首页 > 最新文献

Psychology and Aging最新文献

英文 中文
Social and general cognition are uniquely associated with social connectedness in later life. 社会认知和一般认知与晚年的社会联系有着独特的联系。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-20 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000831
Anne C Krendl, Siyun Peng, Lucas J Hamilton, Brea L Perry

The mechanisms by which older adults maintain large, complex social networks are not well understood. Prior work has primarily focused on general cognitive ability (e.g., executive function, episodic memory), largely overlooking social cognition-the ability to process, store, and remember social information. Because social cognition plays a key role in navigating social interactions and is distinct from general cognition, we examined whether general and social cognition uniquely predicted the nature of older adults' personal social networks. Our study leveraged comprehensive measures of general cognition (executive function, episodic memory), social cognition (face memory and dynamic measures of cognitive and affective theory of mind), and a rigorous measure of personal social networks from 143 community-dwelling older adults. We found that, when modeled together and controlling for sociodemographic variables, only executive function and dynamic cognitive theory of mind positively predicted having social networks with relatively unfamiliar, loosely connected others, accounting for 17% of the unique variance in older adults' social connectedness. Interestingly, having a social network comprised primarily of close, tightly knit relationships was negatively associated with affective theory of mind performance. Findings are discussed in the context of the social-cognitive resource framework-which suggests that social cognition may be more engaged in relatively unfamiliar, versus close, interactions. Specifically, our results show that social-cognitive processes may be relatively automatic for individuals whose primary social relationships are very close but may be more strongly engaged for individuals whose interactions include at least some relatively less close relationships. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们对老年人维持庞大而复杂的社交网络的机制还不甚了解。之前的研究主要集中于一般认知能力(如执行功能、外显记忆),在很大程度上忽略了社会认知--处理、存储和记忆社会信息的能力。由于社会认知在社会交往中起着关键作用,而且有别于一般认知,因此我们研究了一般认知和社会认知是否能独特地预测老年人个人社交网络的性质。我们的研究利用了对 143 名居住在社区的老年人的一般认知(执行功能、外显记忆)、社会认知(面孔记忆以及认知和情感心智理论的动态测量)和个人社交网络的严格测量的综合测量。我们发现,如果将执行功能和动态认知心智理论放在一起建模并控制社会人口学变量,只有执行功能和动态认知心智理论能积极预测与相对陌生、联系松散的人建立社交网络的情况,占老年人社交联系独特差异的 17%。有趣的是,社交网络主要由关系密切、紧密的人组成,这与情感心智理论的表现呈负相关。研究结果将在社会认知资源框架的背景下进行讨论--该框架认为,相对于亲密的互动,社会认知可能更多参与到相对陌生的互动中。具体来说,我们的研究结果表明,对于那些主要社会关系非常密切的人来说,社会认知过程可能会相对自动,但对于那些互动至少包括一些相对不太密切的关系的人来说,社会认知过程可能会更强烈。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Social and general cognition are uniquely associated with social connectedness in later life.","authors":"Anne C Krendl, Siyun Peng, Lucas J Hamilton, Brea L Perry","doi":"10.1037/pag0000831","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000831","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The mechanisms by which older adults maintain large, complex social networks are not well understood. Prior work has primarily focused on general cognitive ability (e.g., executive function, episodic memory), largely overlooking social cognition-the ability to process, store, and remember social information. Because social cognition plays a key role in navigating social interactions and is distinct from general cognition, we examined whether general and social cognition uniquely predicted the nature of older adults' personal social networks. Our study leveraged comprehensive measures of general cognition (executive function, episodic memory), social cognition (face memory and dynamic measures of cognitive and affective theory of mind), and a rigorous measure of personal social networks from 143 community-dwelling older adults. We found that, when modeled together and controlling for sociodemographic variables, only executive function and dynamic cognitive theory of mind positively predicted having social networks with relatively unfamiliar, loosely connected others, accounting for 17% of the unique variance in older adults' social connectedness. Interestingly, having a social network comprised primarily of close, tightly knit relationships was negatively associated with affective theory of mind performance. Findings are discussed in the context of the social-cognitive resource framework-which suggests that social cognition may be more engaged in relatively unfamiliar, versus close, interactions. Specifically, our results show that social-cognitive processes may be relatively automatic for individuals whose primary social relationships are very close but may be more strongly engaged for individuals whose interactions include at least some relatively less close relationships. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141427977","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Age-based stereotype threat in the workplace: A daily diary study of antecedents and mechanisms. 工作场所中基于年龄的刻板印象威胁:关于前因和机制的每日日记研究。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000839
Sophie Coulon, Courtney von Hippel, Kim Peters

Both older and younger employees are stereotyped at work, making them susceptible to age-based stereotype threat or the concern about being negatively stereotyped based on their age. To date, there is limited understanding of the workplace events that spark age-based stereotype threat and the mechanisms through which they do so. The current daily diary study aimed to assess the frequency with which older and younger employees experience various events that have been identified as potential antecedents of age-based stereotype threat (e.g., being overlooked for training or development opportunities, feeling excluded from informal socializing) as well as their association with experiences of stereotype threat. As predicted, we found that employees who reported more frequent experiences of these events subsequently reported greater feelings of stereotype threat. This association was partially mediated by the capacity of these events to increase the salience of a participant's age. There was also evidence of reciprocal association, whereby employees who felt more stereotype threat were also more likely to subsequently report experiencing these events. Employee age did not moderate the relationship between daily feelings of stereotype threat and daily outcomes, suggesting that everyday feelings of stereotype threat may be equally problematic for both older and younger employees. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

无论是年长员工还是年轻员工,在工作中都会受到刻板印象的影响,使他们容易受到年龄刻板印象威胁,或担心因年龄而受到负面刻板印象的影响。迄今为止,人们对引发年龄刻板印象威胁的工作场所事件及其机制的了解还很有限。当前的每日日记研究旨在评估老年员工和年轻员工经历各种事件的频率,这些事件已被确认为年龄刻板印象威胁的潜在前因(如被忽视培训或发展机会、感觉被排除在非正式社交之外),以及它们与刻板印象威胁经历之间的关联。正如我们所预测的那样,我们发现那些报告了更频繁经历这些事件的员工随后报告了更强烈的刻板印象威胁感。这种关联的部分原因是这些事件能够提高参与者年龄的显著性。也有证据表明存在相互关联,即感受到更多刻板印象威胁的员工随后也更有可能报告经历过这些事件。员工年龄并没有调节日常刻板印象威胁感与日常结果之间的关系,这表明日常刻板印象威胁感可能对年龄较大和较年轻的员工造成同样的问题。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Age-based stereotype threat in the workplace: A daily diary study of antecedents and mechanisms.","authors":"Sophie Coulon, Courtney von Hippel, Kim Peters","doi":"10.1037/pag0000839","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000839","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Both older and younger employees are stereotyped at work, making them susceptible to age-based stereotype threat or the concern about being negatively stereotyped based on their age. To date, there is limited understanding of the workplace events that spark age-based stereotype threat and the mechanisms through which they do so. The current daily diary study aimed to assess the frequency with which older and younger employees experience various events that have been identified as potential antecedents of age-based stereotype threat (e.g., being overlooked for training or development opportunities, feeling excluded from informal socializing) as well as their association with experiences of stereotype threat. As predicted, we found that employees who reported more frequent experiences of these events subsequently reported greater feelings of stereotype threat. This association was partially mediated by the capacity of these events to increase the salience of a participant's age. There was also evidence of reciprocal association, whereby employees who felt more stereotype threat were also more likely to subsequently report experiencing these events. Employee age did not moderate the relationship between daily feelings of stereotype threat and daily outcomes, suggesting that everyday feelings of stereotype threat may be equally problematic for both older and younger employees. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141447381","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Weight gain in anorexia nervosa across age groups in higher levels of care. 不同年龄组神经性厌食症患者在较高护理级别下的体重增加情况。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000849
Renee D Rienecke, Daniel Le Grange, Alan Duffy, Philip S Mehler, Dan V Blalock

Eating disorders (EDs) have historically been thought of as afflictions of younger women, but EDs do occur in midlife/older adults, and the incidence of EDs among older women may be increasing. The present study sought to examine outcomes for patients with anorexia nervosa needing to weight restore across four age groups: under 18, 18-25, 26-39, and 40+. Based on prior research, it was hypothesized that there would be no differences between the age groups in percent of expected body weight (%EBW) gained during treatment. Participants were 2,491 patients receiving treatment for an ED at a large multisite treatment facility offering higher levels of care. At this treatment facility, EBW is individualized for each patient, considering a patient's premorbid body weight and historical weight trends. Adult patients ages 26-39 (t = -3.58, p < .001) and ages 40+ (t = -4.70, p < .001) had significantly lower improvements in %EBW compared to adult patients ages 18-25. Child and adolescent patients (under 18) had significantly greater improvements in %EBW than adult patients (t = 14.30, p < .001). Findings from the present study suggest that targeted treatments may need to be developed to increase weight gain in midlife/older adults. In addition, efforts may need to be strengthened to keep adults in treatment longer than they may initially want to, particularly when treatment and weight gain become difficult. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

饮食失调症(ED)历来被认为是年轻女性的痼疾,但中年/老年女性也会患上饮食失调症,而且老年女性的饮食失调症发病率可能会越来越高。本研究试图考察需要恢复体重的神经性厌食症患者在四个年龄段的治疗效果:18 岁以下、18-25 岁、26-39 岁和 40 岁以上。根据之前的研究,假设各年龄组在治疗期间预期体重增加百分比(%EBW)方面没有差异。参与研究的 2,491 名 ED 患者都是在一家提供较高水平治疗的大型多地点治疗机构接受治疗的。在这家治疗机构,EBW 是根据患者病前体重和历史体重趋势为每位患者量身定制的。与18-25岁的成年患者相比,26-39岁(t = -3.58,p < .001)和40岁以上(t = -4.70,p < .001)的成年患者的EBW改善率明显较低。儿童和青少年患者(18 岁以下)的体重增加率明显高于成年患者(t = 14.30,p < .001)。本研究结果表明,可能需要开发有针对性的治疗方法来增加中年/老年患者的体重增加。此外,可能还需要加强努力,使成年人接受治疗的时间比他们最初希望的时间更长,尤其是当治疗和体重增加变得困难时。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Weight gain in anorexia nervosa across age groups in higher levels of care.","authors":"Renee D Rienecke, Daniel Le Grange, Alan Duffy, Philip S Mehler, Dan V Blalock","doi":"10.1037/pag0000849","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000849","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Eating disorders (EDs) have historically been thought of as afflictions of younger women, but EDs do occur in midlife/older adults, and the incidence of EDs among older women may be increasing. The present study sought to examine outcomes for patients with anorexia nervosa needing to weight restore across four age groups: under 18, 18-25, 26-39, and 40+. Based on prior research, it was hypothesized that there would be no differences between the age groups in percent of expected body weight (%EBW) gained during treatment. Participants were 2,491 patients receiving treatment for an ED at a large multisite treatment facility offering higher levels of care. At this treatment facility, EBW is individualized for each patient, considering a patient's premorbid body weight and historical weight trends. Adult patients ages 26-39 (<i>t</i> = -3.58, <i>p</i> < .001) and ages 40+ (<i>t</i> = -4.70, <i>p</i> < .001) had significantly lower improvements in %EBW compared to adult patients ages 18-25. Child and adolescent patients (under 18) had significantly greater improvements in %EBW than adult patients (<i>t</i> = 14.30, <i>p</i> < .001). Findings from the present study suggest that targeted treatments may need to be developed to increase weight gain in midlife/older adults. In addition, efforts may need to be strengthened to keep adults in treatment longer than they may initially want to, particularly when treatment and weight gain become difficult. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142113530","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotional support across adulthood: A 60-year study of men's social networks. 成年后的情感支持:长达 60 年的男性社交网络研究。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-15 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000843
Kate Petrova, Michael D Nevarez, Robert J Waldinger, Marc S Schulz

Past research shows that social networks get smaller with age. But not all types of relationships may shrink at the same rate or for similar reasons. In the present study, we used a unique data set from a sample of 235 men who were followed longitudinally for 71 years to examine how the general pattern of network shrinkage documented in previous research generalizes to the number of emotional support providers in people's networks. We additionally examined early-life predictors of the size of later-life support network. Growth curve analyses revealed that, mirroring the more general pattern of network shrinkage, emotional support networks shrink by as much as 50% between the ages of 30 and 90, reflecting an average reduction from two to one support providers. Examining the associations between prospectively collected measures of childhood family environment and later-life emotional support, we also found that men who grew up in warmer family environments had larger support networks in adulthood. In contrast, childhood family socioeconomic status was not connected to the size of emotional support networks later in life. The generalizability of this work is limited by the use of an archival all-male sample from the United States. Despite this limitation, these findings make important contributions to our understanding of adult socioemotional development and underscore the importance of prospectively collected longitudinal data in developmental research. Additional research is needed to examine the consequences of changing emotional support across the lifespan for health and well-being. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

过去的研究表明,社交网络会随着年龄的增长而缩小。但并非所有类型的关系都会以相同的速度或出于类似的原因缩小。在本研究中,我们使用了一个独特的数据集,对 235 名男性进行了长达 71 年的纵向跟踪调查,以研究以往研究中记录的人际关系网络缩减的一般模式如何扩展到人们人际关系网络中情感支持提供者的数量。此外,我们还研究了晚年支持网络规模的早期预测因素。增长曲线分析表明,与更普遍的网络萎缩模式一样,情感支持网络在 30 岁到 90 岁之间的萎缩幅度高达 50%,反映出支持提供者平均从两个减少到一个。在研究前瞻性收集的童年家庭环境与晚年情感支持之间的关联时,我们还发现,在温暖家庭环境中长大的男性,其成年后的支持网络更大。相比之下,童年时期的家庭社会经济地位与日后生活中情感支持网络的规模没有关系。由于使用的是来自美国的全男性档案样本,这项研究的推广性受到了限制。尽管存在这种局限性,但这些发现对我们理解成人社会情感发展做出了重要贡献,并强调了前瞻性收集纵向数据在发展研究中的重要性。我们还需要进行更多的研究,以探讨在整个生命周期中,情感支持的变化对健康和幸福的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Emotional support across adulthood: A 60-year study of men's social networks.","authors":"Kate Petrova, Michael D Nevarez, Robert J Waldinger, Marc S Schulz","doi":"10.1037/pag0000843","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000843","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Past research shows that social networks get smaller with age. But not all types of relationships may shrink at the same rate or for similar reasons. In the present study, we used a unique data set from a sample of 235 men who were followed longitudinally for 71 years to examine how the general pattern of network shrinkage documented in previous research generalizes to the number of emotional support providers in people's networks. We additionally examined early-life predictors of the size of later-life support network. Growth curve analyses revealed that, mirroring the more general pattern of network shrinkage, emotional support networks shrink by as much as 50% between the ages of 30 and 90, reflecting an average reduction from two to one support providers. Examining the associations between prospectively collected measures of childhood family environment and later-life emotional support, we also found that men who grew up in warmer family environments had larger support networks in adulthood. In contrast, childhood family socioeconomic status was not connected to the size of emotional support networks later in life. The generalizability of this work is limited by the use of an archival all-male sample from the United States. Despite this limitation, these findings make important contributions to our understanding of adult socioemotional development and underscore the importance of prospectively collected longitudinal data in developmental research. Additional research is needed to examine the consequences of changing emotional support across the lifespan for health and well-being. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141989167","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When and how perceived control buffers against cognitive declines: A moderated mediation analysis. 感知控制何时以及如何缓冲认知能力的下降?调节中介分析
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-12 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000841
Jeremy M Hamm, Margie E Lachman, Katherine A Duggan, Jacqueline A Mogle, Ryan McGrath, Kelly Parker, Laura M Klepacz

Although perceived control is a well-established predictor of cognitive aging, less is known about how and under what developmental circumstances these beliefs about personal influence may protect against cognitive declines. Our study examined light physical activity (LPA) as an unexplored mechanism that may link changes in two facets of perceived control (personal mastery, perceived constraints) to longitudinal trajectories of cognitive functioning. We also examined whether mediated pathways were moderated by age (i.e., differed across the adult lifespan). We analyzed two-wave, 9-year data from the national Midlife in the United States Study (n = 2,456; Mage = 56 years, range = 30-84; 56% female) using autoregressive mediation and moderated mediation models. Mediation models showed that changes in personal mastery and perceived constraints predicted episodic memory and executive functioning via self-reported change in LPA. Only the mediated effects of constraints remained significant in a model that included both mastery and constraints as predictors. Moderated mediation models showed that, for episodic memory, the mediated pathways were strongest in old age and emerged only for constraints: For older but not younger adults, declines in constraints were associated with less decline in episodic memory, as mediated by increases in LPA. Results were consistent in sensitivity analyses that controlled for levels and change in moderate-to-vigorous physical activity. Findings inform lifespan theories of control and provide initial evidence that change in a largely overlooked health behavior (LPA) may underlie the link between perceived constraints and cognitive functioning, with this pathway becoming more pronounced in late life. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管感知控制是一个公认的认知老化预测因子,但人们对这些关于个人影响力的信念如何以及在何种发展情况下可以防止认知能力下降知之甚少。我们的研究将轻度体育锻炼(LPA)作为一种尚未探索的机制进行了研究,这种机制可能会将感知控制的两个方面(个人掌握、感知限制)的变化与认知功能的纵向轨迹联系起来。我们还研究了中介途径是否会受到年龄的调节(即在成年人的整个生命周期中存在差异)。我们使用自回归中介模型和调节中介模型分析了美国全国中年研究(n = 2,456;年龄 = 56 岁,范围 = 30-84;56% 为女性)的两波、为期 9 年的数据。中介模型显示,个人主观能动性和感知到的制约因素的变化通过自我报告的 LPA 变化来预测外显记忆和执行功能。在同时包含掌握程度和制约因素的预测模型中,只有制约因素的中介效应仍然显著。调节中介模型显示,在表观记忆方面,老年期的中介途径最强,而只有制约因素出现了中介途径:对于老年人而非年轻人来说,制约因素的减少与外显记忆的减少有关,而外显记忆的减少与 LPA 的增加有关。在控制中度到剧烈运动水平和变化的敏感性分析中,结果是一致的。研究结果为生命周期控制理论提供了参考,并提供了初步证据表明,一种被忽视的健康行为(LPA)的变化可能是感知到的限制与认知功能之间联系的基础,而这种途径在晚年会变得更加明显。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"When and how perceived control buffers against cognitive declines: A moderated mediation analysis.","authors":"Jeremy M Hamm, Margie E Lachman, Katherine A Duggan, Jacqueline A Mogle, Ryan McGrath, Kelly Parker, Laura M Klepacz","doi":"10.1037/pag0000841","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000841","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although perceived control is a well-established predictor of cognitive aging, less is known about how and under what developmental circumstances these beliefs about personal influence may protect against cognitive declines. Our study examined light physical activity (LPA) as an unexplored mechanism that may link changes in two facets of perceived control (personal mastery, perceived constraints) to longitudinal trajectories of cognitive functioning. We also examined whether mediated pathways were moderated by age (i.e., differed across the adult lifespan). We analyzed two-wave, 9-year data from the national Midlife in the United States Study (<i>n</i> = 2,456; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 56 years, range = 30-84; 56% female) using autoregressive mediation and moderated mediation models. Mediation models showed that changes in personal mastery and perceived constraints predicted episodic memory and executive functioning via self-reported change in LPA. Only the mediated effects of constraints remained significant in a model that included both mastery and constraints as predictors. Moderated mediation models showed that, for episodic memory, the mediated pathways were strongest in old age and emerged only for constraints: For older but not younger adults, declines in constraints were associated with less decline in episodic memory, as mediated by increases in LPA. Results were consistent in sensitivity analyses that controlled for levels and change in moderate-to-vigorous physical activity. Findings inform lifespan theories of control and provide initial evidence that change in a largely overlooked health behavior (LPA) may underlie the link between perceived constraints and cognitive functioning, with this pathway becoming more pronounced in late life. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141972120","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Predictors of cognitive aging profiles over 15 years: A longitudinal population-based study. 15 年认知老化特征的预测因素:基于人口的纵向研究
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000807
Ingrid Ekström, Maria Josefsson, Lars Bäckman, Erika J Laukka

The present study aimed to characterize profiles of cognitive aging and how these can be predicted from interindividual differences in demographic, lifestyle, health, and genetic factors. The participants were 1,966 older adults (mean baseline age = 71.6 years; 62.9% female), free from dementia at baseline and with at least two cognitive assessments over the 15-year follow-up, from the population-based Swedish National Study on Aging and Care in Kungsholmen. The cognitive assessment comprised tests of semantic and episodic memory, letter and category fluency, perceptual speed, and executive function. First, we estimated the level and change within each of the cognitive domains with linear mixed effect models, based on which we grouped our sample into participants with "maintained high cognition," "moderate cognitive decline," or "accelerated cognitive decline." Second, we analyzed determinants of group membership within each cognitive domain with multinomial logistic regression. Third, group memberships within each cognitive domain were used to derive general cognitive aging profiles with latent class analysis. Fourth, the determinants of these profile memberships were analyzed with multinomial logistic regression. Follow-up analyses targeted profiles and predictors specifically related to the rate of cognitive change. We identified three latent profiles of overall cognitive performance during the follow-up period with 31.6% of the sample having maintained high cognition, 50.6% having moderate cognitive decline, and 17.8% having accelerated cognitive decline. In multiadjusted analyses, maintained high cognition was predicted by female sex, higher education, and faster walking speed. Smoking, loneliness, and being an ε4 carrier were associated with a lower likelihood of maintained high cognition. Higher age, diagnosis of diabetes, depression, and carrying the apolipoprotein E ε4 allele increased the likelihood of accelerated cognitive decline. Factors at baseline that could significantly predict profile membership within the specific cognitive domains included age, sex, years of education, walking speed, diabetes, and the ε4 allele. Of note, these factors differed across cognitive domains. In sum, we identified demographic, lifestyle, health, and genetic factors of interindividual differences in domain-specific and general cognitive aging profiles, some of which are modifiable. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究旨在描述认知老化的特征,以及如何从人口、生活方式、健康和遗传因素的个体差异中预测这些特征。研究对象是1966名老年人(平均基线年龄=71.6岁;62.9%为女性),他们在基线年龄时没有痴呆症,在15年的随访期间至少接受过两次认知评估。认知评估包括语义和情节记忆、字母和类别流利度、感知速度和执行功能测试。首先,我们用线性混合效应模型估计了每个认知领域的水平和变化,并据此将样本分为 "认知能力保持较高水平"、"认知能力中度下降 "或 "认知能力加速下降 "的参与者。其次,我们通过多项式逻辑回归分析了每个认知领域内的组别成员资格的决定因素。第三,我们利用潜类分析法得出了每个认知领域内的一般认知老化特征。第四,通过多项式逻辑回归分析这些特征成员的决定因素。后续分析针对的是与认知变化率具体相关的特征和预测因素。我们确定了随访期间总体认知表现的三个潜在特征,其中 31.6% 的样本保持较高认知水平,50.6% 的样本认知水平中度下降,17.8% 的样本认知水平加速下降。在多重调整分析中,女性、高学历和较快的步行速度可预测认知能力保持在较高水平。吸烟、孤独和ε4携带者与较低的高认知能力维持可能性相关。年龄越大、诊断出糖尿病、抑郁以及携带载脂蛋白 E ε4等位基因,认知能力加速下降的可能性就越大。基线时能显著预测特定认知领域特征成员的因素包括年龄、性别、受教育年限、步行速度、糖尿病和ε4等位基因。值得注意的是,这些因素在不同认知领域存在差异。总之,我们发现了造成特定领域和一般认知老化特征个体间差异的人口、生活方式、健康和遗传因素,其中一些因素是可以改变的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Predictors of cognitive aging profiles over 15 years: A longitudinal population-based study.","authors":"Ingrid Ekström, Maria Josefsson, Lars Bäckman, Erika J Laukka","doi":"10.1037/pag0000807","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000807","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present study aimed to characterize profiles of cognitive aging and how these can be predicted from interindividual differences in demographic, lifestyle, health, and genetic factors. The participants were 1,966 older adults (mean baseline age = 71.6 years; 62.9% female), free from dementia at baseline and with at least two cognitive assessments over the 15-year follow-up, from the population-based Swedish National Study on Aging and Care in Kungsholmen. The cognitive assessment comprised tests of semantic and episodic memory, letter and category fluency, perceptual speed, and executive function. First, we estimated the level and change within each of the cognitive domains with linear mixed effect models, based on which we grouped our sample into participants with \"maintained high cognition,\" \"moderate cognitive decline,\" or \"accelerated cognitive decline.\" Second, we analyzed determinants of group membership within each cognitive domain with multinomial logistic regression. Third, group memberships within each cognitive domain were used to derive general cognitive aging profiles with latent class analysis. Fourth, the determinants of these profile memberships were analyzed with multinomial logistic regression. Follow-up analyses targeted profiles and predictors specifically related to the rate of cognitive change. We identified three latent profiles of overall cognitive performance during the follow-up period with 31.6% of the sample having maintained high cognition, 50.6% having moderate cognitive decline, and 17.8% having accelerated cognitive decline. In multiadjusted analyses, maintained high cognition was predicted by female sex, higher education, and faster walking speed. Smoking, loneliness, and being an ε4 carrier were associated with a lower likelihood of maintained high cognition. Higher age, diagnosis of diabetes, depression, and carrying the apolipoprotein E ε4 allele increased the likelihood of accelerated cognitive decline. Factors at baseline that could significantly predict profile membership within the specific cognitive domains included age, sex, years of education, walking speed, diabetes, and the ε4 allele. Of note, these factors differed across cognitive domains. In sum, we identified demographic, lifestyle, health, and genetic factors of interindividual differences in domain-specific and general cognitive aging profiles, some of which are modifiable. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140945315","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Flynn effect and cognitive decline among americans aged 65 years and older. 弗林效应与 65 岁及以上美国人的认知能力下降。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000833
Yun Zhang, Joseph Lee Rodgers, Patrick O'Keefe, Wei Hou, Stacey Voll, Graciela Muniz-Terrera, Linda Wänström, Frank Mann, Scott M Hofer, Sean A P Clouston

To contribute to our understanding of cohort differences and the Flynn effect in the cognitive decline among older Americans, this study aims to compare rates of cognitive decline between two birth cohorts within a study of older Americans and to examine the importance of medical and demographic confounders. Analyses used data from the National Health and Aging Trends Study (2011-2019), which recruited older Americans in 2011 and again in 2015 who were then followed for 5 years. We employed mixed-effect models to examine the linear and quadratic main and interaction effects of year of birth while adjusting for covariates such as annual round, sex/gender, education, race/ethnicity, heart disease, hypertension, diabetes, test unfamiliarity, and survey design. We analyzed data from 11,167 participants: 7,325 from 2011 to 2015 and 3,842 from 2015 to 2019. The cohort recruited in 2015 was born, on average, 5.33 years later than that recruited in 2011 and had higher functioning than the one recruited in 2011 across all observed cognitive domains that persisted after adjusting for covariates. In multivariable-adjusted analyses, a 1-year increase in year of birth was associated with increased episodic memory (β = 0.045, SE = 0.001, p < .001), orientation (β = 0.034, SE = 0.001, p < .001), and executive function (β = 0.036, SE = 0.001, p < .001). Participants born 1 year later had slower rates of decline in episodic memory (β = 0.004, SE = 0.000, p < .001), orientation (β = 0.003, SE = 0.000, p < .001), and executive function (β = 0.001, SE = 0.000, p = .002). Additionally, sex/gender modified this relationship for episodic memory (-0.007, SE = 0.002, p < .001), orientation (-0.005, SE = 0.002, p = .008), and executive function (-0.008, SE = 0.002, p < .001). These results demonstrate the persistence of the Flynn effect in old age across cognitive domains and identified a deceleration in the rate of cognitive decline across cognitive domains. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

为了帮助我们了解美国老年人认知能力下降的队列差异和弗林效应,本研究旨在比较美国老年人研究中两个出生队列之间的认知能力下降率,并考察医疗和人口混杂因素的重要性。分析使用了美国国家健康与老龄化趋势研究(2011-2019 年)的数据,该研究在 2011 年和 2015 年分别招募了美国老年人,并对他们进行了为期 5 年的跟踪调查。我们采用混合效应模型来检验出生年份的线性和二次主效应和交互效应,同时调整了年轮、性别、教育程度、种族/民族、心脏病、高血压、糖尿病、测试不熟悉程度和调查设计等协变量。我们分析了 11,167 名参与者的数据:其中 7,325 人来自 2011 年至 2015 年,3,842 人来自 2015 年至 2019 年。2015年招募的组群比2011年招募的组群平均晚出生5.33年,在所有观察到的认知领域中,2015年招募的组群比2011年招募的组群具有更高的功能,这种情况在调整协变量后依然存在。在多变量调整分析中,出生年份每增加一年,外显记忆(β = 0.045,SE = 0.001,p < .001)、定向力(β = 0.034,SE = 0.001,p < .001)和执行功能(β = 0.036,SE = 0.001,p < .001)就会增加。出生 1 年后的参与者在情节记忆(β = 0.004,SE = 0.000,p < .001)、定向力(β = 0.003,SE = 0.000,p < .001)和执行功能(β = 0.001,SE = 0.000,p = .002)方面的下降速度较慢。此外,性/性别改变了外显记忆(-0.007,SE = 0.002,p < .001)、定向力(-0.005,SE = 0.002,p = .008)和执行功能(-0.008,SE = 0.002,p < .001)的这种关系。这些结果表明,在老年期,弗林效应在各个认知领域都持续存在,并确定了各个认知领域的认知能力衰退速度减慢。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The Flynn effect and cognitive decline among americans aged 65 years and older.","authors":"Yun Zhang, Joseph Lee Rodgers, Patrick O'Keefe, Wei Hou, Stacey Voll, Graciela Muniz-Terrera, Linda Wänström, Frank Mann, Scott M Hofer, Sean A P Clouston","doi":"10.1037/pag0000833","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000833","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To contribute to our understanding of cohort differences and the Flynn effect in the cognitive decline among older Americans, this study aims to compare rates of cognitive decline between two birth cohorts within a study of older Americans and to examine the importance of medical and demographic confounders. Analyses used data from the National Health and Aging Trends Study (2011-2019), which recruited older Americans in 2011 and again in 2015 who were then followed for 5 years. We employed mixed-effect models to examine the linear and quadratic main and interaction effects of year of birth while adjusting for covariates such as annual round, sex/gender, education, race/ethnicity, heart disease, hypertension, diabetes, test unfamiliarity, and survey design. We analyzed data from 11,167 participants: 7,325 from 2011 to 2015 and 3,842 from 2015 to 2019. The cohort recruited in 2015 was born, on average, 5.33 years later than that recruited in 2011 and had higher functioning than the one recruited in 2011 across all observed cognitive domains that persisted after adjusting for covariates. In multivariable-adjusted analyses, a 1-year increase in year of birth was associated with increased episodic memory (β = 0.045, SE = 0.001, p < .001), orientation (β = 0.034, SE = 0.001, p < .001), and executive function (β = 0.036, SE = 0.001, p < .001). Participants born 1 year later had slower rates of decline in episodic memory (β = 0.004, SE = 0.000, p < .001), orientation (β = 0.003, SE = 0.000, p < .001), and executive function (β = 0.001, SE = 0.000, p = .002). Additionally, sex/gender modified this relationship for episodic memory (-0.007, SE = 0.002, p < .001), orientation (-0.005, SE = 0.002, p = .008), and executive function (-0.008, SE = 0.002, p < .001). These results demonstrate the persistence of the Flynn effect in old age across cognitive domains and identified a deceleration in the rate of cognitive decline across cognitive domains. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141761792","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Efficacy of the residential care transition module: A telehealth intervention for dementia family caregivers of relatives living in residential long-term care settings. 住院护理过渡模块的功效:针对居住在长期护理机构的痴呆症家庭照顾者的远程医疗干预。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000820
Joseph E Gaugler, Robyn W Birkeland, Elizabeth A Albers, Colleen M Peterson, Katie Louwagie, Zachary Baker, Mary S Mittelman, Kenneth Hepburn, David L Roth

The purpose of this study was to evaluate the efficacy of the Residential Care Transition Module, a six-session, psychosocial, and psychoeducational telehealth intervention for family caregivers of cognitively impaired relatives living in a residential long-term care setting. Eligible participants (including care recipients, regardless of time since admission) were randomized to treatment or usual care control conditions. Survey data were collected at baseline, 4 months, 8 months, and 12 months (N = 240). Primary analytic outcomes included caregiver subjective stress (a stress process mechanism) and depressive symptoms (a measure of global well-being). Secondary analytic outcomes included secondary role strains, residential care stress, caregiver sense of competence, and self-efficacy (additional mechanisms of action). General linear models tested for the main effects of the intervention at 4 months, and longitudinal mixed models examined the 12-month effects of the intervention. Post hoc analyses also examined the influence of moderators. No significant differences between the treatment and control groups for any primary analytic outcome were apparent. Caregivers in the treatment group whose relatives were admitted to residential long-term care in the prior 3 months were more likely to indicate reductions in depressive symptoms over the first 4 months of participation. Over the 12-month study period, caregivers in the treatment group who were employed reported increased self-efficacy over time. The heterogeneity of dementia care requires a broader consideration of key contextual factors that may influence the efficacy of nonpharmacological interventions. Aligning measures with the preferences, goals, and values of dementia caregivers may further demonstrate the direct benefits of interventions such as the Residential Care Transition Module. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究旨在评估 "寄宿护理过渡模块 "的疗效。该模块是一项针对长期寄宿护理环境中认知功能受损亲属的家庭照顾者的远程医疗干预措施,为期六节,包括社会心理和心理教育。符合条件的参与者(包括护理对象,无论其入院时间长短)被随机分配到治疗或常规护理对照组。在基线、4 个月、8 个月和 12 个月时收集调查数据(N = 240)。主要分析结果包括护理人员的主观压力(一种压力过程机制)和抑郁症状(一种总体幸福感测量指标)。次要分析结果包括次要角色压力、寄宿护理压力、护理者能力感和自我效能感(其他作用机制)。一般线性模型检验了干预措施在 4 个月内的主要效果,纵向混合模型检验了干预措施在 12 个月内的效果。事后分析还检验了调节因素的影响。在任何主要分析结果上,治疗组和对照组之间都没有明显的差异。在治疗组中,如果其亲属在前 3 个月曾入住长期护理机构,则其护理人员更有可能在参与治疗的前 4 个月中表示抑郁症状有所减轻。在为期 12 个月的研究期间,治疗组中有工作的护理人员的自我效能感随着时间的推移有所提高。痴呆症护理的异质性要求我们更广泛地考虑可能影响非药物干预疗效的关键背景因素。根据痴呆症照护者的偏好、目标和价值观来调整干预措施,可以进一步证明住宿照护过渡模块等干预措施的直接益处。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Efficacy of the residential care transition module: A telehealth intervention for dementia family caregivers of relatives living in residential long-term care settings.","authors":"Joseph E Gaugler, Robyn W Birkeland, Elizabeth A Albers, Colleen M Peterson, Katie Louwagie, Zachary Baker, Mary S Mittelman, Kenneth Hepburn, David L Roth","doi":"10.1037/pag0000820","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000820","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The purpose of this study was to evaluate the efficacy of the Residential Care Transition Module, a six-session, psychosocial, and psychoeducational telehealth intervention for family caregivers of cognitively impaired relatives living in a residential long-term care setting. Eligible participants (including care recipients, regardless of time since admission) were randomized to treatment or usual care control conditions. Survey data were collected at baseline, 4 months, 8 months, and 12 months (<i>N</i> = 240). Primary analytic outcomes included caregiver subjective stress (a stress process mechanism) and depressive symptoms (a measure of global well-being). Secondary analytic outcomes included secondary role strains, residential care stress, caregiver sense of competence, and self-efficacy (additional mechanisms of action). General linear models tested for the main effects of the intervention at 4 months, and longitudinal mixed models examined the 12-month effects of the intervention. Post hoc analyses also examined the influence of moderators. No significant differences between the treatment and control groups for any primary analytic outcome were apparent. Caregivers in the treatment group whose relatives were admitted to residential long-term care in the prior 3 months were more likely to indicate reductions in depressive symptoms over the first 4 months of participation. Over the 12-month study period, caregivers in the treatment group who were employed reported increased self-efficacy over time. The heterogeneity of dementia care requires a broader consideration of key contextual factors that may influence the efficacy of nonpharmacological interventions. Aligning measures with the preferences, goals, and values of dementia caregivers may further demonstrate the direct benefits of interventions such as the Residential Care Transition Module. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11552057/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140945287","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Impact of stroke on cognition in old age: Comparison of two population-based cohorts, born up to 30 years apart and followed from age 70 to 85. 中风对老年认知能力的影响:比较两组相差 30 年出生、70 至 85 岁随访的人群。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-23 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000824
Valgeir Thorvaldsson, Johan Skoog, Ingmar Skoog, Boo Johansson

Little is known about birth cohort differences in the impact of stroke on cognitive aging. Given improved poststroke rehabilitation and better treatments for vascular health risk, we may expect a reduction in the stroke impact in later-born cohorts. We tested this prediction using data from two cohorts, born in 1901-1907 (n = 1,155) and 1930 (n = 919), identified from the same city population at the same age of 70 and subsequently measured on the same cognitive outcomes (i.e., spatial ability, perceptual-motor speed, and reasoning) at ages 70, 75, 79, and 85. We fitted multiple-group second-order latent growth curve models to the data, regressing the first-order cognitive factor on the time-varying stroke variable and controlling for relevant covariates. Findings revealed moderate to large average cognitive decline (d = -.45) following stroke, and the impact was relatively similar across cohorts (1901-1907: d = -.52; 1930: d = -.39). However, there was a stroke by age by cohort interaction, implying that the stroke impact increased with age in the 1901-1907 cohort (dage ≤ 75 = -.42; dage ≥ 79 = -.70) but decreased in the 1930 cohort (dage ≤ 75 = -.53; dage ≥ 79 = -.17). We found no evidence for lagged effect of stroke beyond the impact on measures following the incidence. Our hypothesis was only partially supported, as the impact of stroke was reduced in the later-born cohort but solely at higher ages. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们对中风对认知老化影响的出生队列差异知之甚少。鉴于脑卒中后康复治疗的改善和血管健康风险治疗的改进,我们可以预期脑卒中对较晚出生队列的影响会减小。我们使用 1901-1907 年出生(n = 1,155 人)和 1930 年出生(n = 919 人)的两个队列的数据对这一预测进行了检验,这两个队列在 70 岁时从同一城市人口中识别出来,随后在 70、75、79 和 85 岁时进行了相同认知结果(即空间能力、感知运动速度和推理能力)的测量。我们对数据拟合了多组二阶潜增长曲线模型,将一阶认知因子与随时间变化的中风变量进行回归,并控制相关协变量。研究结果显示,中风后的平均认知能力下降幅度为中等至较大(d = -.45),不同组群之间的影响相对相似(1901-1907:d = -.52;1930:d = -.39)。然而,中风与年龄、队列之间存在交互作用,这意味着在 1901-1907 年队列中,中风的影响随着年龄的增长而增大(dage ≤ 75 = -.42; dage ≥ 79 = -.70),但在 1930 年队列中则减小(dage ≤ 75 = -.53; dage ≥ 79 = -.17)。除了对发病后各项指标的影响外,我们没有发现中风滞后效应的证据。我们的假设只得到了部分支持,因为中风对较晚出生组群的影响有所减弱,但仅对较高年龄组群有影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Impact of stroke on cognition in old age: Comparison of two population-based cohorts, born up to 30 years apart and followed from age 70 to 85.","authors":"Valgeir Thorvaldsson, Johan Skoog, Ingmar Skoog, Boo Johansson","doi":"10.1037/pag0000824","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000824","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Little is known about birth cohort differences in the impact of stroke on cognitive aging. Given improved poststroke rehabilitation and better treatments for vascular health risk, we may expect a reduction in the stroke impact in later-born cohorts. We tested this prediction using data from two cohorts, born in 1901-1907 (<i>n</i> = 1,155) and 1930 (<i>n</i> = 919), identified from the same city population at the same age of 70 and subsequently measured on the same cognitive outcomes (i.e., spatial ability, perceptual-motor speed, and reasoning) at ages 70, 75, 79, and 85. We fitted multiple-group second-order latent growth curve models to the data, regressing the first-order cognitive factor on the time-varying stroke variable and controlling for relevant covariates. Findings revealed moderate to large average cognitive decline (<i>d</i> = -.45) following stroke, and the impact was relatively similar across cohorts (1901-1907: <i>d</i> = -.52; 1930: <i>d</i> = -.39). However, there was a stroke by age by cohort interaction, implying that the stroke impact increased with age in the 1901-1907 cohort (<i>d</i><sub>age</sub> ≤ 75 = -.42; <i>d</i><sub>age</sub> ≥ 79 = -.70) but decreased in the 1930 cohort (dage ≤ 75 = -.53; <i>d</i><sub>age</sub> ≥ 79 = -.17). We found no evidence for lagged effect of stroke beyond the impact on measures following the incidence. Our hypothesis was only partially supported, as the impact of stroke was reduced in the later-born cohort but solely at higher ages. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141079620","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How old do I look? Aging appearance and experiences of aging among U.S. adults ages 50-80. 我看起来有多老?50-80 岁美国成年人的衰老外观和衰老体验。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-29 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000800
Julie Ober Allen, Valerie Moïse, Erica Solway, Marshall K Cheney, Daniel Joseph Larson, Preeti N Malani, Dianne Singer, Jeffrey T Kullgren

Appearance is an indicator of age and life stage, which are linked to socially salient stereotypes and prejudices. Older adults' appearance-related perceptions and behaviors may affect their experiences of aging within broader society, which may in turn influence health. This study examined associations between two measures related to aging appearance-assessment of one's aging appearance relative to same-age peers and investing time or effort to look younger-positive and negative experiences of aging, and health using multivariable regression. Cross-sectional data were from a nationally representative sample of 2006 U.S. adults ages 50-80 (Mage = 63, 52% women, 71% White) who completed Wave 6 of the National Poll on Healthy Aging in 2019. The majority (59%) reported appearing relatively younger than peers, while fewer reported appearing the same age (35%) or older (6%). About a third (35%) reported investing in looking younger. Appearing relatively younger was associated with more positive (p < .001) and less negative experiences of aging (p = .019). Appearing relatively older showed the opposite relationships (p values < .001). Investing in looking younger was associated with more positive and more negative experiences of aging (p values < .001). Few sociodemographic variations were detected. More positive and less negative experiences of aging were associated with better physical and mental health (p values < .001). While aging appearance is often the basis for jokes, it may affect the quality of older adults' experiences of aging and associated health outcomes. Nuanced findings caution against framing youthful biases in aging appearance and investments in looking younger as solely negative (or positive). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

外貌是年龄和生活阶段的指标,而年龄和生活阶段又与社会上突出的刻板印象和偏见有关。老年人与外貌相关的认知和行为可能会影响他们在更广泛的社会中的衰老体验,进而影响健康。本研究采用多元回归法考察了与衰老外观相关的两个测量指标--相对于同龄人对自己衰老外观的评估以及为使自己看起来更年轻而投入时间或精力--与衰老的积极和消极体验以及健康之间的关联。横截面数据来自于一个具有全国代表性的样本,该样本包含 2006 名年龄在 50-80 岁之间的美国成年人(年龄 = 63,52% 为女性,71% 为白人),他们在 2019 年完成了第 6 波全国健康老龄化民意调查。大多数人(59%)表示自己看起来比同龄人相对年轻,而较少人(35%)表示自己看起来与同龄人年龄相当或更老(6%)。约有三分之一(35%)的人表示投资于让自己看起来更年轻。看起来相对年轻与对衰老的积极体验较多(p < .001)和消极体验较少(p = .019)有关。看起来相对较老则显示出相反的关系(p 值 < .001)。投资让自己看起来更年轻与更积极和更消极的衰老体验相关(p 值 < .001)。几乎没有发现社会人口学方面的差异。更积极和更消极的衰老体验与更好的身心健康有关(p 值 < .001)。虽然衰老的外表往往是笑话的基础,但它可能会影响老年人衰老体验的质量和相关的健康结果。细致入微的研究结果告诫我们,不要把衰老外表中的青春偏见和对看起来更年轻的投资仅仅归结为负面(或正面)。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"How old do I look? Aging appearance and experiences of aging among U.S. adults ages 50-80.","authors":"Julie Ober Allen, Valerie Moïse, Erica Solway, Marshall K Cheney, Daniel Joseph Larson, Preeti N Malani, Dianne Singer, Jeffrey T Kullgren","doi":"10.1037/pag0000800","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000800","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Appearance is an indicator of age and life stage, which are linked to socially salient stereotypes and prejudices. Older adults' appearance-related perceptions and behaviors may affect their experiences of aging within broader society, which may in turn influence health. This study examined associations between two measures related to aging appearance-assessment of one's aging appearance relative to same-age peers and investing time or effort to look younger-positive and negative experiences of aging, and health using multivariable regression. Cross-sectional data were from a nationally representative sample of 2006 U.S. adults ages 50-80 (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 63, 52% women, 71% White) who completed Wave 6 of the National Poll on Healthy Aging in 2019. The majority (59%) reported appearing relatively younger than peers, while fewer reported appearing the same age (35%) or older (6%). About a third (35%) reported investing in looking younger. Appearing relatively younger was associated with more positive (<i>p</i> < .001) and less negative experiences of aging (<i>p</i> = .019). Appearing relatively older showed the opposite relationships (<i>p</i> values < .001). Investing in looking younger was associated with more positive and more negative experiences of aging (<i>p</i> values < .001). Few sociodemographic variations were detected. More positive and less negative experiences of aging were associated with better physical and mental health (<i>p</i> values < .001). While aging appearance is often the basis for jokes, it may affect the quality of older adults' experiences of aging and associated health outcomes. Nuanced findings caution against framing youthful biases in aging appearance and investments in looking younger as solely negative (or positive). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139997921","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychology and Aging
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1