪求情建议的依据。在第 2 部分中,儿童报告的一致性对他们的决定影响最大;与儿童报告前后不一致时相比,他们认为儿童报告前后一致时对被告不利的证据更有力。此外,他们对证据力度的看法也影响了他们的建议。当不利于被告的证据更有力时,律师认为被告在审判中被定罪的可能性更大;因此,检察官不太愿意而辩护律师更愿意建议被告认罪:结论:与其他案件类似,在儿童性虐待案件中,证据力度和认为定罪的可能性促使律师决定向被告提出或建议认罪。儿童报告的一致性在预测对证据力度的看法方面起着重要作用。今后还需要进行研究,以确定儿童性虐待案件中的其他因素也会对律师的看法和认罪建议产生影响。(PsycInfo 数据库记录 (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Attorneys' experiences, perceptions, and plea recommendations in child sexual abuse cases.","authors":"Melanie B Fessinger, Bradley D McAuliff, Eliana Aronson, Kelly McWilliams","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000551","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000551","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>We examined attorneys' experiences, perceptions, and decisions regarding plea recommendations in child sexual cases.</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>We hypothesized that characteristics of the child (age, relationship to alleged perpetrator) and the report (timing of disclosure, consistency across reports) would affect attorneys' perceptions of evidence strength, likelihood of conviction, and plea recommendations.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We collected data from a national sample of actively practicing prosecutors (n = 217) and defense attorneys (n = 251) who had experience with child abuse cases. They averaged 18 years of experience practicing law, were slightly more likely to be men (53%) than women, and primarily identified as White, non-Hispanic (86%). In Part 1, attorneys answered general questions about their experiences in child sexual abuse cases. In Part 2, they reviewed materials from a hypothetical case that varied the child's age (5 years, 11 years), the child's relationship to the alleged perpetrator (familial, nonfamilial), the timing of the child's initial disclosure (1 week, 6 months), and the consistency of the child's report (inconsistent, consistent). They rated the evidence strength, estimated the likelihood of conviction, and assessed whether they would recommend that the defendant accept a plea offer or proceed to trial.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>In Part 1, attorneys reported that they often have access to police reports, information about the alleged perpetrator, and evidence from the child when making plea recommendations. They said that it was important to know about prior allegations against the alleged perpetrator or by the child when assessing their credibility. They reported that the length of the sentence, sex offender registration requirement, and possibility of time served guided their plea recommendations. In Part 2, the consistency of the child's report influenced their decisions the most; they rated the evidence against the defendant as stronger when the child was consistent across reports than when the child was inconsistent. Additionally, their perceptions of evidence strength drove their recommendations. When the evidence against the defendant was stronger, attorneys thought that the defendant was more likely to be convicted at trial; thus, prosecutors were less willing and defense attorneys were more willing to recommend a plea.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Similar to other cases, evidence strength and the perceived likelihood of conviction drive attorneys' decisions to offer or recommend a plea to a defendant in a child sexual abuse case. The consistency of the child's report plays a major role in predicting perceptions of evidence strength. Future research is needed to determine which other factors in child sexual abuse cases may also predict attorneys' perceptions and plea recommendations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"48 1","pages":"13-32"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140867867","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Assessing psychopathic traits with the MMPI-3: Findings from correctional, university, and community samples. 用 MMPI-3 评估精神病患者特质:来自教养所、大学和社区样本的研究结果。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000552
Dustin B Wygant, Martin Sellbom

Objective: The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) instruments have a long history with respect to the assessment of psychopathic personality traits. The most recent version, the MMPI-3, should be in a good position to continue this tradition, and the aim of the current research was to evaluate its scales for this purpose. We examined, on the basis of previous research, how well conceptually relevant MMPI-3 scales mapped onto dominant contemporary psychopathy models: the traditional three-factor model and triarchic psychopathy model.

Hypotheses: We hypothesized that MMPI-3 markers of internalizing would be negatively correlated with boldness, whereas broad and specific markers of externalizing proclivities would be associated with disinhibition and antisociality. We also hypothesized that egocentricity and callousness would be associated with MMPI-3 scales measuring various features of externalizing, interpersonal aggression/antagonism, and grandiosity.

Method: We used archival samples of male prison inmates (n = 452), community members with externalizing proclivities (n = 205), and university students (n = 645). These participants completed the Expanded Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale and the Triarchic Psychopathy Measure.

Results: Zero-order correlation analyses indicated support for many of our hypotheses across samples, with notable exceptions. Regression and dominance analyses yielded information about the most potent MMPI-3 predictors of each psychopathy domain, with consistency across the three samples. Boldness was associated with low scores on Emotional/Internalizing Dysfunction, Low Positive Emotions, Shyness, and Negative Emotionality/Neuroticism and high scores on Self-Importance and Dominance. For meanness and disinhibition, we found substantial overlap with MMPI-3 scales (e.g., Behavioral/Externalizing Dysfunction, Antisocial Behavior). Meanness was indicated by high Aggression, Cynicism, Aggressiveness, and Disaffiliativeness; disinhibition/antisociality was primarily marked by high Antisocial Behavior, Hypomanic Activation, Impulsivity, and Disconstraint; and Anger Proneness, Aggression, and Cynicism were secondary indicators.

Conclusions: These findings provide support for using the MMPI-3 in clinical assessments to corroborate other sources of information regarding psychopathy as well as generate hypotheses for further consideration. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:明尼苏达多相人格量表(MMPI)工具在评估变态人格特质方面有着悠久的历史。最新版本的明尼苏达多相人格问卷(MMPI-3)应该能够很好地继承这一传统,本研究的目的就是为此对其量表进行评估。在以往研究的基础上,我们考察了与概念相关的 MMPI-3 量表与当代主流变态心理学模型(传统的三因素模型和三元变态心理学模型)的映射程度:我们假设,MMPI-3 内化标记与大胆呈负相关,而外化倾向的广泛和特定标记则与抑制和反社会性相关。我们还假设,自我中心和冷酷无情会与测量外化、人际攻击/对抗和自大等各种特征的 MMPI-3 量表相关:我们使用了男性囚犯(452 人)、有外化倾向的社区成员(205 人)和大学生(645 人)的档案样本。这些参与者填写了扩展的莱文森自我报告心理变态量表和三元心理变态测量:零阶相关分析表明,我们的许多假设在不同样本中都得到了支持,但也有明显的例外。回归分析和优势分析得出了有关 MMPI-3 对每个心理变态领域最有效的预测因素的信息,这在三个样本中是一致的。大胆与情绪/内化功能障碍、低积极情绪、害羞和消极情绪/神经质的低分以及自我重要性和支配性的高分有关。在刻薄和抑制方面,我们发现与 MMPI-3 量表(如行为/外化功能障碍、反社会行为)有很大的重叠。高攻击性、愤世嫉俗、攻击性和不合群性是卑鄙的表现;高反社会行为、躁狂激活、冲动和拘谨是抑制/反社会性的主要标志;愤怒倾向、攻击性和愤世嫉俗是次要指标:这些研究结果支持在临床评估中使用 MMPI-3,以证实有关精神变态的其他信息来源,并提出供进一步考虑的假设。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Assessing psychopathic traits with the MMPI-3: Findings from correctional, university, and community samples.","authors":"Dustin B Wygant, Martin Sellbom","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000552","DOIUrl":"10.1037/lhb0000552","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) instruments have a long history with respect to the assessment of psychopathic personality traits. The most recent version, the MMPI-3, should be in a good position to continue this tradition, and the aim of the current research was to evaluate its scales for this purpose. We examined, on the basis of previous research, how well conceptually relevant MMPI-3 scales mapped onto dominant contemporary psychopathy models: the traditional three-factor model and triarchic psychopathy model.</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>We hypothesized that MMPI-3 markers of internalizing would be negatively correlated with boldness, whereas broad and specific markers of externalizing proclivities would be associated with disinhibition and antisociality. We also hypothesized that egocentricity and callousness would be associated with MMPI-3 scales measuring various features of externalizing, interpersonal aggression/antagonism, and grandiosity.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We used archival samples of male prison inmates (n = 452), community members with externalizing proclivities (n = 205), and university students (n = 645). These participants completed the Expanded Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale and the Triarchic Psychopathy Measure.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Zero-order correlation analyses indicated support for many of our hypotheses across samples, with notable exceptions. Regression and dominance analyses yielded information about the most potent MMPI-3 predictors of each psychopathy domain, with consistency across the three samples. Boldness was associated with low scores on Emotional/Internalizing Dysfunction, Low Positive Emotions, Shyness, and Negative Emotionality/Neuroticism and high scores on Self-Importance and Dominance. For meanness and disinhibition, we found substantial overlap with MMPI-3 scales (e.g., Behavioral/Externalizing Dysfunction, Antisocial Behavior). Meanness was indicated by high Aggression, Cynicism, Aggressiveness, and Disaffiliativeness; disinhibition/antisociality was primarily marked by high Antisocial Behavior, Hypomanic Activation, Impulsivity, and Disconstraint; and Anger Proneness, Aggression, and Cynicism were secondary indicators.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>These findings provide support for using the MMPI-3 in clinical assessments to corroborate other sources of information regarding psychopathy as well as generate hypotheses for further consideration. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"48 1","pages":"1-12"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140855662","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reexamining predictors of trial outcomes in New York State's sex offender civil management process. 重新审视纽约州性犯罪者民事管理程序中的审判结果预测因素。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-22 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000550
Nathan S Kemper, Marie L Reilly, Naomi J Freeman, Jeffrey C Sandler

Objective: In 2007, New York enacted the Sex Offender Management and Treatment Act, empowering the state to civilly manage individuals who have committed sexual offenses (respondents) and are deemed to have a mental abnormality (MA) that predisposes them to sexually recidivate after serving their criminal sentences. We sought to replicate and extend a previous study (Lu et al., 2015) to identify factors predicting legal decisions.

Hypotheses: We predicted, on the basis of previous research, that clinical information (e.g., diagnosis) as well as empirically supported risk factors (e.g., sexual deviance) would predict trial outcomes.

Method: We analyzed multiple pieces of demographic, criminogenic, and clinical data on three nested subsamples of respondents on the basis of the legal process: MA consent (n = 713), MA trial (n = 316), and disposition hearing (n = 643). The binary outcomes of interest were as follows: For the MA consent subsample, it was whether the respondent waived their MA trial; for the MA trial subsample, it was whether the respondent was found at trial to have an MA; and for the disposition hearing, it was whether the respondent was ordered to inpatient or outpatient civil management.

Results: The strongest predictor of waiving the trial was geographic location; respondents outside New York City and Long Island were more likely to waive their trials (ORs = 2.38-3.37). The strongest predictors of MA trial and disposition hearing outcomes were Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders diagnoses; pedophilia (ORs = 4.05-7.22) and sexual sadism (ORs = 2.68-7.03) diagnoses increased the likelihood of an MA finding and confinement order.

Conclusions: Judges and juries give significant weight to clinical information, particularly pedophilia diagnoses, when making civil management legal decisions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目 的 :2007 年,纽约州颁布了《性犯罪者管理与治疗法案》,授权州政府对犯有性犯罪(受访者)并被认为存在精神异常(MA)的个人进行民事管理,这种精神异常使他们在服刑期满后容易在性方面再次犯罪。我们试图复制并扩展之前的一项研究(Lu 等人,2015 年),以确定预测法律决定的因素:我们根据以往的研究预测,临床信息(如诊断)以及经验支持的风险因素(如性偏差)将预测审判结果:我们根据法律程序分析了三个嵌套子样本受访者的人口统计学、犯罪原因和临床数据:我们对三个嵌套子样本的人口、犯罪原因和临床数据进行了分析,这三个子样本的法律程序分别是:MA 同意(n = 713)、MA 审判(n = 316)和处置听证(n = 643)。相关的二元结果如下:对于精神病鉴定同意书子样本,是指受访者是否放弃精神病鉴定审判;对于精神病鉴定审判子样本,是指受访者是否在审判中被认定患有精神病鉴定;对于处置听证会,是指受访者是否被下令接受住院或门诊民事管理:放弃审判的最强预测因素是地理位置;纽约市和长岛以外的受访者更有可能放弃审判(ORs = 2.38-3.37)。精神障碍诊断与统计手册》中的精神障碍诊断是预测精神障碍审判和处置听证会结果的最有力因素;恋童癖(ORs = 4.05-7.22)和性虐待狂(ORs = 2.68-7.03)诊断增加了精神障碍鉴定结论和禁闭令的可能性:结论:法官和陪审团在做出民事管理法律裁决时,会非常重视临床信息,尤其是恋童癖诊断。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Reexamining predictors of trial outcomes in New York State's sex offender civil management process.","authors":"Nathan S Kemper, Marie L Reilly, Naomi J Freeman, Jeffrey C Sandler","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000550","DOIUrl":"10.1037/lhb0000550","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>In 2007, New York enacted the Sex Offender Management and Treatment Act, empowering the state to civilly manage individuals who have committed sexual offenses (respondents) and are deemed to have a mental abnormality (MA) that predisposes them to sexually recidivate after serving their criminal sentences. We sought to replicate and extend a previous study (Lu et al., 2015) to identify factors predicting legal decisions.</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>We predicted, on the basis of previous research, that clinical information (e.g., diagnosis) as well as empirically supported risk factors (e.g., sexual deviance) would predict trial outcomes.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We analyzed multiple pieces of demographic, criminogenic, and clinical data on three nested subsamples of respondents on the basis of the legal process: MA consent (<i>n</i> = 713), MA trial (<i>n</i> = 316), and disposition hearing (<i>n</i> = 643). The binary outcomes of interest were as follows: For the MA consent subsample, it was whether the respondent waived their MA trial; for the MA trial subsample, it was whether the respondent was found at trial to have an MA; and for the disposition hearing, it was whether the respondent was ordered to inpatient or outpatient civil management.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The strongest predictor of waiving the trial was geographic location; respondents outside New York City and Long Island were more likely to waive their trials (<i>OR</i>s = 2.38-3.37). The strongest predictors of MA trial and disposition hearing outcomes were <i>Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders</i> diagnoses; pedophilia (<i>OR</i>s = 4.05-7.22) and sexual sadism (<i>OR</i>s = 2.68-7.03) diagnoses increased the likelihood of an MA finding and confinement order.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Judges and juries give significant weight to clinical information, particularly pedophilia diagnoses, when making civil management legal decisions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"67-82"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139514042","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Advancing the Shift-of-Strategy approach: Shifting suspects' strategies in extended interviews. 推进策略转换方法:在扩展访谈中转变嫌疑人的策略。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000554
Lina Nyström, Timothy J Luke, Pär Anders Granhag, Aziz-Kaan Dönmez, Malin Ekelund, Pär D Stern

Objective: Interviewers often face the challenge of obtaining information from suspects who are willing to speak but are motivated to conceal incriminating information. The Shift-of-Strategy (SoS) approach is an interviewing technique designed to obtain new information from such suspects. This study provides a robust empirical test of the SoS approach using more complex crime events and longer interviews than previously tested as well as testing a new variation of the approach (SoS-Reinforcement) that included a strategic summary of the suspect's statement. We compared this new variation with a standard version of the approach (SoS-Standard) and an interviewing approach that involved no confrontation of discrepancies in the suspects' statements (Direct).

Hypotheses: We predicted that the two SoS versions would outperform the Direct condition in terms of participants' disclosure of previously unknown information. We also predicted that SoS-Reinforcement would outperform SoS-Standard. Finally, we expected that participants in the SoS conditions would not assess the interview or the interviewer more poorly than participants in Direct.

Method: A total of 300 participants completed an online mock crime procedure, and they were subsequently interviewed with one of the three interviewing techniques. Following the interview, participants provided assessments of their experiences being interviewed.

Results: Participants in both SoS-Standard (d = 0.49, 95% confidence interval [CI: 0.21, 0.78]) and SoS-Reinforcement (d = 0.59, 95% CI [0.30, 0.87]) disclosed more previously unknown information than participants in the Direct condition, but SoS-Reinforcement did not outperform SoS-Standard (d = 0.08, 95% CI [-0.20, 0.36]). Participants in SoS-Reinforcement assessed their experience more negatively than those in Direct. No such differences were observed in the remaining two-way comparisons.

Conclusions: The study provides support for the effectiveness of eliciting new information through the SoS approach and illuminates possible experiential downsides with being subjected to the SoS-Reinforcement approach. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:访谈者经常面临的挑战是,如何从愿意开口但有隐瞒罪证动机的疑犯口中获取信息。策略转换(SoS)方法是一种访谈技巧,旨在从这类嫌疑人那里获取新信息。本研究使用比以往更复杂的犯罪事件和更长的访谈时间,对策略转换法进行了有力的实证测试,并测试了策略转换法的新变体(策略转换-强化),其中包括对嫌疑人陈述的策略性总结。我们将这种新方法与标准版本的方法(SoS-标准)和不涉及嫌疑人陈述差异的面谈方法(直接)进行了比较:我们预测,就参与者披露先前未知信息的情况而言,两个 SoS 版本的效果将优于 "直接 "条件。我们还预测,"强化 "SoS 将优于 "标准 "SoS。最后,我们预计,SoS 条件下的参与者对访谈或访谈者的评价不会比直接条件下的参与者更差:共有 300 名参与者完成了在线模拟犯罪程序,随后他们使用三种面试技巧中的一种接受了面试。访谈结束后,参与者对自己的访谈经历进行了评估:结果:SoS-标准(d = 0.49,95% 置信区间[CI: 0.21, 0.78])和 SoS-强化(d = 0.59,95% 置信区间[0.30, 0.87])条件下的参与者比直接条件下的参与者披露了更多之前未知的信息,但 SoS-强化条件下的参与者披露的信息并没有超过 SoS-标准(d = 0.08,95% 置信区间[-0.20, 0.36])。SoS-Reinforcement参与实验者对其体验的评价比Direct参与实验者更消极。在其余的双向比较中没有观察到这种差异:本研究为通过 SoS 方法激发新信息的有效性提供了支持,并揭示了采用 SoS-强化方法可能带来的体验上的负面影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Advancing the Shift-of-Strategy approach: Shifting suspects' strategies in extended interviews.","authors":"Lina Nyström, Timothy J Luke, Pär Anders Granhag, Aziz-Kaan Dönmez, Malin Ekelund, Pär D Stern","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000554","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000554","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Interviewers often face the challenge of obtaining information from suspects who are willing to speak but are motivated to conceal incriminating information. The Shift-of-Strategy (SoS) approach is an interviewing technique designed to obtain new information from such suspects. This study provides a robust empirical test of the SoS approach using more complex crime events and longer interviews than previously tested as well as testing a new variation of the approach (SoS-Reinforcement) that included a strategic summary of the suspect's statement. We compared this new variation with a standard version of the approach (SoS-Standard) and an interviewing approach that involved no confrontation of discrepancies in the suspects' statements (Direct).</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>We predicted that the two SoS versions would outperform the Direct condition in terms of participants' disclosure of previously unknown information. We also predicted that SoS-Reinforcement would outperform SoS-Standard. Finally, we expected that participants in the SoS conditions would not assess the interview or the interviewer more poorly than participants in Direct.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>A total of 300 participants completed an online mock crime procedure, and they were subsequently interviewed with one of the three interviewing techniques. Following the interview, participants provided assessments of their experiences being interviewed.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Participants in both SoS-Standard (d = 0.49, 95% confidence interval [CI: 0.21, 0.78]) and SoS-Reinforcement (d = 0.59, 95% CI [0.30, 0.87]) disclosed more previously unknown information than participants in the Direct condition, but SoS-Reinforcement did not outperform SoS-Standard (d = 0.08, 95% CI [-0.20, 0.36]). Participants in SoS-Reinforcement assessed their experience more negatively than those in Direct. No such differences were observed in the remaining two-way comparisons.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The study provides support for the effectiveness of eliciting new information through the SoS approach and illuminates possible experiential downsides with being subjected to the SoS-Reinforcement approach. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"48 1","pages":"50-66"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140855661","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do inconclusive forensic decisions disadvantage the innocent? 不确定的法医裁决是否会使无辜者处于不利地位?
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000553
Stephanie Madon, Kayla A Burd, Max Guyll

Objective: Two experiments examined the potential for inconclusive forensic decisions to disadvantage the innocent.

Hypotheses: Both experiments tested the hypothesis that inconclusive decisions produce more incriminating legal judgments than do clearly exculpatory forensic decisions. Experiment 2 also examined whether this hypothesized effect conformed to a confirmation bias, a communication error, or perceptual accuracy.

Method: In Experiment 1 (N = 492), a forensic expert testified that physical evidence recovered from a crime scene either matched or did not match a suspect's evidence or produced an inconclusive result. In Experiment 2 (N = 1,002), a forensic expert testified that physical evidence recovered from a crime scene either matched or did not match a suspect's evidence, produced an inconclusive result, or was unsuitable for analysis. A fifth condition omitted the forensic evidence and expert testimony.

Results: The inconclusive decision produced less incriminating legal judgments than did the match forensic decision (|d|average = 0.96), more incriminating legal judgments than did the no-match forensic decision (|d|average = 0.62), and equivalent legal judgments to the unsuitable decision (|d|average = 0.12) and to legal judgments made in the absence of forensic evidence (|d|average = 0.07). These results suggest that participants interpreted the inconclusive decision to be forensically neutral, which is consistent with a communication error.

Conclusion: The findings provide preliminary support for the idea that inconclusive decisions can put the innocent at risk of wrongful conviction by depriving them of a clearly exculpatory forensic decision. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:两个实验研究了法医鉴定的不确定裁决可能对无辜者不利:两个实验都检验了这样一个假设,即与明显可以开脱罪责的法医裁决相比,不确定的裁决会产生更多有罪的法律判断。实验 2 还检验了这一假设效应是否与确认偏差、沟通错误或知觉准确性相一致:在实验 1(N = 492)中,法医专家证明从犯罪现场找到的物证与嫌疑人的证据相符或不相符,或得出不确定的结果。在实验 2(N = 1 002)中,法医专家证明从犯罪现场找到的物证与嫌疑人的物证相符或不相符,或得出不确定的结果,或不适合进行分析。第五种情况省略了法医证据和专家证词:结果:与法医证据匹配的结果相比,不确定的结果产生的有罪法律判断较少(|d|平均值=0.96),与法医证据不匹配的结果相比,不确定的结果产生的有罪法律判断较多(|d|平均值=0.62),与不适合的结果(|d|平均值=0.12)以及在没有法医证据的情况下产生的法律判断(|d|平均值=0.07)相当。这些结果表明,被试将不确定的决定解释为法医中性的,这与沟通错误是一致的:结论:研究结果初步支持了这样一种观点,即不确定的裁决可能会使无辜者因无法获得明确的开脱罪责的法医裁决而面临被错误定罪的风险。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Do inconclusive forensic decisions disadvantage the innocent?","authors":"Stephanie Madon, Kayla A Burd, Max Guyll","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000553","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000553","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Two experiments examined the potential for inconclusive forensic decisions to disadvantage the innocent.</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>Both experiments tested the hypothesis that inconclusive decisions produce more incriminating legal judgments than do clearly exculpatory forensic decisions. Experiment 2 also examined whether this hypothesized effect conformed to a confirmation bias, a communication error, or perceptual accuracy.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>In Experiment 1 (N = 492), a forensic expert testified that physical evidence recovered from a crime scene either matched or did not match a suspect's evidence or produced an inconclusive result. In Experiment 2 (N = 1,002), a forensic expert testified that physical evidence recovered from a crime scene either matched or did not match a suspect's evidence, produced an inconclusive result, or was unsuitable for analysis. A fifth condition omitted the forensic evidence and expert testimony.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The inconclusive decision produced less incriminating legal judgments than did the match forensic decision (|d|<sub>average</sub> = 0.96), more incriminating legal judgments than did the no-match forensic decision (|d|<sub>average</sub> = 0.62), and equivalent legal judgments to the unsuitable decision (|d|<sub>average</sub> = 0.12) and to legal judgments made in the absence of forensic evidence (|d|<sub>average</sub> = 0.07). These results suggest that participants interpreted the inconclusive decision to be forensically neutral, which is consistent with a communication error.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The findings provide preliminary support for the idea that inconclusive decisions can put the innocent at risk of wrongful conviction by depriving them of a clearly exculpatory forensic decision. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"48 1","pages":"33-49"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140870253","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An offer you cannot refuse: Plea offer size affects innocent but not guilty defendants' perceptions of voluntariness. 你无法拒绝的提议:认罪提议的大小会影响无罪而非有罪被告人对自愿性的认知。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2023-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000548
Melanie B Fessinger, Margaret Bull Kovera

Objective: We examined whether various plea outcomes-including sentence reduction size (smaller, larger), type (traditional guilty plea, Alford plea), and frame (plea discount, trial penalty)-differentially affected innocent and guilty defendants' perceptions of the voluntariness of their guilty pleas.

Hypotheses: We hypothesized (1) guilty defendants would rate guilty pleas as more voluntary than would innocent defendants; (2) defendants would rate larger sentence reductions either as more voluntary than smaller sentence reductions because they feel more fair or as less voluntary because they feel harder to reject; (3) defendants would rate guilty pleas as more voluntary when the plea offer was framed as a discount compared with a penalty; (4) penalty framing would differentially affect defendants offered large versus small sentence reductions; and (5) Alford pleas would differentially affect guilty versus innocent defendants.

Method: Adults from Qualtrics Research Panels (N = 1,518; Mage = 59.22 years; 52% male; 83% White, non-Hispanic) played the role of a defendant in a simulated plea decision-making process. They were either innocent or guilty of the accusation. The prosecutor offered them a plea deal that varied in sentence reduction size (smaller, versus larger), type (traditional versus Alford plea), and frame (plea discount versus trial penalty). Participants then decided how to plead and rated the voluntariness of the decision-making process.

Results: Plea outcomes affected innocent and guilty defendants in slightly different ways. Innocent and guilty defendants were less likely to plead guilty when the plea offer had a smaller compared with a larger sentence reduction. However, innocent defendants were less likely to plead guilty overall, required more prompting from their defense attorney to plead guilty, and rated the plea decision-making process as less voluntary than did guilty defendants. Innocent defendants also rated the plea decision-making process as less voluntary when offered a smaller compared with larger sentence reduction and when they were offered an Alford plea compared with a traditional guilty plea. Framing the plea offer as a discount or a penalty did not affect defendants' perceptions of voluntariness.

Conclusion: Variations in plea outcomes affect defendants' perceptions of voluntariness. Moreover, at least some courts' definitions of voluntariness do not align with how laypeople-and thus, possible defendants-view the same construct. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:我们研究了各种认罪结果--包括减刑幅度(较小、较大)、类型(传统认罪、阿尔弗德认罪)和框架(认罪折扣、审判处罚)--是否会不同程度地影响无辜被告和有罪被告对认罪自愿性的看法:我们假设:(1) 与无罪被告相比,有罪被告会认为认罪更自愿;(2) 与较小的减刑相比,被告会认为较大的减刑更自愿,因为他们感觉更公平,或者认为较小的减刑更不自愿,因为他们感觉更难拒绝;(3) 与刑罚相比,如果辩诉提议被设定为折扣,被告人会认为认罪更自愿;(4) 刑罚设定对减刑幅度大的被告人和减刑幅度小的被告人的影响不同;(5) Alford 辩诉对有罪被告人和无罪被告人的影响不同。研究方法:来自 Qualtrics 研究小组的成年人(人数 = 1,518;年龄 = 59.22 岁;52% 为男性;83% 为白人,非西班牙裔)在模拟认罪求情决策过程中扮演被告。他们要么是无辜的,要么是有罪的。检察官向他们提出不同减刑幅度(较小与较大)、类型(传统认罪与阿尔弗德认罪)和框架(认罪折扣与审判处罚)的认罪协议。然后,参与者决定如何认罪,并对决策过程的自愿性进行评分:认罪结果对无罪被告和有罪被告的影响略有不同。当辩诉提议的减刑幅度小于减刑幅度大时,无辜被告和有罪被告认罪的可能性都较小。然而,与有罪被告人相比,无罪被告人总体上认罪的可能性较低,需要辩护律师更多的提示才能认罪,并且对认罪决定过程的自愿性评价较低。与传统的认罪相比,无罪被告在获得较小减刑幅度时,与获得较大减刑幅度时相比,以及在获得阿尔弗德认罪协议时,对认罪求情决策过程的自愿性评价也较低。将认罪协议描述为减刑或处罚并不影响被告对自愿性的看法:抗辩结果的不同会影响被告对自愿性的认知。此外,至少有些法院对自愿性的定义与普通人--也就是可能的被告人--对同一概念的看法并不一致。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"An offer you cannot refuse: Plea offer size affects innocent but not guilty defendants' perceptions of voluntariness.","authors":"Melanie B Fessinger, Margaret Bull Kovera","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000548","DOIUrl":"10.1037/lhb0000548","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>We examined whether various plea outcomes-including sentence reduction size (smaller, larger), type (traditional guilty plea, Alford plea), and frame (plea discount, trial penalty)-differentially affected innocent and guilty defendants' perceptions of the voluntariness of their guilty pleas.</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>We hypothesized (1) guilty defendants would rate guilty pleas as more voluntary than would innocent defendants; (2) defendants would rate larger sentence reductions either as more voluntary than smaller sentence reductions because they feel more fair or as less voluntary because they feel harder to reject; (3) defendants would rate guilty pleas as more voluntary when the plea offer was framed as a discount compared with a penalty; (4) penalty framing would differentially affect defendants offered large versus small sentence reductions; and (5) Alford pleas would differentially affect guilty versus innocent defendants.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Adults from Qualtrics Research Panels (N = 1,518; M<sub>age</sub> = 59.22 years; 52% male; 83% White, non-Hispanic) played the role of a defendant in a simulated plea decision-making process. They were either innocent or guilty of the accusation. The prosecutor offered them a plea deal that varied in sentence reduction size (smaller, versus larger), type (traditional versus Alford plea), and frame (plea discount versus trial penalty). Participants then decided how to plead and rated the voluntariness of the decision-making process.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Plea outcomes affected innocent and guilty defendants in slightly different ways. Innocent and guilty defendants were less likely to plead guilty when the plea offer had a smaller compared with a larger sentence reduction. However, innocent defendants were less likely to plead guilty overall, required more prompting from their defense attorney to plead guilty, and rated the plea decision-making process as less voluntary than did guilty defendants. Innocent defendants also rated the plea decision-making process as less voluntary when offered a smaller compared with larger sentence reduction and when they were offered an Alford plea compared with a traditional guilty plea. Framing the plea offer as a discount or a penalty did not affect defendants' perceptions of voluntariness.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Variations in plea outcomes affect defendants' perceptions of voluntariness. Moreover, at least some courts' definitions of voluntariness do not align with how laypeople-and thus, possible defendants-view the same construct. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"47 6","pages":"619-633"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832285","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
More than race? Intragroup differences by gender and age in perceptions of police among street-identified Black men and women. 不仅仅是种族问题?街头黑人男女对警察的看法在性别和年龄上的组内差异。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2023-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000544
Brooklynn K Hitchens, Jeaneé C Miller, Yasser Arafat Payne, Ivan Y Sun, Isabella Castillo

Objective: Whereas studies have documented racial differences in attitudes toward police between White and Black Americans, relatively little is known about the intragroup, gender-based variations among urban Black residents involved in criminal activity (i.e., street-identified men and women).

Hypotheses: We hypothesized Black women would be more likely to believe in police legitimacy and positive intent than men (Hypothesis 1), especially among the younger segment of the sample (Hypothesis 2). We also expected this relationship to be moderated by contact with police (Hypothesis 3) and experiences with victimization (Hypothesis 4).

Method: Using survey data, this Street Participatory Action Research project examined the direct and interactional relationships between gender, age, involuntary police contact, personal victimization, and participants' perceptions of police legitimacy and positive intent. Participants included 515 street-identified Black men (40.4%; n = 208) and women (59.6%; n = 307), ages 18-35 years, from two high-crime neighborhoods in Wilmington, Delaware.

Results: Women had significantly higher perceptions of police legitimacy than did men (Hypothesis 1). While older participants tended to have lower perceptions that the police behave with positive intent, age did not moderate the relationship between gender and perceptions of police (Hypothesis 2). The relationship between gender and perceptions of positive police intent was moderated by involuntary police contact (Hypothesis 3) and experiences of victimization (Hypothesis 4).

Conclusions: Contrary to existing literature, prior involuntary police contact mattered more for street-identified Black women than men in predicting perceptions of police. Experiences of victimization were also more impactful for these perceptions for street-identified Black women than men. Men's perceptions of positive police intent were consistent, regardless of the frequency of police contact, whereas women's favorable perceptions declined with more police contact and victimization experiences, and they eventually became more critical of the police than their male counterparts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:尽管已有研究记录了美国白人和黑人在对待警察态度上的种族差异,但对于参与犯罪活动的城市黑人居民(即街头男性和女性)在群体内部、基于性别的差异却知之甚少:我们假设黑人女性比男性更有可能相信警察的合法性和积极意图(假设 1),尤其是在样本中的年轻群体中(假设 2)。我们还预计这种关系会受到与警察接触(假设 3)和受害经历(假设 4)的调节:本街头参与行动研究项目利用调查数据,研究了性别、年龄、非自愿与警察接触、个人受害经历以及参与者对警察合法性和积极意图的看法之间的直接关系和互动关系。参与者包括来自特拉华州威尔明顿两个高犯罪率社区的 515 名街头黑人男性(40.4%;n = 208)和女性(59.6%;n = 307),年龄在 18-35 岁之间:女性对警察合法性的看法明显高于男性(假设 1)。虽然年龄较大的参与者对警察行为的积极意图的感知往往较低,但年龄并不影响性别与对警察的感知之间的关系(假设 2)。非自愿接触警察(假设 3)和受害经历(假设 4)调节了性别与对警察积极意图的看法之间的关系:与现有文献相反,在预测对警察的看法时,曾非自愿接触过警察的街头黑人女性比男性更重要。与男性相比,受害经历对街头黑人女性的影响也更大。无论与警察接触的频率如何,男性对警察积极意图的看法都是一致的,而女性对警察的好感则随着与警察接触的增多和受害经历的增加而下降,最终她们对警察的批评比男性更多。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"More than race? Intragroup differences by gender and age in perceptions of police among street-identified Black men and women.","authors":"Brooklynn K Hitchens, Jeaneé C Miller, Yasser Arafat Payne, Ivan Y Sun, Isabella Castillo","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000544","DOIUrl":"10.1037/lhb0000544","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Whereas studies have documented racial differences in attitudes toward police between White and Black Americans, relatively little is known about the intragroup, gender-based variations among urban Black residents involved in criminal activity (i.e., street-identified men and women).</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>We hypothesized Black women would be more likely to believe in police legitimacy and positive intent than men (Hypothesis 1), especially among the younger segment of the sample (Hypothesis 2). We also expected this relationship to be moderated by contact with police (Hypothesis 3) and experiences with victimization (Hypothesis 4).</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Using survey data, this Street Participatory Action Research project examined the direct and interactional relationships between gender, age, involuntary police contact, personal victimization, and participants' perceptions of police legitimacy and positive intent. Participants included 515 street-identified Black men (40.4%; n = 208) and women (59.6%; n = 307), ages 18-35 years, from two high-crime neighborhoods in Wilmington, Delaware.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Women had significantly higher perceptions of police legitimacy than did men (Hypothesis 1). While older participants tended to have lower perceptions that the police behave with positive intent, age did not moderate the relationship between gender and perceptions of police (Hypothesis 2). The relationship between gender and perceptions of positive police intent was moderated by involuntary police contact (Hypothesis 3) and experiences of victimization (Hypothesis 4).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Contrary to existing literature, prior involuntary police contact mattered more for street-identified Black women than men in predicting perceptions of police. Experiences of victimization were also more impactful for these perceptions for street-identified Black women than men. Men's perceptions of positive police intent were consistent, regardless of the frequency of police contact, whereas women's favorable perceptions declined with more police contact and victimization experiences, and they eventually became more critical of the police than their male counterparts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"47 6","pages":"634-653"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832287","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Universal mandatory reporting policies show null effects in a statewide college sample. 在全州高校样本中,普遍强制报告政策显示效果为零。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 LAW Pub Date : 2023-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000546
Tara N Richards, Kathryn J Holland, Allison E Cipriano, Alyssa Nystrom

Objective: It is widely assumed that universal mandatory reporting policies (MRPs) for sexual misconduct are important for campus safety, but there is little evidence to support these assumptions.

Hypotheses: Given the exploratory nature of this research, no formal hypotheses were tested. We did not expect universal MRPs to be significantly associated with increased reporting or postreporting outcomes.

Method: Data on MRPs and sexual misconduct reporting in annual security reports and to Title IX coordinators at institutions of higher education in New York (N = 188) were used to examine the prevalence of universal MRPs as well as the relationship between MRPs and reporting and postreporting outcomes.

Results: Descriptives showed that 44% of institutions of higher education have a universal MRP. Multivariate linear regression models indicated that universal MRPs were not significantly related to reporting in annual security reports; reports to Title IX coordinators, campus police, campus safety or security officers; or rates of referrals to additional services, no-contact orders, access to the judicial conduct process for sexual misconduct, or findings of student responsibility for sexual misconduct.

Conclusions: Our findings raise concerns about the widespread implementation of MRPs and highlight the need for future research on their impact on student-survivor reporting and access to remedies and resources. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

目的人们普遍认为,针对性不端行为的普遍强制报告政策(MRPs)对校园安全非常重要,但几乎没有证据支持这些假设:鉴于本研究的探索性质,我们没有测试正式的假设。我们并不期望普遍的 MRPs 会与更多的报告或报告后的结果有明显关联:方法:使用纽约高等教育机构(N = 188)年度安全报告中的 MRPs 和性不端行为报告数据以及向 Title IX 协调员报告的数据,研究普遍 MRPs 的普遍性以及 MRPs 与报告和报告后结果之间的关系:结果:描述性数据显示,44% 的高等院校具有普遍的 MRP。多变量线性回归模型表明,普遍的 MRP 与年度安全报告中的报告、向 Title IX 协调员、校警、校园安全或保安人员的报告、转介到额外服务的比率、禁止接触令、进入性不端行为司法程序或学生对性不端行为的责任认定没有明显关系:我们的研究结果引起了人们对广泛实施 MRPs 的担忧,并强调了未来研究 MRPs 对学生-幸存者报告以及获得补救和资源的影响的必要性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Universal mandatory reporting policies show null effects in a statewide college sample.","authors":"Tara N Richards, Kathryn J Holland, Allison E Cipriano, Alyssa Nystrom","doi":"10.1037/lhb0000546","DOIUrl":"10.1037/lhb0000546","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>It is widely assumed that universal mandatory reporting policies (MRPs) for sexual misconduct are important for campus safety, but there is little evidence to support these assumptions.</p><p><strong>Hypotheses: </strong>Given the exploratory nature of this research, no formal hypotheses were tested. We did not expect universal MRPs to be significantly associated with increased reporting or postreporting outcomes.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Data on MRPs and sexual misconduct reporting in annual security reports and to Title IX coordinators at institutions of higher education in New York (N = 188) were used to examine the prevalence of universal MRPs as well as the relationship between MRPs and reporting and postreporting outcomes.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Descriptives showed that 44% of institutions of higher education have a universal MRP. Multivariate linear regression models indicated that universal MRPs were not significantly related to reporting in annual security reports; reports to Title IX coordinators, campus police, campus safety or security officers; or rates of referrals to additional services, no-contact orders, access to the judicial conduct process for sexual misconduct, or findings of student responsibility for sexual misconduct.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Our findings raise concerns about the widespread implementation of MRPs and highlight the need for future research on their impact on student-survivor reporting and access to remedies and resources. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48230,"journal":{"name":"Law and Human Behavior","volume":"47 6","pages":"686-699"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832288","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Law and Human Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1