首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Neural correlates of unconventional verb extensions reveal preschoolers’ analogical abilities 非传统动词扩展的神经相关性揭示学龄前儿童的类比能力
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105984
Lucas Raynal , Evelyne Clément , Louise Goyet , Pia Rämä , Emmanuel Sander

In the current event-related potential (ERP) study, we assessed 4-year-olds’ ability to extend verbs to new action events on the basis of abstract similarities. Participants were presented with images of actions (e.g., peeling an orange) while hearing sentences containing a conventional verb (e.g., peeling), a verb sharing an abstract relation (i.e., an analogical verb, e.g., undressing), a verb sharing an object type (i.e., an object-related verb, e.g., pressing) with the action, or a pseudoverb (e.g., kebraying). The amplitude of the N400 gradually increased as a function of verb type—from conventional verbs to analogical verbs to object-related verbs to pseudoverbs. These findings suggest that accessing the meaning of a verb is easier when it shares abstract relations with the expected verb. Our results illustrate that measuring brain signals in response to analogical word extensions provides a useful tool to investigate preschools’ analogical abilities.

在本次事件相关电位(ERP)研究中,我们评估了 4 岁儿童根据抽象相似性将动词扩展到新动作事件的能力。我们向受试者展示了动作图像(如剥橘子),同时让受试者听到包含传统动词(如剥皮)、与动作具有抽象关系的动词(即类比动词,如脱衣服)、与动作具有对象类型的动词(即与对象相关的动词,如按压)或假动词(如kebraying)的句子。N400的振幅随着动词类型的变化而逐渐增大--从传统动词到类比动词,再到与对象相关的动词,最后到伪动词。这些研究结果表明,当动词与预期动词具有相同的抽象关系时,更容易获取动词的意义。我们的研究结果表明,测量大脑对类比词扩展的反应信号为研究学龄前儿童的类比能力提供了有用的工具。
{"title":"Neural correlates of unconventional verb extensions reveal preschoolers’ analogical abilities","authors":"Lucas Raynal ,&nbsp;Evelyne Clément ,&nbsp;Louise Goyet ,&nbsp;Pia Rämä ,&nbsp;Emmanuel Sander","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105984","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105984","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In the current event-related potential (ERP) study, we assessed 4-year-olds’ ability to extend verbs to new action events on the basis of abstract similarities. Participants were presented with images of actions (e.g., peeling an orange) while hearing sentences containing a conventional verb (e.g., peeling), a verb sharing an abstract relation (i.e., an analogical verb, e.g., undressing), a verb sharing an object type (i.e., an object-related verb, e.g., pressing) with the action, or a pseudoverb (e.g., kebraying). The amplitude of the N400 gradually increased as a function of verb type—from conventional verbs to analogical verbs to object-related verbs to pseudoverbs. These findings suggest that accessing the meaning of a verb is easier when it shares abstract relations with the expected verb. Our results illustrate that measuring brain signals in response to analogical word extensions provides a useful tool to investigate preschools’ analogical abilities.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022096524001243/pdfft?md5=1dc106257ebeda14364f3b1dacd12eb6&pid=1-s2.0-S0022096524001243-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141328806","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of choice on memory across development 选择对不同发育阶段记忆的影响
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105982
Zhuolei Ding , Wenqing Li , Chuansheng Chen , Zhong Yang , Songxue Wang , Juanjuan Xu , Xun Liu , Mingxia Zhang

Numerous studies have demonstrated the role of making choices as an internal motivator to improve performance, and recent studies in the domain of memory have focused on adults. To chart the developmental trend of the choice effect on memory, we conducted a series of seven experiments involving children, adolescents, and young adults. Participants (N = 512) aged 5 to 26 years performed a choice encoding task that manipulated the opportunities to choose and then took a memory test. Using different types of experimental materials and corroborated by a mini meta-analysis, we found that the choice effect on memory was significant in childhood and early adolescence but not significant in late adolescence and early adulthood. The developmental changes were statistically significant, particularly evident during the transition from early to late adolescence. These findings suggest that the internal value of choice decreases across development and contributes to our understanding of developmental differences in the role of choice in memory.

许多研究已经证明,做出选择是提高成绩的内在动力,而最近在记忆领域的研究主要集中在成年人身上。为了描绘选择对记忆的影响的发展趋向,我们进行了一系列共七项实验,参与者包括儿童、青少年和年轻成年人。年龄在 5 到 26 岁之间的参与者(512 人)进行了一项选择编码任务,该任务操纵选择机会,然后进行记忆测试。通过使用不同类型的实验材料和小型荟萃分析,我们发现选择对记忆的影响在儿童期和青春期早期显著,但在青春期晚期和成年早期并不显著。这种发展变化在统计学上是显著的,尤其是在从青春期早期向晚期过渡的过程中。这些研究结果表明,选择的内部价值在整个发育过程中会降低,这有助于我们理解选择在记忆中的作用的发育差异。
{"title":"The effect of choice on memory across development","authors":"Zhuolei Ding ,&nbsp;Wenqing Li ,&nbsp;Chuansheng Chen ,&nbsp;Zhong Yang ,&nbsp;Songxue Wang ,&nbsp;Juanjuan Xu ,&nbsp;Xun Liu ,&nbsp;Mingxia Zhang","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105982","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105982","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Numerous studies have demonstrated the role of making choices as an internal motivator to improve performance, and recent studies in the domain of memory have focused on adults. To chart the developmental trend of the choice effect on memory, we conducted a series of seven experiments involving children, adolescents, and young adults. Participants (<em>N</em> = 512) aged 5 to 26 years performed a choice encoding task that manipulated the opportunities to choose and then took a memory test. Using different types of experimental materials and corroborated by a mini meta-analysis, we found that the choice effect on memory was significant in childhood and early adolescence but not significant in late adolescence and early adulthood. The developmental changes were statistically significant, particularly evident during the transition from early to late adolescence. These findings suggest that the internal value of choice decreases across development and contributes to our understanding of developmental differences in the role of choice in memory.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141328805","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Early language processing efficiency and pre-literacy outcomes in children born full term and preterm 足月儿和早产儿的早期语言处理效率和识字前成果。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-12 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105980
Virginia A. Marchman , Melanie D. Ashland , Elizabeth C. Loi , Mónica Munévar , Katherine A. Shannon , Anne Fernald , Heidi M. Feldman

Language processing efficiency—that is, the skill at processing language in real time—assessed in toddlerhood is associated with later language outcomes in children born full term (FT) and preterm (PT) during school age. No studies to date have assessed patterns of relations between early language processing efficiency and pre-literacy skills, such as print knowledge and phonological awareness, and whether relations are similar in FT and PT children. In this study, participants (N = 94, 49 FT and 45PT) were assessed in the looking-while-listening (LWL) task at 18 months of age (corrected for degree of prematurity), deriving measures of processing speed and accuracy. At 4½ years of age, children were assessed on standardized tests of print knowledge, phonological awareness, and expressive language. Processing speed and accuracy predicted both pre-literacy outcomes (r2 change = 7.8%–19.5%, p < .01); birth group did not moderate these effects. Relations were significantly reduced when controlling for expressive language. Thus, early language processing efficiency supports later expressive language abilities, which in turn supports developing pre-literacy skills. Processing speed and phonological awareness were also directly related, indicating an independent role for processing speed in literacy development. Mediation effects were not moderated by birth group, suggesting a similar developmental pathway in FT and PT children.

幼儿期评估的语言处理效率--即实时处理语言的技能--与足月儿(FT)和早产儿(PT)学龄期的语言成果有关。迄今为止,还没有研究评估过早期语言处理效率与识字前技能(如印刷知识和语音意识)之间的关系模式,以及足月儿和早产儿之间的关系是否相似。本研究对 18 个月大(按早产程度校正)的参与者(94 人,其中 49 人为 FT 儿童,45 人为 PT 儿童)进行了边听边看(LWL)任务的评估,得出了处理速度和准确性的测量结果。4 岁半时,对儿童进行印刷知识、语音意识和语言表达能力的标准化测试。处理速度和准确性可预测识字前的结果(r2 变化 = 7.8%-19.5%, p
{"title":"Early language processing efficiency and pre-literacy outcomes in children born full term and preterm","authors":"Virginia A. Marchman ,&nbsp;Melanie D. Ashland ,&nbsp;Elizabeth C. Loi ,&nbsp;Mónica Munévar ,&nbsp;Katherine A. Shannon ,&nbsp;Anne Fernald ,&nbsp;Heidi M. Feldman","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105980","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105980","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Language processing efficiency—that is, the skill at processing language in real time—assessed in toddlerhood is associated with later language outcomes in children born full term (FT) and preterm (PT) during school age. No studies to date have assessed patterns of relations between early language processing efficiency and pre-literacy skills, such as print knowledge and phonological awareness, and whether relations are similar in FT and PT children. In this study, participants (<em>N</em> = 94, 49 FT and 45PT) were assessed in the looking-while-listening (LWL) task at 18 months of age (corrected for degree of prematurity), deriving measures of processing speed and accuracy. At 4½ years of age, children were assessed on standardized tests of print knowledge, phonological awareness, and expressive language. Processing speed and accuracy predicted both pre-literacy outcomes (<em>r</em><sup>2</sup> change = 7.8%–19.5%, <em>p</em> &lt; .01); birth group did not moderate these effects. Relations were significantly reduced when controlling for expressive language. Thus, early language processing efficiency supports later expressive language abilities, which in turn supports developing pre-literacy skills. Processing speed and phonological awareness were also directly related, indicating an independent role for processing speed in literacy development. Mediation effects were not moderated by birth group, suggesting a similar developmental pathway in FT and PT children.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141312039","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Shyness and risk-taking during peer observation in children and adolescents 儿童和青少年在同伴观察中的害羞心理和冒险行为
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-10 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105981
Kristie L. Poole, Teena Willoughby

Although temperamental shyness is conceptualized as a trait marked by cautiousness, we know relatively little about its relation to risk-taking. We examined how shyness was related to opportunities for risk-taking while considering how social context (i.e., presence of peers) and developmental stage (i.e., children and adolescents) might influence this relation. In the current study, 198 children (Mage = 10.17 years) and 221 adolescents (Mage = 13.46 years) completed the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) alone or during a peer observation manipulation. For children and adolescents, shyness was related to physiological arousal and self-reporting feeling anxious during the peer condition. However, peer observation did not influence the relation between shyness and behavioral responses during the BART. Across both alone and peer conditions, shyness was related to a longer response time for children and adolescents, which may reflect decisional conflict during risk-taking opportunities. Furthermore, shyness in children (but not in adolescents) was related to poorer performance (i.e., fewer points), whereas shyness was unrelated to risk-taking propensity (i.e., number of pumps) for both children and adolescents. Overall, although the presence of peers may induce anxiety during a risk-taking opportunity for children and adolescents higher in shyness, this does not appear to modify their risk-taking behaviors. Instead, shyer children and adolescents in general may take a longer time to decide whether to act in a risky manner, whereas shy children in particular may show poorer performance in obtaining a reward on a risk-taking task.

虽然害羞被认为是一种以谨慎为特征的性格,但我们对它与冒险的关系却知之甚少。我们研究了害羞与冒险机会之间的关系,同时考虑了社会环境(即同伴的存在)和发展阶段(即儿童和青少年)如何影响这种关系。在本研究中,198 名儿童(年龄=10.17 岁)和 221 名青少年(年龄=13.46 岁)单独或在同伴观察操作中完成了气球模拟风险任务(BART)。对于儿童和青少年来说,害羞与生理唤醒和同伴条件下的自我报告焦虑有关。然而,同伴观察并不影响害羞与 BART 中行为反应之间的关系。在单独和同伴条件下,儿童和青少年的害羞与较长的反应时间有关,这可能反映了冒险机会中的决策冲突。此外,儿童(而非青少年)的害羞与较差的表现(即较少的分数)有关,而儿童和青少年的害羞与冒险倾向(即抽水次数)无关。总之,虽然同伴的存在可能会让害羞程度较高的儿童和青少年在冒险过程中产生焦虑,但这似乎并不会改变他们的冒险行为。相反,一般来说,害羞的儿童和青少年可能需要更长的时间来决定是否以冒险的方式行事,而害羞的儿童在冒险任务中获得奖励的表现可能会更差。
{"title":"Shyness and risk-taking during peer observation in children and adolescents","authors":"Kristie L. Poole,&nbsp;Teena Willoughby","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105981","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105981","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Although temperamental shyness is conceptualized as a trait marked by cautiousness, we know relatively little about its relation to risk-taking. We examined how shyness was related to opportunities for risk-taking while considering how social context (i.e., presence of peers) and developmental stage (i.e., children and adolescents) might influence this relation. In the current study, 198 children (<em>M</em><sub>age</sub> = 10.17 years) and 221 adolescents (<em>M</em><sub>age</sub> = 13.46 years) completed the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) alone or during a peer observation manipulation. For children and adolescents, shyness was related to physiological arousal and self-reporting feeling anxious during the peer condition. However, peer observation did not influence the relation between shyness and behavioral responses during the BART. Across both alone and peer conditions, shyness was related to a longer response time for children and adolescents, which may reflect decisional conflict during risk-taking opportunities. Furthermore, shyness in children (but not in adolescents) was related to poorer performance (i.e., fewer points), whereas shyness was unrelated to risk-taking propensity (i.e., number of pumps) for both children and adolescents. Overall, although the presence of peers may induce anxiety during a risk-taking opportunity for children and adolescents higher in shyness, this does not appear to modify their risk-taking behaviors. Instead, shyer children and adolescents in general may take a longer time to decide whether to act in a risky manner, whereas shy children in particular may show poorer performance in obtaining a reward on a risk-taking task.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141303812","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A cross-cultural comparison of finger-based and symbolic number representations 基于手指的数字表示法和符号数字表示法的跨文化比较
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-10 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105979
Martin H. Fischer , Arianna Felisatti , Xin Li , Samuel Shaki

The current study examined predictions from embodied cognition for effects of finger counting on number processing. Although finger counting is spontaneous and nearly universal, counting habits reflect learning and culture. European cultures use a sub-base-five system, requiring a full hand plus additional fingers to express numbers exceeding 5. Chinese culture requires only one hand to express such numbers. We investigated the differential impact of early-acquired finger-based number representations on adult symbolic number processing. In total, 53 European and 56 Chinese adults performed two versions of the magnitude classification task, where numbers were presented either as Arabic symbols or as finger configurations consistent with respective cultural finger-counting habits. Participants classified numbers as smaller/larger than 5 with horizontally aligned buttons. Finger-based size and distance effects were larger in Chinese compared with Europeans. These differences did not, however, induce reliably different symbol processing signatures. This dissociation challenges the idea that sensory and motor habits shape our conceptual representations and implies notation-specific processing patterns.

本研究考察了体现认知对数数对数字处理影响的预测。虽然数手指是一种自发的、几乎普遍的行为,但数手指的习惯反映了学习和文化。欧洲文化使用的是5以下系统,需要用一只手加上其他手指来表示超过5的数字。而中国文化则只需要一只手来表达这些数字。我们研究了早期获得的基于手指的数字表征对成人符号数字处理的不同影响。共有 53 名欧洲成年人和 56 名中国成年人完成了两个版本的大小分类任务,其中数字以阿拉伯符号或符合各自文化中手指计数习惯的手指构型呈现。受试者用水平排列的按钮将数字分为小于/大于 5。与欧洲人相比,中国人的手指大小和距离效应更大。然而,这些差异并没有引起可靠的不同符号加工特征。这种差异挑战了感觉和运动习惯塑造我们概念表征的观点,并暗示了特定符号的处理模式。
{"title":"A cross-cultural comparison of finger-based and symbolic number representations","authors":"Martin H. Fischer ,&nbsp;Arianna Felisatti ,&nbsp;Xin Li ,&nbsp;Samuel Shaki","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105979","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105979","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The current study examined predictions from embodied cognition for effects of finger counting on number processing. Although finger counting is spontaneous and nearly universal, counting habits reflect learning and culture. European cultures use a sub-base-five system, requiring a full hand plus additional fingers to express numbers exceeding 5. Chinese culture requires only one hand to express such numbers. We investigated the differential impact of early-acquired finger-based number representations on adult symbolic number processing. In total, 53 European and 56 Chinese adults performed two versions of the magnitude classification task, where numbers were presented either as Arabic symbols or as finger configurations consistent with respective cultural finger-counting habits. Participants classified numbers as smaller/larger than 5 with horizontally aligned buttons. Finger-based size and distance effects were larger in Chinese compared with Europeans. These differences did not, however, induce reliably different symbol processing signatures. This dissociation challenges the idea that sensory and motor habits shape our conceptual representations and implies notation-specific processing patterns.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141303811","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Monitoring and control processes within executive functions: Is post-error slowing related to pre-error speeding in children? 执行功能中的监测和控制过程:儿童犯错后速度减慢与犯错前速度加快有关吗?
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105975
Ebru Ger, Claudia M. Roebers

Both pre-error speeding and post-error slowing reflect monitoring and control strategies. Post-error slowing is relatively well-established in children, whereas pre-error speeding is much less studied. Here we investigated (a) whether kindergarten and first-grade children show pre-error speeding in a cognitive control task (Hearts and Flowers) and, if so, (b) whether post-error slowing is associated with pre-error speeding. We analyzed the data from 153 kindergartners and 468 first-graders. Both kindergartners and first-graders showed significant pre-error speeding and post-error slowing, with no differences between the two samples in the magnitude of each. The magnitude of pre-error speeding and post-error slowing was correlated within individuals in both samples and to a similar extent. That is, children who sped up more extremely toward an error also slowed down more extremely after an error. These findings provide evidence that pre-error speeding and post-error slowing are related in children as early as kindergarten age and may in concert reflect how optimal children’s monitoring and control of their performance is in a cognitive control task.

出错前超速和出错后减速都反映了监测和控制策略。在儿童中,错误后减速的研究相对成熟,而错误前加速的研究则要少得多。在此,我们研究了(a) 幼儿园和一年级儿童在认知控制任务(心花怒放)中是否表现出错前超速,如果是,(b) 错后超速是否与错前超速有关。我们分析了 153 名幼儿园学生和 468 名一年级学生的数据。幼儿园学生和一年级学生都出现了明显的错误前超速和错误后减速现象,两个样本之间的差异并不明显。在两个样本中,错误前加速和错误后减速的程度在个体内部存在相关性,且程度相似。也就是说,出错前速度加快的儿童出错后速度也会减慢。这些研究结果证明,早在幼儿园年龄段的儿童中,出错前加速和出错后减速就已经存在关联,并且可能共同反映了儿童在认知控制任务中对其表现的监测和控制的最佳程度。
{"title":"Monitoring and control processes within executive functions: Is post-error slowing related to pre-error speeding in children?","authors":"Ebru Ger,&nbsp;Claudia M. Roebers","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105975","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105975","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Both pre-error speeding and post-error slowing reflect monitoring and control strategies. Post-error slowing is relatively well-established in children, whereas pre-error speeding is much less studied. Here we investigated (a) whether kindergarten and first-grade children show pre-error speeding in a cognitive control task (Hearts and Flowers) and, if so, (b) whether post-error slowing is associated with pre-error speeding. We analyzed the data from 153 kindergartners and 468 first-graders. Both kindergartners and first-graders showed significant pre-error speeding and post-error slowing, with no differences between the two samples in the magnitude of each. The magnitude of pre-error speeding and post-error slowing was correlated within individuals in both samples and to a similar extent. That is, children who sped up more extremely toward an error also slowed down more extremely after an error. These findings provide evidence that pre-error speeding and post-error slowing are related in children as early as kindergarten age and may in concert reflect how optimal children’s monitoring and control of their performance is in a cognitive control task.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022096524001152/pdfft?md5=7b59095de2176f8ff0ec808a42c59c36&pid=1-s2.0-S0022096524001152-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141297069","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Imagining the future improves saving in preschoolers 想象未来能提高学龄前儿童的储蓄能力。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105966
Jinyi Zhang , Kathleen D. Vohs , Stephanie M. Carlson

Preschoolers are notoriously poor at delaying gratification and saving limited resources, yet evidence-based methods of improving these behaviors are lacking. Using the marble game saving paradigm, we examined whether young children’s saving behavior would increase as a result of engaging in future-oriented imagination using a storyboard. Participants were 115 typically developing 4-year-olds from a midwestern U.S. metropolitan area (Mage = 53.48 months, SD = 4.14, range = 47–60; 54.8% female; 84.5% White; 7.3% Hispanic/Latino ethnicity; median annual household income = $150,000–$174,999). Children were randomly assigned to one of four storyboard conditions prior to the marble game: Positive Future Simulation, Negative Future Simulation, Positive Routine, or Negative Routine. In each condition, children were asked to imagine how they would feel in the future situation using a smiley face rating scale. Results showed that children were significantly more likely to save (and to save more marbles) in the experimental conditions compared with the control conditions (medium effect sizes). Moreover, imagining saving for the future (and how good that would feel) was more effective at increasing saving behaviors than imagining not saving (and how bad that would feel). Emotion ratings were consistent with the assigned condition, but positive emotion alone did not account for these effects. Results held after accounting for game order and verbal IQ. Implications of temporal psychological distancing and emotion anticipation for children’s future-oriented decision making are discussed.

众所周知,学龄前儿童不善于延迟满足和节约有限的资源,但却缺乏改善这些行为的循证方法。我们使用弹珠游戏储蓄范例,研究了幼儿的储蓄行为是否会因为使用故事板进行面向未来的想象而增加。参与者是来自美国中西部都市地区的 115 名发育典型的 4 岁儿童(年龄 = 53.48 个月,SD = 4.14,范围 = 47-60;54.8% 为女性;84.5% 为白人;7.3% 为西班牙/拉丁美洲裔;家庭年收入中位数 = 150,000 美元-174,999 美元)。在弹珠游戏之前,孩子们被随机分配到四个故事板条件之一:积极未来模拟"、"消极未来模拟"、"积极常规 "或 "消极常规"。在每种情况下,儿童都被要求使用笑脸评分表想象他们在未来情况下的感受。结果显示,与对照组相比,实验组儿童储蓄的可能性明显更高(而且储蓄的弹珠更多)(中等效应大小)。此外,想象为未来储蓄(以及这种感觉有多好)比想象不储蓄(以及这种感觉有多糟)更能有效地增加储蓄行为。情绪评级与指定条件一致,但仅有积极情绪并不能解释这些效果。在考虑了游戏顺序和言语智商后,结果仍然成立。本文讨论了时间心理距离和情绪预期对儿童面向未来决策的影响。
{"title":"Imagining the future improves saving in preschoolers","authors":"Jinyi Zhang ,&nbsp;Kathleen D. Vohs ,&nbsp;Stephanie M. Carlson","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105966","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105966","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Preschoolers are notoriously poor at delaying gratification and saving limited resources, yet evidence-based methods of improving these behaviors are lacking. Using the marble game saving paradigm, we examined whether young children’s saving behavior would increase as a result of engaging in future-oriented imagination using a storyboard. Participants were 115 typically developing 4-year-olds from a midwestern U.S. metropolitan area (<em>M</em><sub>age</sub> = 53.48 months, <em>SD</em> = 4.14, range = 47–60; 54.8% female; 84.5% White; 7.3% Hispanic/Latino ethnicity; median annual household income = $150,000–$174,999). Children were randomly assigned to one of four storyboard conditions prior to the marble game: Positive Future Simulation, Negative Future Simulation, Positive Routine, or Negative Routine. In each condition, children were asked to imagine how they would feel in the future situation using a smiley face rating scale. Results showed that children were significantly more likely to save (and to save more marbles) in the experimental conditions compared with the control conditions (medium effect sizes). Moreover, imagining saving for the future (and how good that would feel) was more effective at increasing saving behaviors than imagining not saving (and how bad that would feel). Emotion ratings were consistent with the assigned condition, but positive emotion alone did not account for these effects. Results held after accounting for game order and verbal IQ. Implications of temporal psychological distancing and emotion anticipation for children’s future-oriented decision making are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141297068","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When is it appropriate to ask a question? The role of age, social context, and personality 什么时候提问合适?年龄、社会环境和个性的作用。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105976
Ashley Ransom , Azzurra Ruggeri , Samuel Ronfard

How do children decide when it is appropriate to ask a question? In Study 1 (preregistered), 50 4- and 5-year-olds, 50 7- and 8-year-olds, and 100 adults watched vignettes featuring a child who had a question, and participants indicated whether they thought the child should ask the question “right now.” Both adults and children endorsed more question-asking to a well-known informant than to an acquaintance and to someone doing nothing than to someone busy working or busy socializing. However, younger children endorsed asking questions to someone who was busy more often than older children and adults. In addition, Big Five personality traits predicted endorsement of question-asking. In Study 2 (preregistered, N = 500), mothers’ self-reports showed that children’s actual question-asking varied with age, informant activity, and informant familiarity in ways that paralleled the results of Study 1. In Study 3 (N = 100), we examined mothers’ responses to their children’s question-asking and found that mothers’ responses to their children’s question-asking varied based on the mother’s activity. In addition, mothers high in authoritarianism were less likely to answer their children’s questions when they were busy than mothers low in authoritarianism. In sum, across three studies, we found evidence that the age-related decline in children’s question-asking to their parents reflects a change in children’s reasoning about when it is appropriate to ask a question.

儿童如何决定何时提问合适?在研究 1(预先注册)中,50 名 4-5 岁儿童、50 名 7-8 岁儿童和 100 名成人观看了一个儿童提问的小故事,参与者就他们是否认为该儿童应该 "现在 "提问作出回答。成人和儿童都赞同向熟人提问多于向熟人提问,向无所事事的人提问多于向忙于工作或忙于社交的人提问。不过,与年龄较大的儿童和成人相比,年龄较小的儿童更倾向于向忙碌的人提问。此外,大五人格特质也会影响对提问的认可度。在研究 2(预先注册,N = 500)中,母亲的自我报告显示,儿童实际提问的情况随年龄、信息提供者的活动和信息提供者的熟悉程度而变化,这与研究 1 的结果相同。在研究 3(N = 100)中,我们考察了母亲对孩子提问的反应,发现母亲对孩子提问的反应因母亲的活动而异。此外,与专制程度低的母亲相比,专制程度高的母亲在忙碌时较少回答孩子的问题。总之,在三项研究中,我们发现有证据表明,儿童向父母提问的次数随年龄增长而减少,这反映了儿童对何时提问合适的推理发生了变化。
{"title":"When is it appropriate to ask a question? The role of age, social context, and personality","authors":"Ashley Ransom ,&nbsp;Azzurra Ruggeri ,&nbsp;Samuel Ronfard","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105976","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105976","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>How do children decide when it is appropriate to ask a question? In Study 1 (preregistered), 50 4- and 5-year-olds, 50 7- and 8-year-olds, and 100 adults watched vignettes featuring a child who had a question, and participants indicated whether they thought the child should ask the question “right now.” Both adults and children endorsed more question-asking to a well-known informant than to an acquaintance and to someone doing nothing than to someone busy working or busy socializing. However, younger children endorsed asking questions to someone who was busy more often than older children and adults. In addition, Big Five personality traits predicted endorsement of question-asking. In Study 2 (preregistered, <em>N</em> = 500), mothers’ self-reports showed that children’s actual question-asking varied with age, informant activity, and informant familiarity in ways that paralleled the results of Study 1. In Study 3 (<em>N</em> = 100), we examined mothers’ responses to their children’s question-asking and found that mothers’ responses to their children’s question-asking varied based on the mother’s activity. In addition, mothers high in authoritarianism were less likely to answer their children’s questions when they were busy than mothers low in authoritarianism. In sum, across three studies, we found evidence that the age-related decline in children’s question-asking to their parents reflects a change in children’s reasoning about when it is appropriate to ask a question.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141201106","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mind the suffix: Pseudoword processing in children and adults 注意后缀:儿童和成人的假词处理。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105977
Miguel Lázaro , Teresa Simón , Ainoa Escalonilla , Trinidad Ruiz

Previous evidence has shown that pseudowords made up of real morphemes take more time to process and generate more errors than pseudowords without morphemes in a lexical decision task. The explanation for these results is controversial because two possible arguments may be posited; the first is related to the morphological composition of the stimuli, and the second is related to the larger semantic interpretability of pseudowords with morphemes in comparison with pseudowords without morphemes (a semantic-based explanation). To disentangle this issue, we conducted an experiment with 92 children and 42 adults. For this purpose, a lexical decision task was implemented, controlling for semantic interpretability while manipulating the morphological status of pseudowords. The results show that the morphological composition of pseudowords generates larger latencies and more errors than pseudowords without morphemes, thereby corroborating that morphemes are activated during pseudoword processing even in the case of young readers.

以往的证据表明,在词汇判断任务中,由真实词素构成的伪词比不含词素的伪词需要更多的处理时间,产生的错误也更多。对这些结果的解释存在争议,因为有两种可能的论点:第一种与刺激物的词素构成有关,第二种与带词素的伪词与不带词素的伪词相比具有更大的语义可解释性有关(基于语义的解释)。为了弄清这个问题,我们对 92 名儿童和 42 名成人进行了实验。为此,我们实施了一项词汇决策任务,在控制语义可解释性的同时,对伪词的形态状态进行了操作。结果表明,与不带词素的伪词相比,由词素构成的伪词会产生更大的延迟和更多的错误,从而证实了词素在伪词处理过程中会被激活,即使是在小读者的情况下也是如此。
{"title":"Mind the suffix: Pseudoword processing in children and adults","authors":"Miguel Lázaro ,&nbsp;Teresa Simón ,&nbsp;Ainoa Escalonilla ,&nbsp;Trinidad Ruiz","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105977","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105977","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous evidence has shown that pseudowords made up of real morphemes take more time to process and generate more errors than pseudowords without morphemes in a lexical decision task. The explanation for these results is controversial because two possible arguments may be posited; the first is related to the morphological composition of the stimuli, and the second is related to the larger semantic interpretability of pseudowords with morphemes in comparison with pseudowords without morphemes (a semantic-based explanation). To disentangle this issue, we conducted an experiment with 92 children and 42 adults. For this purpose, a lexical decision task was implemented, controlling for semantic interpretability while manipulating the morphological status of pseudowords. The results show that the morphological composition of pseudowords generates larger latencies and more errors than pseudowords without morphemes, thereby corroborating that morphemes are activated during pseudoword processing even in the case of young readers.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022096524001176/pdfft?md5=b7ad50af2d51e5aba984e638b18cd587&pid=1-s2.0-S0022096524001176-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141201105","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceptual narrowing continues throughout childhood: Evidence from specialization of face processing 感知狭窄会持续整个童年:人脸处理专业化的证据
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-31 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105964
Marissa Hartston , Tal Lulav-Bash , Yael Goldstein-Marcusohn , Galia Avidan , Bat-Sheva Hadad

Face recognition shows a long trajectory of development and is known to be closely associated with the development of social skills. However, it is still debated whether this long trajectory is perceptually based and what the role is of experience-based refinements of face representations throughout development. We examined the effects of short and long-term experienced stimulus history on face processing, using regression biases of face representations towards the experienced mean. Children and adults performed same–different judgments in a serial discrimination task where two consecutive faces were drawn from a distribution of morphed faces. The results show that face recognition continues to improve after 9 years of age, with more pronounced improvements for own-race faces. This increased narrowing with age is also indicated by similar use of stimulus statistics for own-race and other-race faces in children, contrary to the different use of the overall stimulus history for these two face types in adults. Increased face proficiency in adulthood renders the perceptual system less tuned to other-race face statistics. Altogether, the results demonstrate associations between levels of specialization and the extent to which perceptual representations become narrowly tuned with age.

人脸识别的发展轨迹很长,而且众所周知与社交技能的发展密切相关。然而,这一漫长的发展轨迹是否以知觉为基础,以及在整个发展过程中以经验为基础的人脸表征的完善所起的作用是什么,目前仍存在争议。我们利用人脸表征对经验平均值的回归偏差,研究了短期和长期经验刺激史对人脸处理的影响。在连续辨别任务中,从变形人脸的分布中抽取连续的两张人脸,儿童和成人进行同异判断。结果表明,人脸识别能力在 9 岁以后会继续提高,对同种人脸的识别能力提高更为明显。随着年龄的增长,儿童对自己种族和其他种族面孔的刺激统计数据的使用也越来越窄,这与成人对这两种面孔类型的总体刺激历史的不同使用相反。成年后,人脸熟练程度的提高使得感知系统对其他种族人脸统计数据的调谐能力降低。总之,研究结果表明,随着年龄的增长,专业化水平与知觉表征的狭义调整程度之间存在关联。
{"title":"Perceptual narrowing continues throughout childhood: Evidence from specialization of face processing","authors":"Marissa Hartston ,&nbsp;Tal Lulav-Bash ,&nbsp;Yael Goldstein-Marcusohn ,&nbsp;Galia Avidan ,&nbsp;Bat-Sheva Hadad","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105964","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105964","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Face recognition shows a long trajectory of development and is known to be closely associated with the development of social skills. However, it is still debated whether this long trajectory is perceptually based and what the role is of experience-based refinements of face representations throughout development. We examined the effects of short and long-term experienced stimulus history on face processing, using regression biases of face representations towards the experienced mean. Children and adults performed same–different judgments in a serial discrimination task where two consecutive faces were drawn from a distribution of morphed faces. The results show that face recognition continues to improve after 9 years of age, with more pronounced improvements for own-race faces. This increased narrowing with age is also indicated by similar use of stimulus statistics for own-race and other-race faces in children, contrary to the different use of the overall stimulus history for these two face types in adults. Increased face proficiency in adulthood renders the perceptual system less tuned to other-race face statistics. Altogether, the results demonstrate associations between levels of specialization and the extent to which perceptual representations become narrowly tuned with age.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141187141","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1