首页 > 最新文献

Personality and Social Psychology Review最新文献

英文 中文
Insight in the Conspiracist's Mind. 阴谋家头脑中的洞察力。
IF 7.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-30 DOI: 10.1177/10888683231203145
Sander Van de Cruys, Jo Bervoets, Stephen Gadsby, David Gijbels, Karolien Poels

Academic AbstractThe motto of the conspiracist, "Do your own research," may seem ludicrous to scientists. Indeed, it is often dismissed as a mere rhetorical device that conspiracists use to give themselves the semblance of science. In this perspective paper, we explore the information-seeking activities ("research") that conspiracists do engage in. Drawing on the experimental psychology of aha experiences, we explain how these activities, as well as the epistemic experiences that precede (curiosity) or follow (insight or "aha" experiences) them, may play a crucial role in the appeal and development of conspiracy beliefs. Aha moments have properties that can be exploited by conspiracy theories, such as the potential for false but seemingly grounded conclusions. Finally, we hypothesize that the need for autonomous epistemic agency and discovery is universal but increases as people experience more uncertainty and/or feel epistemically excluded in society, hence linking it to existing literature on explaining conspiracy theories.

阴谋论者的座右铭“做你自己的研究”在科学家看来可能很可笑。事实上,它经常被认为是阴谋论者用来给自己披上科学外衣的一种修辞手段。在这篇前瞻性的论文中,我们探讨了阴谋论者确实参与的信息寻求活动(“研究”)。利用aha体验的实验心理学,我们解释了这些活动,以及在它们之前(好奇心)或之后(洞察力或“aha”体验)的认知体验,如何在阴谋信念的吸引力和发展中发挥关键作用。Aha时刻具有阴谋论可以利用的特性,例如可能得出虚假但似乎有根据的结论。最后,我们假设,对自主认知能动性和发现的需求是普遍的,但随着人们在社会中经历更多的不确定性和/或感到被认知排斥,这种需求会增加,因此将其与现有的解释阴谋论的文献联系起来。
{"title":"Insight in the Conspiracist's Mind.","authors":"Sander Van de Cruys, Jo Bervoets, Stephen Gadsby, David Gijbels, Karolien Poels","doi":"10.1177/10888683231203145","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10888683231203145","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Academic AbstractThe motto of the conspiracist, \"Do your own research,\" may seem ludicrous to scientists. Indeed, it is often dismissed as a mere rhetorical device that conspiracists use to give themselves the semblance of science. In this perspective paper, we explore the information-seeking activities (\"research\") that conspiracists <i>do</i> engage in. Drawing on the experimental psychology of aha experiences, we explain how these activities, as well as the epistemic experiences that precede (curiosity) or follow (insight or \"aha\" experiences) them, may play a crucial role in the appeal and development of conspiracy beliefs. Aha moments have properties that can be exploited by conspiracy theories, such as the potential for false but seemingly grounded conclusions. Finally, we hypothesize that the need for autonomous epistemic agency and discovery is universal but increases as people experience more uncertainty and/or feel epistemically excluded in society, hence linking it to existing literature on explaining conspiracy theories.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":7.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41155258","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On Personality Measures and Their Data: A Classification of Measurement Approaches and Their Recommended Uses. 关于人格测量及其数据:测量方法分类及其建议用途》。
IF 7.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-05 DOI: 10.1177/10888683231222519
John D Mayer, Victoria M Bryan

We employ a new approach for classifying methods of personality measurement such as self-judgment, mental ability, and lifespace measures and the data they produce. We divide these measures into two fundamental groups: personal-source data, which arise from the target person's own reports, and external-source data, which derive from the areas surrounding the person. These two broad classes are then further divided according to what they target and the response processes that produce them. We use the model to organize roughly a dozen kinds of data currently employed in the field. With this classification system in hand, we describe how much we might expect two types of measures of the same attribute to converge-and explain why methods often yield somewhat different results. Given that each measurement method has its own strengths and weaknesses, we examine the pros and cons of selecting a given type of measure to assess a specific area of personality.

我们采用了一种新的方法来对自我判断、心智能力和生命空间测量等人格测量方法及其产生的数据进行分类。我们将这些测量方法分为两个基本组别:个人来源数据和外部来源数据,前者来源于目标人物自己的报告,后者来源于目标人物周围的区域。这两大类数据又根据其目标和产生这些数据的反应过程进一步划分。我们使用该模型整理了该领域目前使用的大约十几种数据。有了这个分类系统,我们就可以描述对同一属性的两种测量方法的趋同程度--并解释为什么这些方法经常会产生不同的结果。鉴于每种测量方法都有其自身的优缺点,我们将探讨选择特定类型的测量方法来评估特定人格领域的利弊。
{"title":"On Personality Measures and Their Data: A Classification of Measurement Approaches and Their Recommended Uses.","authors":"John D Mayer, Victoria M Bryan","doi":"10.1177/10888683231222519","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10888683231222519","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We employ a new approach for classifying methods of personality measurement such as <i>self-judgment, mental ability</i>, and <i>lifespace</i> measures and the data they produce. We divide these measures into two fundamental groups: <i>personal-source data</i>, which arise from the target person's own reports, and <i>external-source data</i>, which derive from the areas surrounding the person. These two broad classes are then further divided according to what they target and the response processes that produce them. We use the model to organize roughly a dozen kinds of data currently employed in the field. With this classification system in hand, we describe how much we might expect two types of measures of the same attribute to converge-and explain why methods often yield somewhat different results. Given that each measurement method has its own strengths and weaknesses, we examine the pros and cons of selecting a given type of measure to assess a specific area of personality.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":7.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139693171","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Power to Detect What? Considerations for Planning and Evaluating Sample Size. 检测什么的能力?规划和评估样本大小的注意事项。
IF 7.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-12 DOI: 10.1177/10888683241228328
Roger Giner-Sorolla, Amanda K Montoya, Alan Reifman, Tom Carpenter, Neil A Lewis, Christopher L Aberson, Dries H Bostyn, Beverly G Conrique, Brandon W Ng, Alexander M Schoemann, Courtney Soderberg

Academic abstract: In the wake of the replication crisis, social and personality psychologists have increased attention to power analysis and the adequacy of sample sizes. In this article, we analyze current controversies in this area, including choosing effect sizes, why and whether power analyses should be conducted on already-collected data, how to mitigate the negative effects of sample size criteria on specific kinds of research, and which power criterion to use. For novel research questions, we advocate that researchers base sample sizes on effects that are likely to be cost-effective for other people to implement (in applied settings) or to study (in basic research settings), given the limitations of interest-based minimums or field-wide effect sizes. We discuss two alternatives to power analysis, precision analysis and sequential analysis, and end with recommendations for improving the practices of researchers, reviewers, and journal editors in social-personality psychology.

Public abstract: Recently, social-personality psychology has been criticized for basing some of its conclusions on studies with low numbers of participants. As a result, power analysis, a mathematical way to ensure that a study has enough participants to reliably "detect" a given size of psychological effect, has become popular. This article describes power analysis and discusses some controversies about it, including how researchers should derive assumptions about effect size, and how the requirements of power analysis can be applied without harming research on hard-to-reach and marginalized communities. For novel research questions, we advocate that researchers base sample sizes on effects that are likely to be cost-effective for other people to implement (in applied settings) or to study (in basic research settings). We discuss two alternatives to power analysis, precision analysis and sequential analysis, and end with recommendations for improving the practices of researchers, reviewers, and journal editors in social-personality psychology.

学术摘要:在复制危机之后,社会和人格心理学家们更加关注效应分析和样本大小的适当性。在这篇文章中,我们分析了这一领域目前存在的争议,包括效应大小的选择、为什么以及是否应该对已收集的数据进行功率分析、如何减轻样本大小标准对特定类型研究的负面影响,以及使用哪种功率标准。对于新颖的研究问题,考虑到基于兴趣的最小值或全领域效应大小的局限性,我们主张研究人员将样本大小建立在对其他人来说实施(在应用环境中)或研究(在基础研究环境中)可能具有成本效益的效应之上。我们讨论了功率分析的两种替代方法--精确分析和序列分析,最后提出了改进社会人格心理学研究人员、审稿人和期刊编辑实践的建议。公众号摘要:最近,社会人格心理学因其某些结论建立在参与人数较少的研究基础上而受到批评。因此,功率分析--一种确保研究有足够的参与者以可靠地 "检测 "出一定大小的心理效应的数学方法--开始流行起来。本文介绍了动力分析,并讨论了一些相关争议,包括研究人员应如何推导效应大小的假设,以及如何应用动力分析的要求而不损害对难以接触和边缘化群体的研究。对于新颖的研究问题,我们主张研究人员将样本大小建立在对其他人来说实施(在应用环境中)或研究(在基础研究环境中)可能具有成本效益的效果之上。我们讨论了功率分析的两种替代方法--精确分析和序列分析,最后提出了改进社会个性心理学研究人员、审稿人和期刊编辑工作的建议。
{"title":"Power to Detect What? Considerations for Planning and Evaluating Sample Size.","authors":"Roger Giner-Sorolla, Amanda K Montoya, Alan Reifman, Tom Carpenter, Neil A Lewis, Christopher L Aberson, Dries H Bostyn, Beverly G Conrique, Brandon W Ng, Alexander M Schoemann, Courtney Soderberg","doi":"10.1177/10888683241228328","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10888683241228328","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Academic abstract: </strong>In the wake of the replication crisis, social and personality psychologists have increased attention to power analysis and the adequacy of sample sizes. In this article, we analyze current controversies in this area, including choosing effect sizes, why and whether power analyses should be conducted on already-collected data, how to mitigate the negative effects of sample size criteria on specific kinds of research, and which power criterion to use. For novel research questions, we advocate that researchers base sample sizes on effects that are likely to be cost-effective for other people to implement (in applied settings) or to study (in basic research settings), given the limitations of interest-based minimums or field-wide effect sizes. We discuss two alternatives to power analysis, precision analysis and sequential analysis, and end with recommendations for improving the practices of researchers, reviewers, and journal editors in social-personality psychology.</p><p><strong>Public abstract: </strong>Recently, social-personality psychology has been criticized for basing some of its conclusions on studies with low numbers of participants. As a result, power analysis, a mathematical way to ensure that a study has enough participants to reliably \"detect\" a given size of psychological effect, has become popular. This article describes power analysis and discusses some controversies about it, including how researchers should derive assumptions about effect size, and how the requirements of power analysis can be applied without harming research on hard-to-reach and marginalized communities. For novel research questions, we advocate that researchers base sample sizes on effects that are likely to be cost-effective for other people to implement (in applied settings) or to study (in basic research settings). We discuss two alternatives to power analysis, precision analysis and sequential analysis, and end with recommendations for improving the practices of researchers, reviewers, and journal editors in social-personality psychology.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":7.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11193916/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139724527","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Connecting to Community: A Social Identity Approach to Neighborhood Mental Health. 连接社区:邻里心理健康的社会认同方法。
IF 7.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-26 DOI: 10.1177/10888683231216136
S Alexander Haslam, Polly Fong, Catherine Haslam, Tegan Cruwys

Academic abstract: Integrative theorizing is needed to advance our understanding of the relationship between where a person lives and their mental health. To this end, we introduce a social identity model that provides an integrated explanation of the ways in which social-psychological processes mediate and moderate the links between neighborhood and mental health. In developing this model, we first review existing models that are derived primarily from a resource-availability perspective informed by research in social epidemiology, health geography, and urban sociology. Building on these, the social identity model implicates neighborhood identification in four key pathways between residents' local environment and their mental health. We review a wealth of recent research that supports this model and which speaks to its capacity to integrate and extend insights from established models. We also explore the implications of the social identity approach for policy and intervention.

Public abstract: We need to understand the connection between where people live and their mental health better than we do. This article helps us do this by presenting an integrated model of the way that social and psychological factors affect the relationship between someone's neighborhood and their mental health. This model builds on insights from social epidemiology, health geography, and urban sociology. Its distinct and novel contribution is to point to the importance of four pathways through which neighborhood identification shapes residents' mental health. A large body of recent research supports this model and highlights its potential to integrate and expand upon existing theories. We also discuss how our model can inform policies and interventions that seek to improve mental health outcomes in communities.

学术摘要:我们需要综合理论来加深对居住地与心理健康之间关系的理解。为此,我们引入了一个社会认同模型,该模型综合解释了社会心理过程如何调解和缓和邻里关系与心理健康之间的联系。在建立这一模型的过程中,我们首先回顾了现有的模型,这些模型主要从资源可得性的角度出发,参考了社会流行病学、健康地理学和城市社会学的研究成果。在此基础上,社会认同模型将邻里认同与居民的当地环境及其心理健康之间的四个关键途径联系起来。我们回顾了支持这一模式的大量最新研究,这些研究表明,该模式能够整合并扩展已有模式的见解。我们还探讨了社会认同方法对政策和干预措施的影响。公众号摘要:我们需要更好地理解人们的居住地与其心理健康之间的联系。本文提出了一个社会和心理因素影响居民区与其心理健康之间关系的综合模型,以帮助我们实现这一目标。这一模型建立在社会流行病学、健康地理学和城市社会学的基础之上。其独特而新颖的贡献在于指出了邻里认同塑造居民心理健康的四个途径的重要性。最近的大量研究都支持这一模型,并强调了其整合和扩展现有理论的潜力。我们还讨论了我们的模型如何为旨在改善社区心理健康成果的政策和干预措施提供参考。
{"title":"Connecting to Community: A Social Identity Approach to Neighborhood Mental Health.","authors":"S Alexander Haslam, Polly Fong, Catherine Haslam, Tegan Cruwys","doi":"10.1177/10888683231216136","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10888683231216136","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Academic abstract: </strong>Integrative theorizing is needed to advance our understanding of the relationship between where a person lives and their mental health. To this end, we introduce a social identity model that provides an integrated explanation of the ways in which social-psychological processes mediate and moderate the links between neighborhood and mental health. In developing this model, we first review existing models that are derived primarily from a resource-availability perspective informed by research in social epidemiology, health geography, and urban sociology. Building on these, the social identity model implicates neighborhood identification in four key pathways between residents' local environment and their mental health. We review a wealth of recent research that supports this model and which speaks to its capacity to integrate and extend insights from established models. We also explore the implications of the social identity approach for policy and intervention.</p><p><strong>Public abstract: </strong>We need to understand the connection between where people live and their mental health better than we do. This article helps us do this by presenting an integrated model of the way that social and psychological factors affect the relationship between someone's neighborhood and their mental health. This model builds on insights from social epidemiology, health geography, and urban sociology. Its distinct and novel contribution is to point to the importance of four pathways through which neighborhood identification shapes residents' mental health. A large body of recent research supports this model and highlights its potential to integrate and expand upon existing theories. We also discuss how our model can inform policies and interventions that seek to improve mental health outcomes in communities.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":7.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11193917/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139038125","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Case for Heterogeneity in Metacognitive Appraisals of Biased Beliefs. 偏差信念的元认知评估中的异质性案例。
IF 10.8 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-06-07 DOI: 10.1177/10888683241251520
Corey Cusimano

Academic abstract: Prominent theories of belief and metacognition make different predictions about how people evaluate their biased beliefs. These predictions reflect different assumptions about (a) people's conscious belief regulation goals and (b) the mechanisms and constraints underlying belief change. I argue that people exhibit heterogeneity in how they evaluate their biased beliefs. Sometimes people are blind to their biases, sometimes people acknowledge and condone them, and sometimes people resent them. The observation that people adopt a variety of "metacognitive positions" toward their beliefs provides insight into people's belief regulation goals as well as insight into way that belief formation is free and constrained. The way that people relate to their beliefs illuminates why they hold those beliefs. Identifying how someone thinks about their belief is useful for changing their mind.

Public abstract: The same belief can be alternatively thought of as rational, careful, unfortunate, or an act of faith. These beliefs about one's beliefs are called "metacognitive positions." I review evidence that people hold at least four different metacognitive positions. For each position, I discuss what kinds of cognitive processes generated belief and what role people's values and preferences played in belief formation. We can learn a lot about someone's belief based on how they relate to that belief. Learning how someone relates to their belief is useful for identifying the best ways to try to change their mind.

学术摘要:关于信念和元认知的著名理论对人们如何评价自己有偏见的信念做出了不同的预测。这些预测反映了关于(a)人们有意识的信念调节目标和(b)信念变化的机制和制约因素的不同假设。我认为,人们在如何评估其偏差信念方面表现出异质性。有时,人们对自己的偏见视而不见;有时,人们承认并宽容自己的偏见;有时,人们对自己的偏见深恶痛绝。通过观察人们对自己的信念采取的各种 "元认知立场",我们可以深入了解人们的信念调节目标,以及信念形成的自由和受限方式。人们与自己信念的关系揭示了他们为什么会持有这些信念。公众摘要:同样的信念可以被认为是理性的、谨慎的、不幸的,或者是一种信仰行为。这些对自己信念的看法被称为 "元认知立场"。我回顾了人们至少持有四种不同元认知立场的证据。对于每种立场,我都会讨论产生信念的认知过程,以及人们的价值观和偏好在信念形成中扮演的角色。我们可以根据一个人与信念的关系来了解他的信念。了解一个人与其信念的关系有助于确定改变其想法的最佳方法。
{"title":"The Case for Heterogeneity in Metacognitive Appraisals of Biased Beliefs.","authors":"Corey Cusimano","doi":"10.1177/10888683241251520","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10888683241251520","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Academic abstract: </strong>Prominent theories of belief and metacognition make different predictions about how people evaluate their biased beliefs. These predictions reflect different assumptions about (a) people's conscious belief regulation goals and (b) the mechanisms and constraints underlying belief change. I argue that people exhibit heterogeneity in how they evaluate their biased beliefs. Sometimes people are blind to their biases, sometimes people acknowledge and condone them, and sometimes people resent them. The observation that people adopt a variety of \"metacognitive positions\" toward their beliefs provides insight into people's belief regulation goals as well as insight into way that belief formation is free and constrained. The way that people relate to their beliefs illuminates why they hold those beliefs. Identifying how someone thinks about their belief is useful for changing their mind.</p><p><strong>Public abstract: </strong>The same belief can be alternatively thought of as rational, careful, unfortunate, or an act of faith. These beliefs about one's beliefs are called \"metacognitive positions.\" I review evidence that people hold at least four different metacognitive positions. For each position, I discuss what kinds of cognitive processes generated belief and what role people's values and preferences played in belief formation. We can learn a lot about someone's belief based on how they relate to that belief. Learning how someone relates to their belief is useful for identifying the best ways to try to change their mind.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141285084","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How and Why People Synchronize: An Integrated Perspective. 人们如何以及为何同步:综合视角。
IF 10.8 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-05-21 DOI: 10.1177/10888683241252036
Elizabeth B daSilva, Adrienne Wood

Academic AbstractInterpersonal synchrony, the alignment of behavior and/or physiology during interactions, is a pervasive phenomenon observed in diverse social contexts. Here we synthesize across contexts and behaviors to classify the different forms and functions of synchrony. We provide a concise framework for classifying the manifold forms of synchrony along six dimensions: periodicity, discreteness, spatial similarity, directionality, leader-follower dynamics, and observability. We also distill the various proposed functions of interpersonal synchrony into four interconnected functions: reducing complexity and improving understanding, accomplishing joint tasks, strengthening social connection, and influencing partners' behavior. These functions derive from first principles, emerge from each other, and are accomplished by some forms of synchrony more than others. Effective synchrony flexibly adapts to social goals and more synchrony is not always better. Our synthesis offers a shared framework and language for the field, allowing for better cross-context and cross-behavior comparisons, generating new hypotheses, and highlighting future research directions.

学术摘要人际同步是指互动过程中行为和/或生理的一致性,是在不同社会环境中观察到的一种普遍现象。在此,我们综合了各种情境和行为,对同步的不同形式和功能进行了分类。我们提供了一个简明的框架,从六个维度对多种形式的同步进行分类:周期性、离散性、空间相似性、方向性、领导者-追随者动态和可观察性。我们还将所提出的人际同步的各种功能提炼为四种相互关联的功能:降低复杂性和增进理解、完成共同任务、加强社会联系以及影响伙伴的行为。这些功能源于第一性原理,相互影响,某些形式的同步比其他形式的同步更能实现这些功能。有效的同步可以灵活地适应社会目标,同步并不总是越多越好。我们的综述为该领域提供了一个共同的框架和语言,从而可以更好地进行跨情境和跨行为比较,提出新的假设,并突出未来的研究方向。
{"title":"How and Why People Synchronize: An Integrated Perspective.","authors":"Elizabeth B daSilva, Adrienne Wood","doi":"10.1177/10888683241252036","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10888683241252036","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Academic AbstractInterpersonal synchrony, the alignment of behavior and/or physiology during interactions, is a pervasive phenomenon observed in diverse social contexts. Here we synthesize across contexts and behaviors to classify the different forms and functions of synchrony. We provide a concise framework for classifying the manifold forms of synchrony along six dimensions: periodicity, discreteness, spatial similarity, directionality, leader-follower dynamics, and observability. We also distill the various proposed functions of interpersonal synchrony into four interconnected functions: reducing complexity and improving understanding, accomplishing joint tasks, strengthening social connection, and influencing partners' behavior. These functions derive from first principles, emerge from each other, and are accomplished by some forms of synchrony more than others. Effective synchrony flexibly adapts to social goals and more synchrony is not always better. Our synthesis offers a shared framework and language for the field, allowing for better cross-context and cross-behavior comparisons, generating new hypotheses, and highlighting future research directions.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141071920","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distinguishing Emotion Regulation Success in Daily Life From Maladaptive Regulation and Dysregulation. 研究日常生活中情绪调节的成功:与适应不良和调节障碍的区别。
IF 10.8 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-20 DOI: 10.1177/10888683231199140
Tabea Springstein, Tammy English

Academic abstract: This paper aims to motivate research on emotion regulation success in naturalistic settings. We define emotion regulation success as achieving one's emotion regulation goal and differentiate it from related concepts (i.e., maladaptive regulation and dysregulation). As goals vary across individuals and situations, it is insufficient to conceptualize emotion regulation success as maximizing positive affect and minimizing negative affect. Instead, emotion regulation success can be measured through novel approaches targeting the achievement of emotion regulation goals. In addition to utilizing novel data analytic tools (e.g., response surface analyses), future research can make use of informant reports and observing ambulatory behavior or physiology. Considering emotion regulation goals when measuring daily emotion regulation success has the potential to answer key questions about personality, development, and mental health.

Public abstract: People differ in how they want to feel in daily situations (e.g., excited) and why they want to feel that way (e.g., to make others feel better), depending on factors such as culture or age. Although people manage their emotions to reach these goals, most research assessing emotion regulation success has not taken individual goals into account. When assessing if people successfully regulate their emotions, most research in daily life has been focused on whether people feel more positive or less negative. To help study emotion regulation success in a more thoughtful and inclusive way, we propose a new approach to conceptualizing emotion regulation success that incorporates individual differences in what motivates people to regulate and discuss future research directions and applications.

学术摘要:本文旨在推动在自然主义环境中成功进行情绪调节的研究。我们将情绪调节成功定义为实现一个人的情绪调节目标,并将其与相关概念(即适应不良调节和失调)区分开来。由于目标因个人和情况而异,将情绪调节成功概念化为最大化积极影响和最小化消极影响是不够的。相反,情绪调节的成功可以通过针对情绪调节目标实现的新方法来衡量。除了利用新的数据分析工具(如响应面分析)外,未来的研究还可以利用线人报告和观察动态行为或生理学。在衡量日常情绪调节成功率时,考虑情绪调节目标有可能回答有关个性、发展和心理健康的关键问题。公共摘要:根据文化或年龄等因素,人们在日常生活中想要的感受(例如兴奋)以及为什么想要这种感觉(例如让他人感觉更好)各不相同。尽管人们通过控制自己的情绪来达到这些目标,但大多数评估情绪调节成功率的研究都没有考虑到个人目标。在评估人们是否成功地调节了自己的情绪时,日常生活中的大多数研究都集中在人们感觉更积极还是更少消极上。为了帮助以更深思熟虑和更具包容性的方式研究情绪调节的成功,我们提出了一种新的方法来概念化情绪调节成功,该方法将个体差异纳入激励人们调节的因素中,并讨论未来的研究方向和应用。
{"title":"Distinguishing Emotion Regulation Success in Daily Life From Maladaptive Regulation and Dysregulation.","authors":"Tabea Springstein, Tammy English","doi":"10.1177/10888683231199140","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10888683231199140","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Academic abstract: </strong>This paper aims to motivate research on emotion regulation success in naturalistic settings. We define emotion regulation success as achieving one's emotion regulation goal and differentiate it from related concepts (i.e., maladaptive regulation and dysregulation). As goals vary across individuals and situations, it is insufficient to conceptualize emotion regulation success as maximizing positive affect and minimizing negative affect. Instead, emotion regulation success can be measured through novel approaches targeting the achievement of emotion regulation goals. In addition to utilizing novel data analytic tools (e.g., response surface analyses), future research can make use of informant reports and observing ambulatory behavior or physiology. Considering emotion regulation goals when measuring daily emotion regulation success has the potential to answer key questions about personality, development, and mental health.</p><p><strong>Public abstract: </strong>People differ in how they want to feel in daily situations (e.g., excited) and why they want to feel that way (e.g., to make others feel better), depending on factors such as culture or age. Although people manage their emotions to reach these goals, most research assessing emotion regulation success has not taken individual goals into account. When assessing if people successfully regulate their emotions, most research in daily life has been focused on whether people feel more positive or less negative. To help study emotion regulation success in a more thoughtful and inclusive way, we propose a new approach to conceptualizing emotion regulation success that incorporates individual differences in what motivates people to regulate and discuss future research directions and applications.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41148374","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Intergroup Value Protection Model: A Theoretically Integrative and Dynamic Approach to Intergroup Conflict Escalation in Democratic Societies. 群体间价值保护模型:民主社会中群体间冲突升级的理论整合与动态方法。
IF 10.8 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-05 DOI: 10.1177/10888683231192120
Martijn van Zomeren, Chantal d'Amore, Inga Lisa Pauls, Eric Shuman, Ana Leal

Scientific abstract: We review social-psychological evidence for a theoretically integrative and dynamic model of intergroup conflict escalation within democratic societies. Viewing individuals as social regulators who protect their social embeddedness (e.g., in their group or in society), the intergroup value protection model (IVPM) integrates key insights and concepts from moral and group psychology (e.g., group identification, outrage, moralization, protest) into a functional intergroup value protection process. The model assumes that social regulators are continuously looking for information diagnostic of the outgroup's intentions to terminate the relationship with the ingroup, and that their specific cognitive interpretations of an outgroup's action (i.e., as a violation of ingroup or shared values) trigger this process. The visible value-protective responses of one group can trigger the other group's value-protective responses, thus dynamically increasing chances of conflict escalation. We discuss scientific implications of integrating moral and group psychology and practical challenges for managing intergroup conflict within democratic societies.

Public abstract: The 2021 Capitol Hill attack exemplifies a major "trigger event" for different groups to protect their values within a democratic society. Which specific perceptions generate such a triggering event, which value-protective responses does it trigger, and do such responses escalate intergroup conflict? We offer the intergroup value protection model to analyze the moral and group psychology of intergroup conflict escalation in democratic societies. It predicts that when group members cognitively interpret another group's actions as violating ingroup or shared values, this triggers the intergroup value protection process (e.g., increased ingroup identification, outrage, moralization, social protest). When such value-protective responses are visible to the outgroup, this can in turn constitute a trigger event for them to protect their values, thus increasing chances of intergroup conflict escalation. We discuss scientific implications and practical challenges for managing intergroup value conflict in democratic societies, including fears of societal breakdown and scope for social change.

科学摘要:我们回顾了民主社会中群体间冲突升级的理论整合动态模型的社会心理学证据。群际价值保护模型(IVPM)将个人视为保护其社会嵌入性(如在其群体或社会中)的社会调节者,将道德和群体心理学(如群体认同、愤怒、道德化、抗议)的主要观点和概念整合到一个功能性的群际价值保护过程中。该模型假定,社会监管者会不断寻找外群体意图终止与内群体关系的诊断信息,而他们对外群体行为的特定认知解释(即认为其违反了内群体或共同价值观)会触发这一过程。一个群体可见的价值保护反应会触发另一个群体的价值保护反应,从而动态地增加冲突升级的机会。我们讨论了整合道德心理学和群体心理学的科学意义,以及在民主社会中管理群体间冲突所面临的实际挑战。公众摘要:2021 年的国会山袭击事件是民主社会中不同群体保护自身价值观的重大 "触发事件 "的典范。哪些具体的观念会产生这样的触发事件,它引发了哪些价值保护反应,以及这些反应是否会使群体间冲突升级?我们提供了群体间价值保护模型来分析民主社会中群体间冲突升级的道德和群体心理。该模型预测,当群体成员在认知上将另一群体的行为解释为违反了本群体或共同价值观时,就会触发群体间价值保护过程(例如,增强本群体认同、愤怒、道德化、社会抗议)。当这种价值保护反应被外群体看到时,这反过来又会成为他们保护自身价值的触发事件,从而增加群体间冲突升级的机会。我们讨论了在民主社会中管理群体间价值冲突的科学意义和实际挑战,包括对社会崩溃和社会变革范围的担忧。
{"title":"The Intergroup Value Protection Model: A Theoretically Integrative and Dynamic Approach to Intergroup Conflict Escalation in Democratic Societies.","authors":"Martijn van Zomeren, Chantal d'Amore, Inga Lisa Pauls, Eric Shuman, Ana Leal","doi":"10.1177/10888683231192120","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10888683231192120","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Scientific abstract: </strong>We review social-psychological evidence for a theoretically integrative and dynamic model of intergroup conflict escalation within democratic societies. Viewing individuals as social regulators who protect their social embeddedness (e.g., in their group or in society), the <i>intergroup value protection model (IVPM)</i> integrates key insights and concepts from moral and group psychology (e.g., group identification, outrage, moralization, protest) into a functional intergroup value protection process. The model assumes that social regulators are continuously looking for information diagnostic of the outgroup's intentions to terminate the relationship with the ingroup, and that their specific cognitive interpretations of an outgroup's action (i.e., as a violation of ingroup or shared values) trigger this process. The visible value-protective responses of one group can trigger the other group's value-protective responses, thus dynamically increasing chances of conflict escalation. We discuss scientific implications of integrating moral and group psychology and practical challenges for managing intergroup conflict within democratic societies.</p><p><strong>Public abstract: </strong>The 2021 Capitol Hill attack exemplifies a major \"trigger event\" for different groups to protect their values within a democratic society. Which specific perceptions generate such a triggering event, which value-protective responses does it trigger, and do such responses escalate intergroup conflict? We offer the <i>intergroup value protection model</i> to analyze the moral and group psychology of intergroup conflict escalation in democratic societies. It predicts that when group members cognitively interpret another group's actions as violating ingroup or shared values, this triggers the intergroup value protection process (e.g., increased ingroup identification, outrage, moralization, social protest). When such value-protective responses are visible to the outgroup, this can in turn constitute a trigger event for them to protect <i>their</i> values, thus increasing chances of intergroup conflict escalation. We discuss scientific implications and practical challenges for managing intergroup value conflict in democratic societies, including fears of societal breakdown and scope for social change.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11010547/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10145536","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mobilizing or Sedative Effects? A Narrative Review of the Association Between Intergroup Contact and Collective Action Among Advantaged and Disadvantaged Groups. 动员作用还是镇静作用?有利和不利群体之间的群体间联系和集体行动之间的联系的叙述性回顾。
IF 10.8 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-21 DOI: 10.1177/10888683231203141
Veronica Margherita Cocco, Loris Vezzali, Sofia Stathi, Gian Antonio Di Bernardo, John F Dovidio

Academic abstract: In this narrative review, we examined 134 studies of the relationship between intergroup contact and collective action benefiting disadvantaged groups. We aimed to identify whether, when, and why contact has mobilizing effects (promoting collective action) or sedative effects (inhibiting collective action). For both moderators and mediators, factors associated with the intergroup situation (compared with those associated with the out-group or the in-group) emerged as the most important. Group status had important effects. For members of socially advantaged groups (examined in 98 studies, 100 samples), contact had a general mobilizing effect, which was stronger when contact increased awareness of experiences of injustice among members of disadvantaged groups. For members of disadvantaged groups (examined in 49 studies, 58 samples), contact had mixed effects. Contact that increased awareness of injustice mobilized collection action; contact that made the legitimacy of group hierarchy or threat of retaliation more salient produced sedative effects.

Public abstract: We present a review of existing studies that have investigated the relationship between intergroup contact and collective action aimed at promoting equity for disadvantaged groups. We further consider the influence of contact that is positive or negative and face-to-face or indirect (e.g., through mass or social media), and we distinguish between collective action that involves socially acceptable behaviors or is destructive and violent. We identified 134 studies, considering both advantaged (100 samples) and disadvantaged groups (58 samples). We found that intergroup contact impacts collective action differently depending on group status. Contact generally leads advantaged groups to mobilize in favor of disadvantaged groups. However, contact has variable effects on members of disadvantaged groups: It sometimes promotes their collective action in support of their own group; in other cases, it leads them to be less likely to engage in such action. We examine when and why contact can have these different effects.

学术摘要:在这篇叙述性综述中,我们研究了134项关于群体间接触和有利于弱势群体的集体行动之间关系的研究。我们旨在确定接触是否、何时以及为什么具有动员作用(促进集体行动)或镇静作用(抑制集体行动)。对于调节者和中介者来说,与组间情况相关的因素(与组外或组内相关的因素相比)是最重要的。群体地位产生了重要影响。对于社会弱势群体的成员(在98项研究、100个样本中进行了调查),接触具有普遍的动员作用,当接触提高了弱势群体成员对不公正经历的认识时,这种动员作用更强。对于弱势群体的成员(在49项研究、58个样本中进行了调查),接触产生了混合影响。提高对不公正认识的联系动员了收集行动;使群体等级制度的合法性或报复威胁更加突出的接触产生了镇静作用。公共摘要:我们对现有研究进行了回顾,这些研究调查了群体间接触和旨在促进弱势群体公平的集体行动之间的关系。我们进一步考虑积极或消极、面对面或间接(例如,通过大众或社交媒体)的接触的影响,并区分涉及社会可接受行为或破坏性和暴力的集体行动。我们确定了134项研究,同时考虑了优势群体(100个样本)和弱势群体(58个样本)。我们发现,群体间接触对集体行动的影响因群体地位而异。接触通常会导致优势群体动员起来支持弱势群体。然而,接触对弱势群体成员的影响各不相同:它有时会促进他们支持自己群体的集体行动;在其他情况下,这会导致他们不太可能参与此类行动。我们研究了接触何时以及为什么会产生这些不同的影响。
{"title":"Mobilizing or Sedative Effects? A Narrative Review of the Association Between Intergroup Contact and Collective Action Among Advantaged and Disadvantaged Groups.","authors":"Veronica Margherita Cocco, Loris Vezzali, Sofia Stathi, Gian Antonio Di Bernardo, John F Dovidio","doi":"10.1177/10888683231203141","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10888683231203141","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Academic abstract: </strong>In this narrative review, we examined 134 studies of the relationship between intergroup contact and collective action benefiting disadvantaged groups. We aimed to identify whether, when, and why contact has mobilizing effects (promoting collective action) or sedative effects (inhibiting collective action). For both moderators and mediators, factors associated with the intergroup situation (compared with those associated with the out-group or the in-group) emerged as the most important. Group status had important effects. For members of socially advantaged groups (examined in 98 studies, 100 samples), contact had a general mobilizing effect, which was stronger when contact increased awareness of experiences of injustice among members of disadvantaged groups. For members of disadvantaged groups (examined in 49 studies, 58 samples), contact had mixed effects. Contact that increased awareness of injustice mobilized collection action; contact that made the legitimacy of group hierarchy or threat of retaliation more salient produced sedative effects.</p><p><strong>Public abstract: </strong>We present a review of existing studies that have investigated the relationship between intergroup contact and collective action aimed at promoting equity for disadvantaged groups. We further consider the influence of contact that is positive or negative and face-to-face or indirect (e.g., through mass or social media), and we distinguish between collective action that involves socially acceptable behaviors or is destructive and violent. We identified 134 studies, considering both advantaged (100 samples) and disadvantaged groups (58 samples). We found that intergroup contact impacts collective action differently depending on group status. Contact generally leads advantaged groups to mobilize in favor of disadvantaged groups. However, contact has variable effects on members of disadvantaged groups: It sometimes promotes their collective action in support of their own group; in other cases, it leads them to be less likely to engage in such action. We examine when and why contact can have these different effects.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11010580/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49683676","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mental Time Travel as Self-Affirmation. 精神时间旅行作为自我肯定。
IF 10.8 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-24 DOI: 10.1177/10888683231203143
Elena Stephan, Constantine Sedikides

Academic abstract: This article integrates and advances the scope of research on the role of mental time travel in bolstering the self. We propose that imagining the self in the future (prospection) or in the past (retrospection) highlights central and positive self-aspects. Thus, bringing to mind one's future or past broadens the perceived bases of self-integrity and offers a route to self-affirmation. In reviewing corresponding research programs on self-prospection and nostalgia, we illustrate that mental time travel serves to affirm the self in terms of self-esteem, coherence, and control. Mental time travel could be implemented as a source of self-affirmation for facilitating coping and behavior change in several domains such as relationships, health, education, and organizational contexts.

Public abstract: People can mentally travel to their future or to their past. When people imagine what they will be like in the future, or what they were like in the past, they tend to think about themselves in terms of the important and positive attributes that they possess. Thinking about themselves in such an affirming way expands and consolidates their self-views. This broader image of themselves can increase self-esteem (the extent to which one likes who they are), coherence (the extent to which one perceives life as meaningful), and control (the extent to which one feels capable of initiating and pursuing goals or effecting desirable outcomes). Mental time travel, then, has favorable or affirming consequences for one's self-views. These consequences can be harnessed to modify one's behavior in such life domains as relationships, health, education, and work.

学术摘要:本文整合并推进了心理时间旅行在增强自我中作用的研究范围。我们提出,想象未来的自我(前瞻)或过去的自我(回顾)突出了中心和积极的自我方面。因此,让人想起自己的未来或过去,拓宽了自我完整的感知基础,并提供了一条自我肯定的途径。在回顾相应的关于自我前瞻和怀旧的研究项目时,我们表明心理时间旅行有助于在自尊、连贯性和控制方面肯定自我。心理时间旅行可以作为自我肯定的来源,在关系、健康、教育和组织环境等多个领域促进应对和行为改变。公共摘要:人们可以在精神上旅行到他们的未来或过去。当人们想象自己未来会是什么样子,或者过去是什么样子时,他们倾向于从自己所拥有的重要和积极的特质来思考自己。以这样一种肯定的方式思考自己,扩展并巩固了他们的自我观。这种更广泛的自我形象可以增强自尊(一个人喜欢自己的程度)、连贯性(一个人认为生活有意义的程度)和控制力(一个人觉得有能力发起和追求目标或实现理想结果的程度)。因此,心理时间旅行对一个人的自我观点有着有利或肯定的影响。这些后果可以用来改变一个人在人际关系、健康、教育和工作等生活领域的行为。
{"title":"Mental Time Travel as Self-Affirmation.","authors":"Elena Stephan, Constantine Sedikides","doi":"10.1177/10888683231203143","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10888683231203143","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Academic abstract: </strong>This article integrates and advances the scope of research on the role of mental time travel in bolstering the self. We propose that imagining the self in the future (prospection) or in the past (retrospection) highlights central and positive self-aspects. Thus, bringing to mind one's future or past broadens the perceived bases of self-integrity and offers a route to self-affirmation. In reviewing corresponding research programs on self-prospection and nostalgia, we illustrate that mental time travel serves to affirm the self in terms of self-esteem, coherence, and control. Mental time travel could be implemented as a source of self-affirmation for facilitating coping and behavior change in several domains such as relationships, health, education, and organizational contexts.</p><p><strong>Public abstract: </strong>People can mentally travel to their future or to their past. When people imagine what they will be like in the future, or what they were like in the past, they tend to think about themselves in terms of the important and positive attributes that they possess. Thinking about themselves in such an affirming way expands and consolidates their self-views. This broader image of themselves can increase self-esteem (the extent to which one likes who they are), coherence (the extent to which one perceives life as meaningful), and control (the extent to which one feels capable of initiating and pursuing goals or effecting desirable outcomes). Mental time travel, then, has favorable or affirming consequences for one's self-views. These consequences can be harnessed to modify one's behavior in such life domains as relationships, health, education, and work.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50159005","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Personality and Social Psychology Review
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1