首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
New measures of number line estimation performance reveal children’s ordinal understanding of numbers 新的数列估算成绩测量方法揭示了儿童对数字的顺序理解。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-31 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105965
Jenny Yun-Chen Chan , Michèle M.M. Mazzocco

Children’s performance on the number line estimation task, often measured by the percentage of absolute error, predicts their later mathematics achievement. This task may also reveal (a) children’s ordinal understanding of the target numbers in relation to each other and the benchmarks (e.g., endpoints, midpoint) and (b) the ordinal skills that are a necessary precursor to children’s ability to understand the interval nature of a number line as measured by percentage of absolute error. Using data from 104 U.S. kindergartners, we measured whether children’s estimates were correctly sequenced across trials and correctly positioned relative to given benchmarks within trials at two time points. For both time points, we found that each ordinal error measure revealed a distinct pattern of data distribution, providing opportunities to tap into different aspects of children’s ordinal understanding. Furthermore, children who made fewer ordinal errors scored higher on the Test of Early Mathematics Ability and showed greater improvement on their interval understanding of numbers as reflected by a larger reduction of percentage of absolute error from Time 1 to Time 2. The findings suggest that our number line measures reveal individual differences in children’s ordinal understanding of numbers, and that such understanding may be a precursor to their interval understanding and later mathematics performance.

儿童在数线估算任务中的表现(通常以绝对误差百分比来衡量)可预测其日后的数学成绩。这项任务还可以揭示:(a) 儿童对目标数之间的顺序关系和基准(如端点、中点)的理解;(b) 以绝对误差百分比来衡量的儿童理解数列的区间性质的能力所必需的顺序技能。利用 104 名美国幼儿园学生的数据,我们测量了在两个时间点上,儿童的估算是否正确地按顺序排列,以及相对于试验中的给定基准是否正确定位。我们发现,在这两个时间点上,每种顺序错误测量都揭示了数据分布的独特模式,为挖掘儿童对顺序理解的不同方面提供了机会。此外,顺序错误较少的儿童在早期数学能力测试中得分较高,他们对数字的区间理解能力也有较大提高,这体现在从第一时间点到第二时间点绝对错误百分比的大幅减少上。研究结果表明,我们的数线测量揭示了儿童在对数字的顺序理解方面的个体差异,而这种理解可能是他们对数字的间隔理解和日后数学成绩的先导。
{"title":"New measures of number line estimation performance reveal children’s ordinal understanding of numbers","authors":"Jenny Yun-Chen Chan ,&nbsp;Michèle M.M. Mazzocco","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105965","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105965","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Children’s performance on the number line estimation task, often measured by the percentage of absolute error, predicts their later mathematics achievement. This task may also reveal (a) children’s ordinal understanding of the target numbers in relation to each other and the benchmarks (e.g., endpoints, midpoint) and (b) the ordinal skills that are a necessary precursor to children’s ability to understand the interval nature of a number line as measured by percentage of absolute error. Using data from 104 U.S. kindergartners, we measured whether children’s estimates were correctly sequenced across trials and correctly positioned relative to given benchmarks within trials at two time points. For both time points, we found that each ordinal error measure revealed a distinct pattern of data distribution, providing opportunities to tap into different aspects of children’s ordinal understanding. Furthermore, children who made fewer ordinal errors scored higher on the Test of Early Mathematics Ability and showed greater improvement on their interval understanding of numbers as reflected by a larger reduction of percentage of absolute error from Time 1 to Time 2. The findings suggest that our number line measures reveal individual differences in children’s ordinal understanding of numbers, and that such understanding may be a precursor to their interval understanding and later mathematics performance.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S002209652400105X/pdfft?md5=568bef43c70309b6916ab5d2a2b935b1&pid=1-s2.0-S002209652400105X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141187138","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Schadenfreude or empathy? Children’s emotional responses to the physical pain and pleasure of prosocial and antisocial others 幸灾乐祸还是感同身受?儿童对亲社会和反社会他人身体上的痛苦和快乐的情绪反应。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-31 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105974
Yiyi Wang , Wen Zhou , Jingyun Zhu , Yanjie Su

Adults’ emotional reactions to the pain and pleasure of others are influenced by the moral character of those individuals. However, it remains unclear whether children’s emotional responses also show such selectivity. To investigate this, we compared 4- to 8-year-old children’s emotional responses to the physical pain and pleasure of prosocial versus antisocial puppets. In Study 1, children reported unhappiness after witnessing the pain of the prosocial and antisocial puppets but reported less unhappiness after witnessing the pain of the antisocial puppet. In Study 2, children reported happiness after witnessing the pleasure of both puppets but reported being less happy for the antisocial puppet. These results suggest that children are less likely to empathize with antisocial individuals. Meanwhile, children did not display Schadenfreude (pleasure at others’ pain) or Gluckschmerz (displeasure at others’ pleasure) toward antisocial individuals in our studies. Moreover, the selectivity of children's emotional responses disappeared after we manipulated the physical competence rather than the moral character of the puppets in Study 3. Our findings help to reveal the moral selectivity of emotional responses to others’ pleasure and pain during early childhood.

成人对他人痛苦和快乐的情绪反应受到这些人道德品质的影响。然而,儿童的情绪反应是否也表现出这种选择性,目前仍不清楚。为了研究这个问题,我们比较了 4 至 8 岁儿童对亲善木偶和反社会木偶的身体痛苦和快乐的情绪反应。在研究 1 中,儿童在目睹亲社会木偶和反社会木偶的痛苦后表示不快乐,但在目睹反社会木偶的痛苦后表示不快乐的程度较低。在研究 2 中,儿童在目睹了两个木偶的快乐后都表示快乐,但对反社会木偶的快乐程度较低。这些结果表明,儿童不太可能对反社会的人产生共鸣。与此同时,在我们的研究中,儿童并没有对反社会个体表现出幸灾乐祸(以他人的痛苦为乐)或耿耿于怀(以他人的快乐为不乐)。此外,在研究 3 中,当我们操纵木偶的身体能力而非道德品质后,儿童情绪反应的选择性也消失了。我们的研究结果有助于揭示幼儿期对他人快乐和痛苦的情绪反应的道德选择性。
{"title":"Schadenfreude or empathy? Children’s emotional responses to the physical pain and pleasure of prosocial and antisocial others","authors":"Yiyi Wang ,&nbsp;Wen Zhou ,&nbsp;Jingyun Zhu ,&nbsp;Yanjie Su","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105974","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105974","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Adults’ emotional reactions to the pain and pleasure of others are influenced by the moral character of those individuals. However, it remains unclear whether children’s emotional responses also show such selectivity. To investigate this, we compared 4- to 8-year-old children’s emotional responses to the physical pain and pleasure of prosocial versus antisocial puppets. In Study 1, children reported unhappiness after witnessing the pain of the prosocial and antisocial puppets but reported less unhappiness after witnessing the pain of the antisocial puppet. In Study 2, children reported happiness after witnessing the pleasure of both puppets but reported being less happy for the antisocial puppet. These results suggest that children are less likely to empathize with antisocial individuals. Meanwhile, children did not display Schadenfreude (pleasure at others’ pain) or Gluckschmerz (displeasure at others’ pleasure) toward antisocial individuals in our studies. Moreover, the selectivity of children's emotional responses disappeared after we manipulated the physical competence rather than the moral character of the puppets in Study 3. Our findings help to reveal the moral selectivity of emotional responses to others’ pleasure and pain during early childhood.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141187145","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Which skills underlie French-speaking children’s lexical spelling acquisition in elementary school? Insight from a cross-sectional exploratory network study from Grade 1 to Grade 5 哪些技能是小学法语儿童掌握词汇拼写的基础?从一年级到五年级的横断面探索性网络研究中获得的启示。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-29 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105963
Estelle Ardanouy , Elise Lefèvre , Hélène Delage , Pascal Zesiger

Learning to spell in an inconsistent orthographic system is a true challenge for primary school children. Previous empirical studies have highlighted three main skills involved in this learning process: phonological skills, morphological skills, and children’s sensitivity to graphotactic regularities. However, the literature shows contradictions in the exact nature of the contribution of each skill at different stages of the learning process. So, the aim of our study was to test the contribution of this set of skills in the acquisition of lexical spelling as a function of children’s grade level. For this purpose, we assessed these dimensions in a cross-sectional sample of 1101 French-speaking children from Grade 1 to Grade 5. The analyses were conducted using data-driven exploratory network modeling. The results showed (a) a predominant role of phonological skills at the beginning of learning, which tends to decrease with advancing schooling; (b) an increasing contribution of morphological skills from Grade 1 to Grade 5 with a drop in Grade 4, which is the only contribution that continues to increase in Grade 5; and (c) a contribution of the sensitivity to graphotactic regularities that tends to be stable until Grade 4 before decreasing in Grade 5. Our findings show the importance of all three skills in a dynamic process in learning to spell. The implications of these results are discussed in light of the integration of multiple patterns model of learning to spell.

对于小学生来说,在不一致的正字法系统中学习拼写是一项真正的挑战。以往的实证研究强调了这一学习过程中涉及的三种主要技能:语音技能、形态技能和儿童对图形规律的敏感性。然而,文献显示,在学习过程的不同阶段,每种技能所起作用的确切性质是相互矛盾的。因此,我们研究的目的是测试这一系列技能在儿童词汇拼写学习中的作用,并将其与儿童的年级水平联系起来。为此,我们对 1101 名一年级至五年级的法语儿童进行了横向抽样评估。分析采用了数据驱动的探索性网络模型。结果显示:(a) 语音技能在学习初期起主导作用,但随着学龄的增加,这种作用会逐渐减弱;(b) 形态技能的贡献从一年级到五年级不断增加,但在四年级有所下降,这是唯一在五年级继续增加的贡献;(c) 对图形规律的敏感性的贡献在四年级之前趋于稳定,但在五年级有所下降。我们的研究结果表明,在学习拼读的动态过程中,这三种技能都非常重要。我们将根据拼读学习的多模式整合模型来讨论这些结果的意义。
{"title":"Which skills underlie French-speaking children’s lexical spelling acquisition in elementary school? Insight from a cross-sectional exploratory network study from Grade 1 to Grade 5","authors":"Estelle Ardanouy ,&nbsp;Elise Lefèvre ,&nbsp;Hélène Delage ,&nbsp;Pascal Zesiger","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105963","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105963","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Learning to spell in an inconsistent orthographic system is a true challenge for primary school children. Previous empirical studies have highlighted three main skills involved in this learning process: phonological skills, morphological skills, and children’s sensitivity to graphotactic regularities. However, the literature shows contradictions in the exact nature of the contribution of each skill at different stages of the learning process. So, the aim of our study was to test the contribution of this set of skills in the acquisition of lexical spelling as a function of children’s grade level. For this purpose, we assessed these dimensions in a cross-sectional sample of 1101 French-speaking children from Grade 1 to Grade 5. The analyses were conducted using data-driven exploratory network modeling. The results showed (a) a predominant role of phonological skills at the beginning of learning, which tends to decrease with advancing schooling; (b) an increasing contribution of morphological skills from Grade 1 to Grade 5 with a drop in Grade 4, which is the only contribution that continues to increase in Grade 5; and (c) a contribution of the sensitivity to graphotactic regularities that tends to be stable until Grade 4 before decreasing in Grade 5. Our findings show the importance of all three skills in a dynamic process in learning to spell. The implications of these results are discussed in light of the <em>integration of multiple patterns</em> model of learning to spell.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022096524001036/pdfft?md5=56bf7952beaea29200c761c95417b393&pid=1-s2.0-S0022096524001036-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141181207","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Executive functions and problem-solving—The contribution of inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility to science problem-solving performance in elementary school students 执行功能与问题解决--抑制、工作记忆和认知灵活性对小学生科学问题解决能力的贡献
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105962
Jonas Schäfer , Timo Reuter , Miriam Leuchter , Julia Karbach

Previous research has shown that executive functions can contribute to successful problem-solving in preschool and elementary school children. However, most studies did not simultaneously assess the role of different specific aspects of executive functions. Therefore, the aim of our study was to investigate the individual contribution of inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility to science problem-solving performance in elementary school children. A total of 478 children from first and second grades (Mage = 7.44 years) participated in our study. They performed a Go/No-go task (inhibition), a Corsi blocks backward task (working memory), a flexible item selection task (cognitive flexibility), and three science problem-solving tasks, including two gear turning tasks and one stabilization task. Structural equation modeling showed that working memory and cognitive flexibility individually contributed to problem-solving performance, whereas inhibition did not. We conclude that maintaining task requirements and dynamic object relations (working memory) and switching between different problem-solving phases (cognitive flexibility) are essential components of successful science problem-solving in elementary school children. Inhibitory processes may be more relevant in tasks involving a higher degree of interference at the task or response level.

以往的研究表明,执行功能有助于学龄前儿童和小学生成功地解决问题。然而,大多数研究并没有同时评估执行功能不同具体方面的作用。因此,我们的研究旨在调查抑制、工作记忆和认知灵活性对小学生科学问题解决能力的个体贡献。共有 478 名一、二年级的儿童(年龄 = 7.44 岁)参加了我们的研究。他们分别完成了 "去/不去 "任务(抑制)、"柯西积木倒退 "任务(工作记忆)、"灵活项目选择 "任务(认知灵活性)和三个科学问题解决任务,包括两个齿轮转动任务和一个稳定任务。结构方程模型显示,工作记忆和认知灵活性对解决问题的成绩有单独的促进作用,而抑制作用则没有。我们的结论是,保持任务要求和动态对象关系(工作记忆)以及在不同问题解决阶段之间切换(认知灵活性)是小学生成功解决科学问题的重要组成部分。抑制过程可能与任务或反应层面干扰程度较高的任务更为相关。
{"title":"Executive functions and problem-solving—The contribution of inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility to science problem-solving performance in elementary school students","authors":"Jonas Schäfer ,&nbsp;Timo Reuter ,&nbsp;Miriam Leuchter ,&nbsp;Julia Karbach","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105962","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105962","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous research has shown that executive functions can contribute to successful problem-solving in preschool and elementary school children. However, most studies did not simultaneously assess the role of different specific aspects of executive functions. Therefore, the aim of our study was to investigate the individual contribution of inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility to science problem-solving performance in elementary school children. A total of 478 children from first and second grades (<em>M</em><sub>age</sub> = 7.44 years) participated in our study. They performed a Go/No-go task (inhibition), a Corsi blocks backward task (working memory), a flexible item selection task (cognitive flexibility), and three science problem-solving tasks, including two gear turning tasks and one stabilization task. Structural equation modeling showed that working memory and cognitive flexibility individually contributed to problem-solving performance, whereas inhibition did not. We conclude that maintaining task requirements and dynamic object relations (working memory) and switching between different problem-solving phases (cognitive flexibility) are essential components of successful science problem-solving in elementary school children. Inhibitory processes may be more relevant in tasks involving a higher degree of interference at the task or response level.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022096524001024/pdfft?md5=88f3d30de3cc5fee555a563d4b3fc430&pid=1-s2.0-S0022096524001024-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141164187","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children’s consideration of motivation in evaluations of socially (un)mindful actions 儿童在评价社会(非)有意行为时对动机的考虑
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105960
Yuhan Wang , Xin Zhao

Previous research shows that both adults and children by 5 or 6 years of age appreciate socially mindful actions where one leaves a choice for others. However, less is known as to whether children consider motivations in their evaluations of socially (un)mindful actions. Here we investigated whether children and adults can spontaneously evaluate socially (un)mindful behaviors depending on contextual cues, specifically whether the actions happen in public or in private. We also investigated how children evaluate these actions when provided with explicit information on motivations. We presented 99 children (aged 6–11 years) and 99 adults in China with two characters performing the same socially mindful or unmindful behaviors. One character acted publicly, whereas the other acted privately. Participants were asked to compare the two characters first spontaneously and then again after explicit information on the characters’ motivations was provided. We found that whereas adults spontaneously favor private socially mindful acts, children favor public socially mindful acts. Only after motivations were provided did children favor private socially mindful acts like adults. In addition, we found asymmetry in that motivation seems to matter more in evaluations of socially mindful actions than in evaluations of socially unmindful ones. These findings are the first to reveal children’s consideration of motivations in their evaluations of socially mindful behaviors.

以往的研究表明,成人和 5、6 岁的儿童都会欣赏为他人留出选择余地的有社会意识的行为。然而,关于儿童在评价有(无)社会意识的行为时是否会考虑动机的问题,我们却知之甚少。在此,我们研究了儿童和成人能否根据情境线索,特别是行为发生在公开场合还是私下场合,自发地对社交(不)注意行为进行评价。我们还研究了儿童在获得明确的动机信息时如何评价这些行为。我们向中国的 99 名儿童(6-11 岁)和 99 名成人展示了两个角色,他们做出了同样的有社会意识或无社会意识的行为。其中一个角色是公开行为,而另一个角色是私下行为。参与者首先被要求自发地比较这两个角色,然后在获得有关角色动机的明确信息后再次进行比较。我们发现,成人自发地倾向于私下的有社会意识的行为,而儿童则倾向于公开的有社会意识的行为。只有在提供了动机信息后,儿童才会像成人一样倾向于私人社交行为。此外,我们还发现了一种不对称性,即动机似乎在对有社会意识行为的评价中比在对无社会意识行为的评价中更重要。这些发现首次揭示了儿童在评价有社会意识的行为时对动机的考虑。
{"title":"Children’s consideration of motivation in evaluations of socially (un)mindful actions","authors":"Yuhan Wang ,&nbsp;Xin Zhao","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105960","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105960","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous research shows that both adults and children by 5 or 6 years of age appreciate socially mindful actions where one leaves a choice for others. However, less is known as to whether children consider motivations in their evaluations of socially (un)mindful actions. Here we investigated whether children and adults can spontaneously evaluate socially (un)mindful behaviors depending on contextual cues, specifically whether the actions happen in public or in private. We also investigated how children evaluate these actions when provided with explicit information on motivations. We presented 99 children (aged 6–11 years) and 99 adults in China with two characters performing the same socially mindful or unmindful behaviors. One character acted publicly, whereas the other acted privately. Participants were asked to compare the two characters first spontaneously and then again after explicit information on the characters’ motivations was provided. We found that whereas adults spontaneously favor private socially mindful acts, children favor public socially mindful acts. Only after motivations were provided did children favor private socially mindful acts like adults. In addition, we found asymmetry in that motivation seems to matter more in evaluations of socially mindful actions than in evaluations of socially unmindful ones. These findings are the first to reveal children’s consideration of motivations in their evaluations of socially mindful behaviors.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141156402","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Object play and problem solving in infancy: Insights into tool use 婴儿期的物体游戏和问题解决:工具使用的启示
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105957
Lauriane Rat-Fischer , Kim Plunkett , Auguste M.P. von Bayern , Alex Kacelnik

Tool use is primarily, but not exclusively, present in species with otherwise advanced cognitive traits. However, the interaction between such traits and conspecific inter-individual variation in the presence, complexity, or intensity of tool use is far from being established. We addressed this matter among human infants, seeking factors that relate to differences in tool use. We examined, both correlationally and experimentally, whether the propensity to engage in object combinations predicts performance in means–end problem-solving tasks involving or not involving the use of a tool. We tested 71 infants aged 15, 18, 21, and 24 months, dividing them into two subgroups: one exposed to an adult demonstrating object–object combinations (i.e., “prompting” infants to combine objects together) and another with comparable social exposure but where the adult demonstrated single-object manipulations. We found a correlation between the combined level of spontaneous and prompted object combinations and problem-solving performance regardless of the involvement of tools in the problem. However, we did not find differences in tool-use performance between the two demonstration subgroups. The correlational analysis suggests that complexity of play, as measured by the frequency of combining objects, is linked to infants’ problem-solving skills rather than being specifically associated with tool use, as previously suggested in the literature.

工具使用主要出现在具有其他高级认知特征的物种中,但并非唯一。然而,这些特征与同种个体间在工具使用的存在、复杂性或强度方面的差异之间的相互作用还远未得到证实。我们在人类婴儿中研究了这一问题,寻找与工具使用差异有关的因素。我们从相关性和实验两方面研究了,在涉及或不涉及使用工具的均值问题解决任务中,物体组合的倾向是否会预测成绩。我们对年龄分别为 15、18、21 和 24 个月的 71 名婴儿进行了测试,并将他们分为两个亚组:一个亚组是由成人演示物体与物体之间的组合(即 "提示 "婴儿将物体组合在一起),另一个亚组是由成人演示单个物体的操作,但其社会接触程度相当。我们发现,无论问题中是否涉及工具,自发和提示性物体组合的综合水平与问题解决的成绩之间都存在相关性。然而,我们并没有发现在两个演示分组之间工具使用成绩的差异。相关分析表明,以组合物体的频率来衡量的游戏复杂性与婴儿解决问题的能力有关,而不是像以前的文献中所说的那样与工具的使用特别相关。
{"title":"Object play and problem solving in infancy: Insights into tool use","authors":"Lauriane Rat-Fischer ,&nbsp;Kim Plunkett ,&nbsp;Auguste M.P. von Bayern ,&nbsp;Alex Kacelnik","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105957","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105957","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Tool use is primarily, but not exclusively, present in species with otherwise advanced cognitive traits. However, the interaction between such traits and conspecific inter-individual variation in the presence, complexity, or intensity of tool use is far from being established. We addressed this matter among human infants, seeking factors that relate to differences in tool use. We examined, both correlationally and experimentally, whether the propensity to engage in object combinations predicts performance in means–end problem-solving tasks involving or not involving the use of a tool. We tested 71 infants aged 15, 18, 21, and 24 months, dividing them into two subgroups: one exposed to an adult demonstrating object–object combinations (i.e., “prompting” infants to combine objects together) and another with comparable social exposure but where the adult demonstrated single-object manipulations. We found a correlation between the combined level of spontaneous and prompted object combinations and problem-solving performance regardless of the involvement of tools in the problem. However, we did not find differences in tool-use performance between the two demonstration subgroups. The correlational analysis suggests that complexity of play, as measured by the frequency of combining objects, is linked to infants’ problem-solving skills rather than being specifically associated with tool use, as previously suggested in the literature.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022096524000973/pdfft?md5=0ee295fbec70ee44344004a1bb4eeeec&pid=1-s2.0-S0022096524000973-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141156374","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The roles of mathematical language and emergent literacy skills in the longitudinal prediction of specific early numeracy skills 数学语言和早期识字能力在纵向预测特定早期计算能力中的作用
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-24 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105959
Caroline Byrd Hornburg , Yemimah A. King , Lauren Westerberg , Sara A. Schmitt , David J. Purpura

Mathematical language (i.e., content-specific language used in mathematics) and emergent literacy skills predict children’s broad numeracy development. However, little work has examined whether these domains predict development of individual numeracy skills (e.g., cardinality, number order). Thus, the aim of the current study was to examine longitudinal relations among mathematical language, emergent literacy skills, and specific early numeracy skills. Participants included 114 preschool children aged 3.12 to 5.26 years (M = 4.17 years, SD = 0.59). Specifically, this study examined whether mathematical language and three emergent literacy skills (print knowledge, phonological awareness, and general vocabulary) in the fall of preschool predicted 12 individual early numeracy skills in the spring, controlling for age, sex, rapid automatized naming, parent education, and autoregressors. Results indicated that mathematical language predicted development of most of the early numeracy skills (e.g., set comparison, numeral comparison, numeral identification), but findings for emergent literacy skills were not robust. Among the three emergent literacy skills, only print knowledge was a significant predictor of development in some specific numeracy skills, including verbal counting, number order, and story problems. Results highlight the important role of mathematical language in children’s numeracy development and provide the foundation for future work in designing interventions to improve early numeracy skills.

数学语言(即数学中使用的特定内容语言)和萌芽识字技能可预测儿童的广泛计算能力发展。然而,很少有人研究过这些领域是否能预测个别算术技能(如心算、数序)的发展。因此,本研究旨在考察数学语言、萌芽识字技能和特定早期计算技能之间的纵向关系。研究对象包括 114 名学龄前儿童,年龄在 3.12 至 5.26 岁之间(中位数 = 4.17 岁,标准差 = 0.59)。具体来说,本研究考察了学前班秋季的数学语言和三种萌芽识字技能(印刷知识、语音意识和一般词汇)是否能预测春季的 12 种个别早期算术技能,并对年龄、性别、快速自动命名、家长教育和自回归因素进行了控制。结果表明,数学语言能预测大多数早期算术技能(如集合比较、数字比较、数字识别)的发展,但对萌芽识字技能的预测结果并不可靠。在三种萌芽识字技能中,只有印刷知识对一些特定算术技能的发展有显著的预测作用,包括口算、数字顺序和故事问题。研究结果强调了数学语言在儿童计算能力发展中的重要作用,并为今后设计干预措施以提高早期计算能力奠定了基础。
{"title":"The roles of mathematical language and emergent literacy skills in the longitudinal prediction of specific early numeracy skills","authors":"Caroline Byrd Hornburg ,&nbsp;Yemimah A. King ,&nbsp;Lauren Westerberg ,&nbsp;Sara A. Schmitt ,&nbsp;David J. Purpura","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105959","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105959","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Mathematical language (i.e., content-specific language used in mathematics) and emergent literacy skills predict children’s broad numeracy development. However, little work has examined whether these domains predict development of individual numeracy skills (e.g., cardinality, number order). Thus, the aim of the current study was to examine longitudinal relations among mathematical language, emergent literacy skills, and specific early numeracy skills. Participants included 114 preschool children aged 3.12 to 5.26 years (<em>M</em> = 4.17 years, <em>SD</em> = 0.59). Specifically, this study examined whether mathematical language and three emergent literacy skills (print knowledge, phonological awareness, and general vocabulary) in the fall of preschool predicted 12 individual early numeracy skills in the spring, controlling for age, sex, rapid automatized naming, parent education, and autoregressors. Results indicated that mathematical language predicted development of most of the early numeracy skills (e.g., set comparison, numeral comparison, numeral identification), but findings for emergent literacy skills were not robust. Among the three emergent literacy skills, only print knowledge was a significant predictor of development in some specific numeracy skills, including verbal counting, number order, and story problems. Results highlight the important role of mathematical language in children’s numeracy development and provide the foundation for future work in designing interventions to improve early numeracy skills.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141095438","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sex differences in direction giving: Are boys better than girls? 指引方向的性别差异:男孩比女孩好吗?
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-23 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105958
Nardin Yacoub, Laura Lakusta, Yingying Yang

Previous research has extensively documented sex differences favoring boys in various domains of spatial cognition. However, relatively little research has examined sex differences in children’s direction giving. The current study aimed to bridge this gap. A total of 143 children aged 3 to 10 years were asked to describe and recall routes from survey perspectives (via maps) and route perspectives (via videos). Significant sex effects (favoring boys) in direction-giving accuracy were found in describing route trials. However, boys and girls did not differ in the frequency of utterances encoding landmarks and direction of turns, suggesting that the quality rather than the quantity of words played a more important role in explaining sex differences. In addition, there was no sex difference in the route recall task. Although accuracy was overall higher in the map condition than in the video condition, it did not moderate sex differences. Overall, our study showed a robust sex difference in direction giving, which has important theoretical implications for understanding the development of human sex differences and critical clinical implications for designing training programs to improve children’s spatial cognition.

以往的研究广泛记录了男孩在空间认知的各个领域中的性别差异。然而,对儿童方向感的性别差异的研究相对较少。本研究旨在弥补这一不足。共有 143 名 3 至 10 岁的儿童被要求从调查角度(通过地图)和路线角度(通过视频)描述和回忆路线。结果发现,在描述路线的试验中,男孩和女孩在辨别方向的准确性上存在显著的性别效应(男孩更有利)。然而,男孩和女孩在编码地标和转弯方向的语句频率上并无差异,这表明在解释性别差异时,语句的质量而非数量起到了更重要的作用。此外,在路线回忆任务中也没有性别差异。虽然地图条件下的准确率总体上高于视频条件下的准确率,但这并没有缓和性别差异。总之,我们的研究表明,在方向选择方面存在明显的性别差异,这对于理解人类性别差异的发展具有重要的理论意义,对于设计训练计划以提高儿童的空间认知能力也具有重要的临床意义。
{"title":"Sex differences in direction giving: Are boys better than girls?","authors":"Nardin Yacoub,&nbsp;Laura Lakusta,&nbsp;Yingying Yang","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105958","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105958","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous research has extensively documented sex differences favoring boys in various domains of spatial cognition. However, relatively little research has examined sex differences in children’s direction giving. The current study aimed to bridge this gap. A total of 143 children aged 3 to 10 years were asked to describe and recall routes from survey perspectives (via maps) and route perspectives (via videos). Significant sex effects (favoring boys) in direction-giving accuracy were found in describing route trials. However, boys and girls did not differ in the frequency of utterances encoding landmarks and direction of turns, suggesting that the quality rather than the quantity of words played a more important role in explaining sex differences. In addition, there was no sex difference in the route recall task. Although accuracy was overall higher in the map condition than in the video condition, it did not moderate sex differences. Overall, our study showed a robust sex difference in direction giving, which has important theoretical implications for understanding the development of human sex differences and critical clinical implications for designing training programs to improve children’s spatial cognition.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141090898","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can touchscreens replace teachers? Chinese children’s character learning from a touchscreen-based app, video, or face-to-face instruction 触摸屏能否取代教师?中国儿童从触摸屏应用程序、视频或面对面教学中学习汉字的情况
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105961
Chunying Gao , Fuxing Wang , Judith H. Danovitch

Given the increasing prevalence of touchscreen devices that are intended for educational purposes, this study explored children’s transfer of learning from touchscreen media compared with video and offline face-to-face learning. A total of 76 5- and 6-year-old Chinese kindergarten children (M = 68.21 months, SD = 3.57, range = 62–76; 30 boys and 46 girls) were randomly assigned to learn eight Chinese characters using a touchscreen-based app, using a video, or through face-to-face interaction. Learning was measured via the recall task scores, recognition task scores, recall efficiency, and recognition efficiency. The results revealed that children’s recall and recognition task scores improved when learning took place using the touchscreen or face-to-face interaction. Children’s recall efficiency and recognition efficiency were strongest in the face-to-face condition, followed by the touchscreen condition and then the video condition. The effects of instructional format on children’s recall and recognition scores and recall efficiency were moderated by age; younger children’s recall and recognition scores in the face-to-face condition and the touchscreen condition were significantly higher than in the video condition, yet older children’s recall and recognition scores did not differ between conditions. However, for recall efficiency, younger children’s recall efficiency in the face-to-face condition and the touchscreen condition was significantly higher than in the video condition; older children’s recall efficiency in the face-to-face condition was higher than in both the touchscreen condition and the video condition. In conclusion, both face-to-face interaction and a touchscreen-based app were helpful ways for children to learn Chinese characters compared with video, but face-to-face learning showed advantages over touchscreen learning in recall efficiency for older children.

鉴于以教育为目的的触摸屏设备日益普及,本研究探讨了与视频和离线面对面学习相比,儿童从触摸屏媒体中获得的学习迁移。76名5-6岁的中国幼儿园儿童(中=68.21个月,标差=3.57,范围=62-76;30名男孩和46名女孩)被随机分配到使用触摸屏应用程序、视频或面对面互动来学习八个汉字。学习效果通过回忆任务得分、识别任务得分、回忆效率和识别效率来衡量。结果显示,使用触摸屏或面对面互动学习时,儿童的回忆和识别任务得分均有所提高。面对面条件下,儿童的回忆效率和识别效率最高,其次是触摸屏条件,然后是视频条件。教学形式对儿童回忆和识别得分以及回忆效率的影响受到年龄的调节;年龄较小的儿童在面对面条件和触摸屏条件下的回忆和识别得分明显高于视频条件,而年龄较大的儿童在不同条件下的回忆和识别得分没有差异。然而,在回忆效率方面,年龄较小的儿童在面对面条件和触摸屏条件下的回忆效率明显高于视频条件;年龄较大的儿童在面对面条件下的回忆效率高于触摸屏条件和视频条件。总之,与视频相比,面对面互动和基于触摸屏的应用程序都有助于儿童学习汉字,但对于年龄较大的儿童来说,面对面学习比触摸屏学习在记忆效率上更有优势。
{"title":"Can touchscreens replace teachers? Chinese children’s character learning from a touchscreen-based app, video, or face-to-face instruction","authors":"Chunying Gao ,&nbsp;Fuxing Wang ,&nbsp;Judith H. Danovitch","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105961","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105961","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Given the increasing prevalence of touchscreen devices that are intended for educational purposes, this study explored children’s transfer of learning from touchscreen media compared with video and offline face-to-face learning. A total of 76 5- and 6-year-old Chinese kindergarten children (<em>M</em> = 68.21 months, <em>SD</em> = 3.57, range = 62–76; 30 boys and 46 girls) were randomly assigned to learn eight Chinese characters using a touchscreen-based app, using a video, or through face-to-face interaction. Learning was measured via the recall task scores, recognition task scores, recall efficiency, and recognition efficiency. The results revealed that children’s recall and recognition task scores improved when learning took place using the touchscreen or face-to-face interaction. Children’s recall efficiency and recognition efficiency were strongest in the face-to-face condition, followed by the touchscreen condition and then the video condition. The effects of instructional format on children’s recall and recognition scores and recall efficiency were moderated by age; younger children’s recall and recognition scores in the face-to-face condition and the touchscreen condition were significantly higher than in the video condition, yet older children’s recall and recognition scores did not differ between conditions. However, for recall efficiency, younger children’s recall efficiency in the face-to-face condition and the touchscreen condition was significantly higher than in the video condition; older children’s recall efficiency in the face-to-face condition was higher than in both the touchscreen condition and the video condition. In conclusion, both face-to-face interaction and a touchscreen-based app were helpful ways for children to learn Chinese characters compared with video, but face-to-face learning showed advantages over touchscreen learning in recall efficiency for older children.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141072893","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Fostering diversity in mathematics cognition 促进数学认知的多样性
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105955
Pooja G. Sidney , Percival G. Matthews

Integrating diverse perspectives in psychological science can enhance innovation in research and allow research teams to better study diverse populations of individuals through an authentic lens. Despite recent efforts to better address issues of race and ethnicity in research samples, the field of psychology broadly—and the area of mathematics cognition specifically—has largely failed to support scientists from diverse racial and ethnic backgrounds. In this essay, we consider the unique contributions that scholars of color can make to psychological research in mathematics cognition. Next, we reveal common challenges faced by scholars of color and challenges to recruiting and maintaining scholars of color in our community with a focus on Black scholars. Finally, we propose actions for diversifying the “pipeline” of promising scholars.

在心理科学中融入多元化的视角可以提高研究的创新性,让研究团队通过真实的视角更好地研究不同的人群。尽管近来心理学界努力在研究样本中更好地解决种族和民族问题,但心理学领域--特别是数学认知领域--在很大程度上未能为来自不同种族和民族背景的科学家提供支持。在本文中,我们将探讨有色人种学者在数学认知领域的心理学研究中所能做出的独特贡献。接下来,我们将以黑人学者为重点,揭示有色人种学者所面临的共同挑战,以及在招募和留住有色人种学者方面所面临的挑战。最后,我们提出了使有前途的学者 "管道 "多样化的行动建议。
{"title":"Fostering diversity in mathematics cognition","authors":"Pooja G. Sidney ,&nbsp;Percival G. Matthews","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105955","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105955","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Integrating diverse perspectives in psychological science can enhance innovation in research and allow research teams to better study diverse populations of individuals through an authentic lens. Despite recent efforts to better address issues of race and ethnicity in research samples, the field of psychology broadly—and the area of mathematics cognition specifically—has largely failed to support scientists from diverse racial and ethnic backgrounds. In this essay, we consider the unique contributions that scholars of color can make to psychological research in mathematics cognition. Next, we reveal common challenges faced by scholars of color and challenges to recruiting and maintaining scholars of color in our community with a focus on Black scholars. Finally, we propose actions for diversifying the “pipeline” of promising scholars.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140950574","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1