首页 > 最新文献

Personality and Social Psychology Review最新文献

英文 中文
Beyond Trolleyology: The CNI Model of Moral-Dilemma Responses. 超越电车学:道德困境应对的 CNI 模型。
IF 7.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-13 DOI: 10.1177/10888683241234114
Bertram Gawronski, Nyx L Ng

Public abstract: How do people make judgments about actions that violate moral norms yet maximize the greater good (e.g., sacrificing the well-being of a small number of people for the well-being of a larger number of people)? Research on this question has been criticized for relying on highly artificial scenarios and for conflating multiple distinct factors underlying responses in moral dilemmas. The current article reviews research that used a computational modeling approach to disentangle the roles of multiple distinct factors in responses to plausible moral dilemmas based on real-world events. By disentangling sensitivity to consequences, sensitivity to moral norms, and general preference for inaction versus action in responses to realistic dilemmas, the reviewed work provides a more nuanced understanding of how people make judgments about the right course of action in moral dilemmas.

公共摘要:人们如何对违反道德规范但又能使更大利益最大化的行为(例如,牺牲少数人的福祉换取更多人的福祉)做出判断?关于这个问题的研究一直受到批评,因为这些研究依赖于高度人为的情景,并且混淆了道德困境中反应的多种不同因素。本文回顾了一些研究,这些研究采用计算建模的方法,以现实世界的事件为基础,厘清了多个不同因素在合理的道德困境反应中的作用。通过将对后果的敏感性、对道德规范的敏感性以及在对现实困境做出反应时对不作为与作为的一般偏好区分开来,所综述的工作提供了对人们如何在道德困境中判断正确行动方针的更细致入微的理解。
{"title":"Beyond Trolleyology: The CNI Model of Moral-Dilemma Responses.","authors":"Bertram Gawronski, Nyx L Ng","doi":"10.1177/10888683241234114","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10888683241234114","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Public abstract: </strong>How do people make judgments about actions that violate moral norms yet maximize the greater good (e.g., sacrificing the well-being of a small number of people for the well-being of a larger number of people)? Research on this question has been criticized for relying on highly artificial scenarios and for conflating multiple distinct factors underlying responses in moral dilemmas. The current article reviews research that used a computational modeling approach to disentangle the roles of multiple distinct factors in responses to plausible moral dilemmas based on real-world events. By disentangling sensitivity to consequences, sensitivity to moral norms, and general preference for inaction versus action in responses to realistic dilemmas, the reviewed work provides a more nuanced understanding of how people make judgments about the right course of action in moral dilemmas.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":" ","pages":"32-80"},"PeriodicalIF":7.7,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11734360/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140111896","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When People Do Allyship: A Typology of Allyship Action. 当人们结成同盟时:盟友关系行动类型学》。
IF 7.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-09 DOI: 10.1177/10888683241232732
Lucy De Souza, Toni Schmader

Academic abstract: Despite increased popular and academic interest, there is conceptual ambiguity about what allyship is and the forms it takes. Viewing allyship as a practice, we introduce the typology of allyship action which organizes the diversity of ways that advantaged individuals seek to support those who are disadvantaged. We characterize allyship actions as reactive (addressing bias when it occurs) and proactive (fostering positive outcomes such as feelings of inclusion, respect, and capacity), both of which can vary in level of analysis (i.e., targeting oneself, one or a few other individuals, or institutions). We use this framework to profile six productive yet largely independent bodies of social psychological literature on social action and directly compare relative benefits and constraints of different actions. We suggest several future directions for empirical research, using the typology of allyship to understand when, where, and how different forms of allyship might succeed.

Public abstract: Despite increased popular and academic interest in the word, people differ in what they believe allyship is and the forms it takes. Viewing allyship as a practice, we introduce a new way (the typology of allyship action) to describe how advantaged individuals seek to support those who are disadvantaged. We characterize allyship actions as reactive (addressing bias when it occurs) and proactive (increasing positive outcomes such as feelings of inclusion, respect, and capacity), both of which can vary in level (i.e., targeting oneself, one or a few other individuals, or institutions). We use this framework to profile six large yet mostly separate areas of social psychological research on social action and directly compare the relative benefits and limitations of different actions. We suggest several future directions for how the typology of allyship action can help us understand when, where, and how different forms of allyship might succeed.

学术摘要:尽管大众和学术界对盟友关系的兴趣与日俱增,但对于什么是盟友关系以及盟友关系的形式,在概念上仍存在模糊不清之处。将同盟关系视为一种实践,我们介绍了同盟关系行动类型学,它组织了优势个体寻求支持弱势个体的多种方式。我们将结盟行动描述为反应性行动(在偏见发生时解决偏见)和主动性行动(促进积极的结果,如包容感、尊重和能力),这两种行动在分析层面上可以有所不同(即针对自己、一个或几个人或机构)。我们利用这一框架,对有关社会行动的六种富有成效但基本独立的社会心理学文献进行了概述,并直接比较了不同行动的相对益处和制约因素。我们提出了未来实证研究的几个方向,利用盟友关系的类型学来理解不同形式的盟友关系在何时、何地以及如何取得成功。公众摘要:尽管公众和学术界对盟友关系一词的兴趣日益浓厚,但人们对盟友关系的理解和形式却各不相同。我们将盟友关系视为一种实践,并引入了一种新的方法(盟友关系行动类型学)来描述处于优势地位的个体如何寻求对处于劣势地位的个体的支持。我们将盟友关系行动描述为反应性行动(在偏见发生时解决偏见问题)和主动性行动(增加积极的结果,如包容感、尊重和能力),这两种行动的程度各不相同(即针对自己、一个或几个人或机构)。我们利用这一框架来概述社会行动方面的六大但大多独立的社会心理学研究领域,并直接比较不同行动的相对益处和局限性。我们就盟友关系行动类型学如何帮助我们理解不同形式的盟友关系何时、何地以及如何取得成功提出了几个未来发展方向。
{"title":"When People Do Allyship: A Typology of Allyship Action.","authors":"Lucy De Souza, Toni Schmader","doi":"10.1177/10888683241232732","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10888683241232732","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Academic abstract: </strong>Despite increased popular and academic interest, there is conceptual ambiguity about what allyship is and the forms it takes. Viewing allyship as a practice, we introduce the <i>typology of allyship action</i> which organizes the diversity of ways that advantaged individuals seek to support those who are disadvantaged. We characterize <i>allyship actions</i> as reactive (addressing bias when it occurs) and proactive (fostering positive outcomes such as feelings of inclusion, respect, and capacity), both of which can vary in level of analysis (i.e., targeting oneself, one or a few other individuals, or institutions). We use this framework to profile six productive yet largely independent bodies of social psychological literature on social action and directly compare relative benefits and constraints of different actions. We suggest several future directions for empirical research, using the typology of allyship to understand when, where, and how different forms of allyship might succeed.</p><p><strong>Public abstract: </strong>Despite increased popular and academic interest in the word, people differ in what they believe allyship is and the forms it takes. Viewing allyship as a practice, we introduce a new way (the <i>typology of allyship action</i>) to describe how advantaged individuals seek to support those who are disadvantaged. We characterize <i>allyship actions</i> as reactive (addressing bias when it occurs) and proactive (increasing positive outcomes such as feelings of inclusion, respect, and capacity), both of which can vary in level (i.e., targeting oneself, one or a few other individuals, or institutions). We use this framework to profile six large yet mostly separate areas of social psychological research on social action and directly compare the relative benefits and limitations of different actions. We suggest several future directions for how the typology of allyship action can help us understand when, where, and how different forms of allyship might succeed.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":" ","pages":"3-31"},"PeriodicalIF":7.7,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11734359/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140066073","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When Social Hierarchy, Power, and Collective Autonomy Motivate Social Movement and Counter-Movement Mobilization Among Disadvantaged and Advantaged Groups 当社会等级、权力和集体自治激发弱势群体和优势群体的社会运动和反运动动员
IF 10.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2025-01-09 DOI: 10.1177/10888683241305662
Adrian Rivera-Rodriguez, Evelyn Mercado, Linda R. Tropp, Nilanjana Dasgupta
What happens when disadvantaged group members try to gain power in an attempt to protect their collective autonomy? The present integrative review outlines dynamic social processes and conditions under which efforts to restrict a group’s collective autonomy motivate social movement mobilization among disadvantaged groups to challenge social hierarchies that limit their power. This, in turn, threatens advantaged groups’ perceptions of their access to power and, by extension, their sense of collective autonomy, motivating them to reaffirm the existing social hierarchy by mobilizing counter-movements. We propose a theoretical model, called the Movement Mobilization Model of Collective Autonomy, to articulate these dynamic processes by integrating sociological, psychological, and organizational science literatures. The model specifies the conditions under which social movements and counter-movements are activated, psychological processes that drive action, how they play off each other, and offer directions for future research.Public AbstractWhat happens when disadvantaged groups feel that their freedom to define and practice their collective identity (i.e., collective autonomy) is restricted? The present theoretical review outlines the conditions under which social inequality activates the feelings of collective autonomy restriction among disadvantaged group members and motivates the mobilization of social movements seeking social equality. As these social movements gain traction, advantaged group members may feel that their privileged position and collective autonomy are threatened. These feelings of threat and collective autonomy restriction among advantaged groups in turn motivate them to mobilize counter-movements that seek to protect social hierarchy and their privileged position within it. The process outlined in this case is relevant to individuals from both marginalized and privileged backgrounds, as it illustrates the different ways in which real-world power structures shape the way they experience and navigate social movements and counter-movements as they unfold in real time.
当弱势群体成员试图获得权力以保护他们的集体自主权时,会发生什么?本综合综述概述了动态的社会过程和条件,在这些过程和条件下,限制群体集体自治的努力激发了弱势群体的社会运动动员,以挑战限制其权力的社会等级制度。反过来,这又威胁到优势群体对自己获得权力的看法,进而威胁到他们的集体自治意识,促使他们通过动员反运动来重申现有的社会等级制度。我们提出了一个理论模型,称为集体自治的运动动员模型,通过整合社会学,心理学和组织科学文献来阐明这些动态过程。该模型详细说明了社会运动和反运动被激活的条件,驱动行动的心理过程,它们是如何相互影响的,并为未来的研究提供了方向。当弱势群体感到他们定义和实践集体身份的自由(即集体自治)受到限制时,会发生什么?本理论综述概述了社会不平等激活弱势群体成员集体自治限制的感觉并激发寻求社会平等的社会运动的动员的条件。随着这些社会运动获得动力,优势群体成员可能会感到他们的特权地位和集体自主权受到威胁。优势群体的这种威胁感和集体自治限制反过来又激励他们动员反运动,寻求保护社会等级和他们在其中的特权地位。本案例中概述的过程与来自边缘和特权背景的个人相关,因为它说明了现实世界权力结构塑造他们体验和驾驭社会运动和反运动的不同方式,因为它们在实时展开。
{"title":"When Social Hierarchy, Power, and Collective Autonomy Motivate Social Movement and Counter-Movement Mobilization Among Disadvantaged and Advantaged Groups","authors":"Adrian Rivera-Rodriguez, Evelyn Mercado, Linda R. Tropp, Nilanjana Dasgupta","doi":"10.1177/10888683241305662","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10888683241305662","url":null,"abstract":"What happens when disadvantaged group members try to gain power in an attempt to protect their collective autonomy? The present integrative review outlines dynamic social processes and conditions under which efforts to restrict a group’s collective autonomy motivate social movement mobilization among disadvantaged groups to challenge social hierarchies that limit their power. This, in turn, threatens advantaged groups’ perceptions of their access to power and, by extension, their sense of collective autonomy, motivating them to reaffirm the existing social hierarchy by mobilizing counter-movements. We propose a theoretical model, called the Movement Mobilization Model of Collective Autonomy, to articulate these dynamic processes by integrating sociological, psychological, and organizational science literatures. The model specifies the conditions under which social movements and counter-movements are activated, psychological processes that drive action, how they play off each other, and offer directions for future research.Public AbstractWhat happens when disadvantaged groups feel that their freedom to define and practice their collective identity (i.e., collective autonomy) is restricted? The present theoretical review outlines the conditions under which social inequality activates the feelings of collective autonomy restriction among disadvantaged group members and motivates the mobilization of social movements seeking social equality. As these social movements gain traction, advantaged group members may feel that their privileged position and collective autonomy are threatened. These feelings of threat and collective autonomy restriction among advantaged groups in turn motivate them to mobilize counter-movements that seek to protect social hierarchy and their privileged position within it. The process outlined in this case is relevant to individuals from both marginalized and privileged backgrounds, as it illustrates the different ways in which real-world power structures shape the way they experience and navigate social movements and counter-movements as they unfold in real time.","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":"204 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":10.8,"publicationDate":"2025-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142940164","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Beyond Good or Bad: The Four Evaluative Quadrants of Relationships 超越好坏:人际关系的四个评价象限
IF 10.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-12-30 DOI: 10.1177/10888683241302247
Francesca Righetti, Mirna Đurić, Iris Schneider
Academic AbstractTraditionally, theoretical and empirical accounts have considered relationship evaluations along one single dimension ranging from positive to negative. However, in this theoretical work, we stress the importance of using a bi-dimensional conceptualization of relationship evaluations in which positive and negative dimensions can vary independently. In doing so, we describe the four evaluative quadrants experienced in relationships and outline their unique interpersonal processes and outcomes, both from the perspective of the person experiencing them (i.e., actor effects) and from the perspective of the recipient of such evaluations (i.e., partner effects) and considering both explicit (i.e., deliberative) and implicit (i.e., automatic) processes. We also provide a framework that predicts how relationship properties are likely to influence relationships evaluations, and we introduce the Trajectories of Relationship Evaluation Model (TREM) that describes changes in evaluations over time and the factors that influence such changes.Public AbstractThe way we evaluate other people has important implications for how we relate to others and for our psychological and physical health. However, previous research has mostly focused on positive or negative evaluations in relationships. But there are two other types of evaluations that people commonly experience in relationships: ambivalence and indifference. In this work, we argue that it is important to study all four different evaluative types in relationships (i.e., mostly positive, mostly negative, ambivalence, and indifference) because they each uniquely predict certain relationship dynamics and processes. We discuss the consequences of these different types of evaluations for the person who holds the evaluation and for the person who is the target of such evaluation, and we discuss how these evaluations affect both deliberative and automatic processes. Finally, we propose a model (TREM) of how relationship evaluations evolve over time and of the factors that influence the changes in evaluations.
学术摘要传统上,理论和实证都是从积极到消极的单一维度来考虑关系评价的。然而,在这项理论工作中,我们强调了使用关系评估的双向概念化的重要性,其中积极和消极的维度可以独立变化。在此过程中,我们描述了在人际关系中经历的四个评估象限,并概述了它们独特的人际过程和结果,无论是从经历它们的人的角度(即,行动者效应)还是从这些评估的接受者的角度(即,伙伴效应),并考虑了显性(即,协商)和隐性(即,自动)过程。我们还提供了一个框架来预测关系属性如何可能影响关系评估,我们引入了关系评估模型的轨迹(TREM),该模型描述了随着时间的推移评估的变化以及影响这种变化的因素。我们评价他人的方式对我们如何与他人相处以及我们的心理和身体健康有着重要的影响。然而,之前的研究主要集中在人际关系中的积极或消极评价上。但人们在恋爱中通常会经历另外两种评估:矛盾心理和冷漠。在这项工作中,我们认为研究关系中所有四种不同的评估类型(即主要是积极的,主要是消极的,矛盾的和冷漠的)是很重要的,因为它们各自独特地预测某些关系的动态和过程。我们讨论了这些不同类型的评估对进行评估的人和被评估的人的影响,我们还讨论了这些评估是如何影响审议过程和自动过程的。最后,我们提出了一个模型(TREM)的关系评价是如何随着时间的推移而演变的,以及影响评价变化的因素。
{"title":"Beyond Good or Bad: The Four Evaluative Quadrants of Relationships","authors":"Francesca Righetti, Mirna Đurić, Iris Schneider","doi":"10.1177/10888683241302247","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10888683241302247","url":null,"abstract":"Academic AbstractTraditionally, theoretical and empirical accounts have considered relationship evaluations along one single dimension ranging from positive to negative. However, in this theoretical work, we stress the importance of using a bi-dimensional conceptualization of relationship evaluations in which positive and negative dimensions can vary independently. In doing so, we describe the four evaluative quadrants experienced in relationships and outline their unique interpersonal processes and outcomes, both from the perspective of the person experiencing them (i.e., actor effects) and from the perspective of the recipient of such evaluations (i.e., partner effects) and considering both explicit (i.e., deliberative) and implicit (i.e., automatic) processes. We also provide a framework that predicts how relationship properties are likely to influence relationships evaluations, and we introduce the Trajectories of Relationship Evaluation Model (TREM) that describes changes in evaluations over time and the factors that influence such changes.Public AbstractThe way we evaluate other people has important implications for how we relate to others and for our psychological and physical health. However, previous research has mostly focused on positive or negative evaluations in relationships. But there are two other types of evaluations that people commonly experience in relationships: ambivalence and indifference. In this work, we argue that it is important to study all four different evaluative types in relationships (i.e., mostly positive, mostly negative, ambivalence, and indifference) because they each uniquely predict certain relationship dynamics and processes. We discuss the consequences of these different types of evaluations for the person who holds the evaluation and for the person who is the target of such evaluation, and we discuss how these evaluations affect both deliberative and automatic processes. Finally, we propose a model (TREM) of how relationship evaluations evolve over time and of the factors that influence the changes in evaluations.","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":"65 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":10.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142901606","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Prioritization of Prospection. 勘探的优先级。
IF 7.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-11-30 DOI: 10.1177/10888683241292849
Eugene M Caruso, Sam J Maglio, Leaf Van Boven

Academic abstract: Humans frequently engage in mental time travel, reflecting on the past and anticipating the future. Although these processes may seem similar, research documents systematic differences between retrospection and prospection. We propose a conceptual framework to organize and explain these differences based on three axiomatic temporal asymmetries: The past occurs before the future; the past is more certain than the future; and the past is less controllable than the future. People's experience with these axiomatic differences is internalized and overgeneralized to shape mental representations of the past and future. Our review shows that people generally prioritize prospection over retrospection, attending more to the future than the past and reacting more intensely to future events than to past events. We consider potential moderators of and constraints on the generality of prioritizing prospection. We explore the implications of these temporal asymmetries, emphasizing their theoretical and practical significance.

Public abstract: While daily life centers on the present, people often reflect on the past and anticipate the future. But which direction of mental time travel-backward or forward-has more influence? We identify three key differences that shape how people engage with the past and future: time flows from past to future, the future is more uncertain, and people have more control over the future. These differences affect the frequency, intensity, and nature of thoughts and feelings, leading to predictable biases in how we mentally represent and emotionally engage with events over time. Because focusing on the future often provides greater benefits, people tend to prioritize prospection over retrospection in everyday life.

学术摘要:人类经常进行心理时间旅行,反思过去,展望未来。尽管这些过程看起来相似,但研究证明了回顾和展望之间的系统性差异。我们提出了一个概念框架来组织和解释基于三个公理时间不对称性的这些差异:过去发生在未来之前;过去比未来更确定;过去比未来更难控制。人们对这些公理差异的经验被内化和过度概括,从而形成对过去和未来的心理表征。我们的研究表明,人们通常优先考虑展望而不是回顾,更关注未来而不是过去,对未来的反应比对过去的反应更强烈。我们考虑潜在的调节因素和限制优先前景的普遍性。我们探讨了这些时间不对称的含义,强调了它们的理论和实践意义。公众摘要:当人们的日常生活以现在为中心时,他们往往会反思过去,展望未来。但是心理时间旅行的哪个方向——向前还是向后——影响更大呢?我们确定了影响人们如何对待过去和未来的三个关键差异:时间从过去流向未来,未来更不确定,人们对未来有更多的控制。这些差异会影响思想和感受的频率、强度和性质,导致我们在心理上代表和情感上参与事件的方式随着时间的推移而产生可预见的偏见。因为关注未来往往会带来更大的好处,所以在日常生活中,人们倾向于优先考虑展望而不是回顾。
{"title":"The Prioritization of Prospection.","authors":"Eugene M Caruso, Sam J Maglio, Leaf Van Boven","doi":"10.1177/10888683241292849","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10888683241292849","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Academic abstract: </strong>Humans frequently engage in mental time travel, reflecting on the past and anticipating the future. Although these processes may seem similar, research documents systematic differences between retrospection and prospection. We propose a conceptual framework to organize and explain these differences based on three axiomatic temporal asymmetries: The past occurs before the future; the past is more certain than the future; and the past is less controllable than the future. People's experience with these axiomatic differences is internalized and overgeneralized to shape mental representations of the past and future. Our review shows that people generally prioritize prospection over retrospection, attending more to the future than the past and reacting more intensely to future events than to past events. We consider potential moderators of and constraints on the generality of prioritizing prospection. We explore the implications of these temporal asymmetries, emphasizing their theoretical and practical significance.</p><p><strong>Public abstract: </strong>While daily life centers on the present, people often reflect on the past and anticipate the future. But which direction of mental time travel-backward or forward-has more influence? We identify three key differences that shape how people engage with the past and future: time flows from past to future, the future is more uncertain, and people have more control over the future. These differences affect the frequency, intensity, and nature of thoughts and feelings, leading to predictable biases in how we mentally represent and emotionally engage with events over time. Because focusing on the future often provides greater benefits, people tend to prioritize prospection over retrospection in everyday life.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":" ","pages":"10888683241292849"},"PeriodicalIF":7.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142755816","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Where Is Capitalism? Unmasking Its Hidden Role in Psychology. 资本主义在哪里?揭开资本主义在心理学中的隐藏角色
IF 7.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-11-16 DOI: 10.1177/10888683241287570
Karim Bettache

Public abstract: Our thoughts, behaviors, and well-being are deeply influenced by the economic system we live in-capitalism. While psychologists have explored capitalist ideologies like neoliberalism, they often overlook capitalism's core foundations driving inequality. This work argues that capitalism, rooted in Western colonial history, generates powerful cultural narratives prioritizing profit, competition, and private ownership. These capitalist principles manifest as pervasive societal mindsets obsessed with personal gain, viewing life as a zero-sum rivalry, and deriving self-worth from possessions. Collectively, they breed an individualistic syndrome of selfish striving at the expense of community. By understanding how these capitalist cultural forces psychologically shape us, maintaining oppressive societal hierarchies, we can reimagine economic systems that truly uplift the human spirit across all peoples and the planet we share. Unveiling capitalism's influence is crucial to recover from its alienating effects and envision liberating alternatives.

公共摘要:我们的思想、行为和福祉深受我们所处的经济体系--资本主义--的影响。虽然心理学家们对新自由主义等资本主义意识形态进行了探讨,但他们往往忽视了资本主义推动不平等的核心基础。本著作认为,植根于西方殖民历史的资本主义产生了强大的文化叙事,将利润、竞争和私有制放在首位。这些资本主义原则表现为普遍存在的社会心态,这些心态痴迷于个人利益,将生活视为零和竞争,并从财产中获得自我价值。总体而言,它们滋生了一种以牺牲社区为代价的自私自利的个人主义综合症。通过了解这些资本主义文化力量如何从心理上塑造我们,维持压迫性的社会等级制度,我们可以重新构想经济体系,真正提升所有民族和我们共同的地球的人类精神。揭示资本主义的影响对于从其异化影响中恢复过来并设想解放的替代方案至关重要。
{"title":"Where Is Capitalism? Unmasking Its Hidden Role in Psychology.","authors":"Karim Bettache","doi":"10.1177/10888683241287570","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10888683241287570","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Public abstract: </strong>Our thoughts, behaviors, and well-being are deeply influenced by the economic system we live in-capitalism. While psychologists have explored capitalist ideologies like neoliberalism, they often overlook capitalism's core foundations driving inequality. This work argues that capitalism, rooted in Western colonial history, generates powerful cultural narratives prioritizing profit, competition, and private ownership. These capitalist principles manifest as pervasive societal mindsets obsessed with personal gain, viewing life as a zero-sum rivalry, and deriving self-worth from possessions. Collectively, they breed an individualistic syndrome of selfish striving at the expense of community. By understanding how these capitalist cultural forces psychologically shape us, maintaining oppressive societal hierarchies, we can reimagine economic systems that truly uplift the human spirit across all peoples and the planet we share. Unveiling capitalism's influence is crucial to recover from its alienating effects and envision liberating alternatives.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":" ","pages":"10888683241287570"},"PeriodicalIF":7.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142644997","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Theoretical Model of Victimization, Perpetration, and Denial in Mass Atrocities: Case Studies From Indonesia, Cambodia, East Timor, and Myanmar. 大规模暴行中的受害、犯罪和否认理论模型:印度尼西亚、柬埔寨、东帝汶和缅甸的案例研究。
IF 7.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-23 DOI: 10.1177/10888683241239097
Idhamsyah Eka Putra, Any Rufaedah, Haidar Buldan Thontowi, Annie Pohlman, Winnifred Louis

Academic abstract: The present article discusses victimization, perpetration, and denial in mass atrocities, using four recent case studies from Southeast Asia. The four cases include Indonesia (in which hundreds of thousands died in anti-Communist violence), Cambodia (in which the Khmer Rouge killed more than one million civilians), East Timor (in which more than one hundred thousand civilians died during the Indonesian occupation), and Myanmar (in which the state/army is accused of genocide toward the Rohingyas). Our aim is to bring a psychological lens to these histories, with a focus on three processes relevant to genocide. We examine, first, how the victims were targeted; second, how the perpetrators were mobilized; and third, the denial, justification, meaning-making, and commemoration of the atrocities. We propose a novel theoretical model, TOPASC: A Theory of the Psychology of Atrocities in Societal Contexts, highlighting the psychology of atrocities as involving factors across the macro, meso, and micro contexts.

Public abstract: We introduce a new model, "TOPASC: A Theory of the Psychology of Atrocities in Societal Contexts," to explain why people justify mass killings and why certain group members are consistently targeted. In our model, we explore how mass atrocities against specific groups are influenced by psychological dynamics in intergroup situations which, in turn, are shaped by socio-historical contexts and individual psychologies. To illustrate these ideas, we analyze four cases of mass atrocities in Southeast Asia: Indonesia, Cambodia, East Timor, and Myanmar. These cases highlight how different social groups, characterized by diverse ideologies, ethnicities, genders, or religions, exhibit varying vulnerabilities as perpetrators or victims based on their social and power status. Mass atrocities are not sudden occurrences but rather result from a series of complex processes and events.

学术摘要:本文利用东南亚最近的四个案例研究,讨论了大规模暴行中的受害、犯罪和否认问题。这四个案例包括印度尼西亚(数十万人死于反共暴力)、柬埔寨(红色高棉杀害了一百多万平民)、东帝汶(十多万平民死于印尼占领期间)和缅甸(国家/军队被指控对罗辛亚人实施种族灭绝)。我们的目的是用心理学的视角来审视这些历史,重点关注与种族灭绝有关的三个过程。首先,我们研究受害者是如何成为目标的;其次,犯罪者是如何被动员起来的;第三,对暴行的否认、辩解、意义塑造和纪念。我们提出了一个新的理论模型--"TOPASC:社会背景下暴行的心理学理论",强调暴行的心理涉及宏观、中观和微观背景下的各种因素。公众摘要:我们提出了一个新的模型--"TOPASC:社会背景下暴行的心理学理论",以解释为什么人们为大规模屠杀辩护,为什么某些群体成员总是成为目标。在我们的模型中,我们探讨了针对特定群体的大规模暴行如何受到群体间心理动态的影响,而群体间心理动态又如何受到社会历史背景和个人心理的影响。为了说明这些观点,我们分析了东南亚的四个大规模暴行案例:印度尼西亚、柬埔寨、东帝汶和缅甸。这些案例凸显了不同的社会群体(具有不同的意识形态、种族、性别或宗教)如何根据其社会和权力地位,表现出作为施暴者或受害者的不同脆弱性。大规模暴行并非突发事件,而是一系列复杂过程和事件的结果。
{"title":"A Theoretical Model of Victimization, Perpetration, and Denial in Mass Atrocities: Case Studies From Indonesia, Cambodia, East Timor, and Myanmar.","authors":"Idhamsyah Eka Putra, Any Rufaedah, Haidar Buldan Thontowi, Annie Pohlman, Winnifred Louis","doi":"10.1177/10888683241239097","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10888683241239097","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Academic abstract: </strong>The present article discusses victimization, perpetration, and denial in mass atrocities, using four recent case studies from Southeast Asia. The four cases include Indonesia (in which hundreds of thousands died in anti-Communist violence), Cambodia (in which the Khmer Rouge killed more than one million civilians), East Timor (in which more than one hundred thousand civilians died during the Indonesian occupation), and Myanmar (in which the state/army is accused of genocide toward the Rohingyas). Our aim is to bring a psychological lens to these histories, with a focus on three processes relevant to genocide. We examine, first, how the victims were targeted; second, how the perpetrators were mobilized; and third, the denial, justification, meaning-making, and commemoration of the atrocities. We propose a novel theoretical model, TOPASC: A Theory of the Psychology of Atrocities in Societal Contexts, highlighting the psychology of atrocities as involving factors across the macro, meso, and micro contexts.</p><p><strong>Public abstract: </strong>We introduce a new model, \"TOPASC: A Theory of the Psychology of Atrocities in Societal Contexts,\" to explain why people justify mass killings and why certain group members are consistently targeted. In our model, we explore how mass atrocities against specific groups are influenced by psychological dynamics in intergroup situations which, in turn, are shaped by socio-historical contexts and individual psychologies. To illustrate these ideas, we analyze four cases of mass atrocities in Southeast Asia: Indonesia, Cambodia, East Timor, and Myanmar. These cases highlight how different social groups, characterized by diverse ideologies, ethnicities, genders, or religions, exhibit varying vulnerabilities as perpetrators or victims based on their social and power status. Mass atrocities are not sudden occurrences but rather result from a series of complex processes and events.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":" ","pages":"398-426"},"PeriodicalIF":7.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140194845","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Being as Having, Loving, and Doing: A Theory of Human Well-Being. 存在即拥有、爱和行动:人类福祉理论》。
IF 7.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-26 DOI: 10.1177/10888683241263634
Frank Martela

Academic abstract: Stronger theory on the nature of human well-being is needed, especially as well-being indicators are increasingly utilized in policy contexts. Building on Erik Allardt, who argued that a theory of well-being is, in essence, a theory of human nature, I propose four modes of existence each capturing one dimension central to human well-being: Having recognizes humans as biological creatures requiring certain material resources for survival. Loving captures human social nature and our dependence on others for well-being. Doing highlights the active and agentic nature of human existence. Being acknowledges humans as experiencing their existence. Each mode of existence gives rise to a few more specific needs, and a full assessment of human well-being requires both subjective and objective indicators tapping into these needs. The proposed theory integrates psychological well-being research with sociological and philosophical traditions and contributes to debates about how the progress of nations and sustainability should be measured.

Public abstract: Well-being is something we all value individually, and it is also a key political goal. Accordingly, how we define and measure well-being influences what physicians, managers, policy-makers, politicians, and international organizations aim to improve through their work. Better theories of well-being make better measurement of well-being possible, which makes possible more effective and evidence-based advancement of human well-being. In this spirit, the present article argues that there are four fundamental dimensions to human well-being: Having highlights that as biological creatures, we have physical needs, loving highlights human social needs, doing highlights that we are active and agentic beings with goals and strivings, and being highlights that we feel and evaluate our lives. To assess well-being, we need measures tapping into all four of these dimensions. And to assess the sustainability of well-being, we need to examine how to provide well-being for all humanity while remaining within planetary boundaries.

学术摘要:我们需要关于人类福祉本质的更强有力的理论,尤其是在福祉指标越来越多地被用于政策制定的情况下。埃里克-阿勒特(Erik Allardt)认为,福祉理论本质上就是人类本性理论,在此基础上,我提出了四种生存模式,每种模式都抓住了人类福祉的一个核心维度:拥有 "承认人类是生物,需要一定的物质资源才能生存。爱 "体现了人类的社会性以及我们对他人的依赖。作为 "强调了人类生存的主动性和能动性。存在 "承认人类在体验自己的存在。每一种存在方式都会产生一些更具体的需求,而对人类福祉的全面评估则需要同时考虑到这些需求的主观和客观指标。所提出的理论将幸福心理学研究与社会学和哲学传统相结合,并对如何衡量国家进步和可持续发展的辩论做出了贡献。公众摘要:幸福是我们每个人都重视的东西,也是一个关键的政治目标。因此,我们如何定义和衡量福祉影响着医生、管理者、政策制定者、政治家和国际组织通过其工作所要改善的目标。有了更好的福祉理论,才有可能对福祉进行更好的衡量,从而更有效、更有依据地促进人类福祉。本着这一精神,本文认为人类福祉有四个基本方面:有 "强调了作为生物,我们有生理需求;"爱 "强调了人类的社会需求;"行 "强调了我们是积极主动的人,有自己的目标和追求;"在 "强调了我们对生活的感受和评价。为了评估幸福感,我们需要对所有这四个方面进行衡量。为了评估福祉的可持续性,我们需要研究如何在不超出地球极限的情况下为全人类提供福祉。
{"title":"Being as Having, Loving, and Doing: A Theory of Human Well-Being.","authors":"Frank Martela","doi":"10.1177/10888683241263634","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10888683241263634","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Academic abstract: </strong>Stronger theory on the nature of human well-being is needed, especially as well-being indicators are increasingly utilized in policy contexts. Building on Erik Allardt, who argued that a theory of well-being is, in essence, a theory of human nature, I propose four modes of existence each capturing one dimension central to human well-being: <i>Having</i> recognizes humans as biological creatures requiring certain material resources for survival. <i>Loving</i> captures human social nature and our dependence on others for well-being. <i>Doing</i> highlights the active and agentic nature of human existence. <i>Being</i> acknowledges humans as experiencing their existence. Each mode of existence gives rise to a few more specific needs, and a full assessment of human well-being requires both subjective and objective indicators tapping into these needs. The proposed theory integrates psychological well-being research with sociological and philosophical traditions and contributes to debates about how the progress of nations and sustainability should be measured.</p><p><strong>Public abstract: </strong>Well-being is something we all value individually, and it is also a key political goal. Accordingly, how we define and measure well-being influences what physicians, managers, policy-makers, politicians, and international organizations aim to improve through their work. Better theories of well-being make better measurement of well-being possible, which makes possible more effective and evidence-based advancement of human well-being. In this spirit, the present article argues that there are four fundamental dimensions to human well-being: <i>Having</i> highlights that as biological creatures, we have physical needs, <i>loving</i> highlights human social needs, <i>doing</i> highlights that we are active and agentic beings with goals and strivings, and <i>being</i> highlights that we feel and evaluate our lives. To assess well-being, we need measures tapping into all four of these dimensions. And to assess the <i>sustainability</i> of well-being, we need to examine how to provide well-being for all humanity while remaining within planetary boundaries.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":" ","pages":"372-397"},"PeriodicalIF":7.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11500488/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141761775","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Intergenerational Storytelling and Positive Psychosocial Development: Stories as Developmental Resources for Marginalized Groups. 代际讲故事与积极的社会心理发展:故事作为边缘群体的发展资源。
IF 7.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-28 DOI: 10.1177/10888683241259902
Nic M Weststrate, Kate C McLean, Robyn Fivush

Academic abstract: We articulate an intergenerational model of positive psychosocial development that centers storytelling in an ecological framework and is motivated by an orientation toward social justice. We bring together diverse literature (e.g., racial-ethnic socialization, family storytelling, narrative psychology) to argue that the intergenerational transmission of stories about one's group is equally important for elders and youth, and especially important for groups who are marginalized, because stories provide a developmental resource for resistance and resilience in the face of injustice. We describe how storytelling activities can support positive psychosocial development in culturally dynamic contexts and illustrate our model with a case study involving LGBTQ+ communities, arguing that intergenerational storytelling is uniquely important for this group given issues of access to stories. We argue that harnessing the power of intergenerational storytelling could provide a culturally safe and sustaining practice for fostering psychosocial development among LGBTQ+ people and other equity-seeking populations.

Public abstract: Understanding one's identity as part of a group with shared history and culture that has existed through time is important for positive psychological functioning. This is especially true for marginalized communities for whom identity-relevant knowledge is often erased, silenced, or distorted in mainstream public discourses (e.g., school curricula, news media, television, and film). To compensate for these limitations around access, one channel for the transmission of this knowledge is through oral storytelling between generations of elders and youth. Contemporary psychological science has often assumed that such storytelling occurs within families, but when families cannot or would not share such knowledge, youth suffer. We present a model of intergenerational storytelling that expands our ideas around who counts as "family" and how knowledge can be transmitted through alternative channels, using LGBTQ+ communities as a case example.

学术摘要:我们阐述了一种积极社会心理发展的代际模式,该模式以生态框架中的讲故事为中心,以社会正义为导向。我们汇集了各种文献(如种族-民族社会化、家庭讲故事、叙事心理学等),论证了关于本群体故事的代际传播对长辈和青少年同等重要,对边缘群体尤为重要,因为故事为面对不公正时的抵抗力和复原力提供了发展资源。我们描述了讲故事活动如何在文化动态背景下支持积极的社会心理发展,并通过一个涉及 LGBTQ+ 群体的案例研究来说明我们的模式,认为鉴于获取故事的途径问题,代际讲故事对这一群体具有独特的重要性。我们认为,利用代际讲故事的力量,可以为促进 LGBTQ+ 和其他追求公平的人群的社会心理发展提供一种文化上安全且可持续的做法。公众摘要:了解自己作为一个具有共同历史和文化的群体的一员的身份对于积极的心理功能非常重要。对于边缘化群体来说尤其如此,因为在主流公共话语(如学校课程、新闻媒体、电视和电影)中,与身份相关的知识往往被抹杀、压制或扭曲。为了弥补这些获取方面的限制,长辈和年轻人之间世代相传的口头故事是传播这些知识的渠道之一。当代心理科学通常假定这种讲故事的方式发生在家庭内部,但当家庭不能或不愿分享这种知识时,青少年就会受到影响。我们以 LGBTQ+ 社区为例,介绍了一种代际讲故事的模式,它拓展了我们对谁算作 "家庭 "以及如何通过其他渠道传播知识的看法。
{"title":"Intergenerational Storytelling and Positive Psychosocial Development: Stories as Developmental Resources for Marginalized Groups.","authors":"Nic M Weststrate, Kate C McLean, Robyn Fivush","doi":"10.1177/10888683241259902","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10888683241259902","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Academic abstract: </strong>We articulate an intergenerational model of positive psychosocial development that centers storytelling in an ecological framework and is motivated by an orientation toward social justice. We bring together diverse literature (e.g., racial-ethnic socialization, family storytelling, narrative psychology) to argue that the intergenerational transmission of stories about one's group is <i>equally</i> important for elders and youth, and <i>especially</i> important for groups who are marginalized, because stories provide a developmental resource for resistance and resilience in the face of injustice. We describe how storytelling activities can support positive psychosocial development in culturally dynamic contexts and illustrate our model with a case study involving LGBTQ+ communities, arguing that intergenerational storytelling is <i>uniquely</i> important for this group given issues of access to stories. We argue that harnessing the power of intergenerational storytelling could provide a culturally safe and sustaining practice for fostering psychosocial development among LGBTQ+ people and other equity-seeking populations.</p><p><strong>Public abstract: </strong>Understanding one's identity as part of a group with shared history and culture that has existed through time is important for positive psychological functioning. This is especially true for marginalized communities for whom identity-relevant knowledge is often erased, silenced, or distorted in mainstream public discourses (e.g., school curricula, news media, television, and film). To compensate for these limitations around access, one channel for the transmission of this knowledge is through oral storytelling between generations of elders and youth. Contemporary psychological science has often assumed that such storytelling occurs within families, but when families cannot or would not share such knowledge, youth suffer. We present a model of intergenerational storytelling that expands our ideas around who counts as \"family\" and how knowledge can be transmitted through alternative channels, using LGBTQ+ communities as a case example.</p>","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":" ","pages":"351-371"},"PeriodicalIF":7.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141789532","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
“My Aim Is True”: An Attribution-Identity Model of Ally Sincerity "我的目标是真实的盟友诚意的归因-认同模型
IF 10.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-09-17 DOI: 10.1177/10888683241273354
Charlotte E. Moser, Shaun Wiley
Academic AbstractAdvantaged group allies have multiple motives for supporting equality, raising questions about their sincerity. We draw upon the covariation model of attributions to explain how disadvantaged group members make attributions about whether advantaged group “allies” are sincerely motivated to empower the disadvantaged group. We propose an Attribution-Identity Model of Sincerity (AIMS) which posits that disadvantaged group members view advantaged group members as sincere allies when they support equality in the presence of inhibitory causes and in the absence of facilitative causes, exceed expectations for the advantaged group, and provide support across time and contexts. Furthermore, those who identify strongly with their disadvantaged group and perceive intergroup inequality as illegitimate are most motivated to ascertain the sincerity of advantaged group members’ allyship. AIMS suggests how members of disadvantaged groups seek to maximize benefits and minimize risks of advantaged group members’ allyship.Public AbstractAdvantaged group members (e.g., men, White Americans) can act as allies for disadvantaged groups (e.g., women, Americans belonging to minoritized racial groups), but members of disadvantaged groups sometimes have reason to question whether their motives are sincere. We argue that members of disadvantaged groups view advantaged group allies as more sincere when they support equality when they do not stand to benefit from it and even when they stand to lose. We also argue that members of disadvantaged groups view advantaged group allies as more sincere when their support for equality goes beyond expectations for their advantaged group, consistently over time, and is not limited to particular situations, forms, or contexts. Members of disadvantaged groups like sincere allies, want to work with them, and feel safe around them. Sincere allies also serve as moral exemplars to other members of advantaged groups.
学术论文摘要 弱势群体盟友支持平等的动机多种多样,这让人怀疑他们的诚意。我们借鉴归因的共变模型来解释弱势群体成员如何对优势群体 "盟友 "是否真心支持弱势群体进行归因。我们提出了 "真诚归因-认同模型"(AIMS),该模型假定,当优势群体成员在存在抑制性原因或不存在促进性原因的情况下支持平等、超出对优势群体的期望并在不同时间和背景下提供支持时,弱势群体成员会将优势群体成员视为真诚的盟友。此外,那些强烈认同其弱势群体并认为群体间不平等是非法的人,最有动力去确定优势群体成员结盟的诚意。AIMS 建议弱势群体成员如何寻求优势群体成员盟友关系的利益最大化和风险最小化。公开摘要优势群体成员(如男性、美国白人)可以充当弱势群体(如女性、属于少数种族群体的美国人)的盟友,但弱势群体成员有时有理由质疑他们的动机是否真诚。我们认为,当优势群体的盟友支持平等时,弱势群体的成员会认为优势群体的盟友更有诚意,因为他们不会从中获益,甚至会蒙受损失。我们还认为,如果优势群体盟友对平等的支持超出了他们对优势群体的期望,并且长期持续,不局限于特定的情况、形式或背景,那么弱势群体成员就会认为优势群体盟友更真诚。弱势群体成员喜欢真诚的盟友,愿意与他们合作,在他们身边感到安全。真诚的盟友也是其他优势群体成员的道德楷模。
{"title":"“My Aim Is True”: An Attribution-Identity Model of Ally Sincerity","authors":"Charlotte E. Moser, Shaun Wiley","doi":"10.1177/10888683241273354","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10888683241273354","url":null,"abstract":"Academic AbstractAdvantaged group allies have multiple motives for supporting equality, raising questions about their sincerity. We draw upon the covariation model of attributions to explain how disadvantaged group members make attributions about whether advantaged group “allies” are sincerely motivated to empower the disadvantaged group. We propose an Attribution-Identity Model of Sincerity (AIMS) which posits that disadvantaged group members view advantaged group members as sincere allies when they support equality in the presence of inhibitory causes and in the absence of facilitative causes, exceed expectations for the advantaged group, and provide support across time and contexts. Furthermore, those who identify strongly with their disadvantaged group and perceive intergroup inequality as illegitimate are most motivated to ascertain the sincerity of advantaged group members’ allyship. AIMS suggests how members of disadvantaged groups seek to maximize benefits and minimize risks of advantaged group members’ allyship.Public AbstractAdvantaged group members (e.g., men, White Americans) can act as allies for disadvantaged groups (e.g., women, Americans belonging to minoritized racial groups), but members of disadvantaged groups sometimes have reason to question whether their motives are sincere. We argue that members of disadvantaged groups view advantaged group allies as more sincere when they support equality when they do not stand to benefit from it and even when they stand to lose. We also argue that members of disadvantaged groups view advantaged group allies as more sincere when their support for equality goes beyond expectations for their advantaged group, consistently over time, and is not limited to particular situations, forms, or contexts. Members of disadvantaged groups like sincere allies, want to work with them, and feel safe around them. Sincere allies also serve as moral exemplars to other members of advantaged groups.","PeriodicalId":48386,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Review","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":10.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142235126","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Personality and Social Psychology Review
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1