首页 > 最新文献

Psychology and Aging最新文献

英文 中文
Delayed onset of cognitive terminal decline in later born cohorts: Evidence from a longitudinal study of two cohorts born 29-years apart. 出生较晚的人群认知能力终末衰退的起始时间较晚:对相隔 29 年出生的两个群体进行纵向研究的证据。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-05 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000846
Valgeir Thorvaldsson, Ingmar Skoog, Boo Johansson

In this study, we evaluated birth cohort (i.e., generational) differences in the onset and rate of acceleration in cognitive decline prior to death (i.e., terminal decline [TD]). We obtained data from two cohorts, born in 1901-1902 (n = 755, 64% females) and 1930 (n = 347, 48% females), identified and sampled at age 70 from the same city population and assessed on the same cognitive tests at ages 70, 75, 79, 85, and 88. The 1901-1902 cohort was additionally assessed at ages 90, 92, 95, 97, 99, and 100. The outcome was defined at each measurement occasion by a composite score of three cognitive tests assessing spatial ability, perceptual and motor speed, and reasoning. Date of death was obtained from population register, with the last update in April 2023, covering over 99% and 38% of the cohort members, respectively. We fitted a random TD change point model to the data while accounting for sex and education. Findings revealed expected TD onset (as conditioned on male with formal education) 5.17 (95% highest density interval [2.54, 8.05]) years prior to death in the 1901-1902 cohort, with an acceleration in rate of decline by a factor of 4.43 within the TD phase. This estimate was delayed by 2.53 (95% highest density interval [5.68, 0.10]) years in the 1930 cohort, with an acceleration by a factor of 5.16 within the TD phase. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to present evidence indicating that today's birth cohort experience, on average, a shorter TD phase in comparison to earlier born cohorts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

在这项研究中,我们评估了出生队列(即世代)在死亡前认知能力衰退(即终末衰退 [TD])的开始和加速率方面的差异。我们从 1901-1902 年(n = 755,64% 为女性)和 1930 年(n = 347,48% 为女性)出生的两个队列中获得了数据,这两个队列在 70 岁时从同一个城市人口中识别和抽样,并在 70、75、79、85 和 88 岁时接受相同的认知测试评估。此外,还在 90、92、95、97、99 和 100 岁时对 1901-1902 年组群进行了评估。每次测量的结果均由三项认知测试的综合得分确定,这三项测试分别评估空间能力、感知和运动速度以及推理能力。死亡日期从人口登记册中获得,最近一次更新是在2023年4月,分别覆盖了队列中超过99%和38%的成员。我们对数据进行了随机 TD 变点模型拟合,同时考虑了性别和教育程度。结果显示,在 1901-1902 年队列中,预计 TD 发病时间(以受过正规教育的男性为条件)比死亡时间早 5.17 年(95% 最高密度区间 [2.54, 8.05]),在 TD 阶段,下降速度加快了 4.43 倍。在 1930 年队列中,这一估计值延迟了 2.53 年(95% 最高密度区间 [5.68, 0.10]),在 TD 阶段加速了 5.16 倍。据我们所知,这是首次有证据表明,与较早出生的人群相比,今天出生的人群平均经历的 TD 阶段较短的研究。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Delayed onset of cognitive terminal decline in later born cohorts: Evidence from a longitudinal study of two cohorts born 29-years apart.","authors":"Valgeir Thorvaldsson, Ingmar Skoog, Boo Johansson","doi":"10.1037/pag0000846","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000846","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this study, we evaluated birth cohort (i.e., generational) differences in the onset and rate of acceleration in cognitive decline prior to death (i.e., terminal decline [TD]). We obtained data from two cohorts, born in 1901-1902 (<i>n</i> = 755, 64% females) and 1930 (<i>n</i> = 347, 48% females), identified and sampled at age 70 from the same city population and assessed on the same cognitive tests at ages 70, 75, 79, 85, and 88. The 1901-1902 cohort was additionally assessed at ages 90, 92, 95, 97, 99, and 100. The outcome was defined at each measurement occasion by a composite score of three cognitive tests assessing spatial ability, perceptual and motor speed, and reasoning. Date of death was obtained from population register, with the last update in April 2023, covering over 99% and 38% of the cohort members, respectively. We fitted a random TD change point model to the data while accounting for sex and education. Findings revealed expected TD onset (as conditioned on male with formal education) 5.17 (95% highest density interval [2.54, 8.05]) years prior to death in the 1901-1902 cohort, with an acceleration in rate of decline by a factor of 4.43 within the TD phase. This estimate was delayed by 2.53 (95% highest density interval [5.68, 0.10]) years in the 1930 cohort, with an acceleration by a factor of 5.16 within the TD phase. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to present evidence indicating that today's birth cohort experience, on average, a shorter TD phase in comparison to earlier born cohorts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":"109-116"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141375","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Advancing with age: Older adults excel in comprehension of novel metaphors. 随着年龄的增长而进步:老年人在理解新奇隐喻方面表现出色。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000836
Nicholas Ichien, Dušan Stamenković, Mary C Whatley, Alan D Castel, Keith J Holyoak

Older adults may experience certain forms of cognitive decline, but some forms of semantic memory remain intact in older age. To address how metaphor comprehension changes with age and whether metaphor comprehension relies more heavily on analogical reasoning (supported by fluid intelligence) or on conceptual combination (supported by crystalized intelligence), we compared performance of younger and older adults. In two experiments, healthy older adults (54-88 years) scored lower on a measure of fluid intelligence (Ravens Progressive Matrices) but higher on a measure of crystalized intelligence (Mill Hill Vocabulary Test) relative to younger adults (18-34 years). Groups were equally successful in comprehending relatively easy metaphors (Study 1), but older adults showed a striking advantage over younger adults for novel literary metaphors (Study 2). Mixed-effects modeling showed that measures of fluid and crystalized intelligence each made separable contributions to metaphor comprehension for both groups, but older adults relied more on crystalized intelligence than did younger adults. These age-related dissociations clarify cognitive effects of aging and highlight the importance of crystalized intelligence for metaphor comprehension in both younger and older adults. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

老年人可能会出现某些形式的认知能力衰退,但某些形式的语义记忆在老年时仍然完好无损。为了解决隐喻理解如何随着年龄的增长而变化,以及隐喻理解是更依赖于类比推理(由流体智力支持)还是概念组合(由晶体智力支持)的问题,我们比较了年轻人和老年人的表现。在两项实验中,健康的老年人(54-88 岁)与年轻人(18-34 岁)相比,在流体智力测量(瑞文累进矩阵)中得分较低,但在晶体化智力测量(米尔希尔词汇测试)中得分较高。在理解相对容易的隐喻方面,各组的成功率相当(研究 1),但在理解新颖的文学隐喻方面,老年人比年轻人有明显优势(研究 2)。混合效应模型显示,流体智力和晶体智力对两组人的隐喻理解能力都有不同的贡献,但老年人比年轻人更依赖晶体智力。这些与年龄相关的差异澄清了衰老对认知的影响,并强调了晶体化智力对年轻人和老年人隐喻理解的重要性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Advancing with age: Older adults excel in comprehension of novel metaphors.","authors":"Nicholas Ichien, Dušan Stamenković, Mary C Whatley, Alan D Castel, Keith J Holyoak","doi":"10.1037/pag0000836","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000836","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Older adults may experience certain forms of cognitive decline, but some forms of semantic memory remain intact in older age. To address how metaphor comprehension changes with age and whether metaphor comprehension relies more heavily on analogical reasoning (supported by fluid intelligence) or on conceptual combination (supported by crystalized intelligence), we compared performance of younger and older adults. In two experiments, healthy older adults (54-88 years) scored lower on a measure of fluid intelligence (Ravens Progressive Matrices) but higher on a measure of crystalized intelligence (Mill Hill Vocabulary Test) relative to younger adults (18-34 years). Groups were equally successful in comprehending relatively easy metaphors (Study 1), but older adults showed a striking advantage over younger adults for novel literary metaphors (Study 2). Mixed-effects modeling showed that measures of fluid and crystalized intelligence each made separable contributions to metaphor comprehension for both groups, but older adults relied more on crystalized intelligence than did younger adults. These age-related dissociations clarify cognitive effects of aging and highlight the importance of crystalized intelligence for metaphor comprehension in both younger and older adults. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":"6-16"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141447380","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Outliving oneself through the next generations: (grand)parenthood and values in later life.
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-23 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000879
Suzana Ignjatović, Zoran Pavlović, Bojan Todosijević

The article explores personal values among older adults in relation to their "offspring status." Erikson's theory of psychosocial development and Schwartz's theory of human values suggest a positive relationship between having offspring and prosocial values. We tested this hypothesis by comparing older adults who have none, one, or two generations of descendants: childless (with no descendants), grandchildless (with adult children and no grandchildren), and grandparents. We conducted a hierarchical multilevel regression analysis using data from Round 9 of the European Social Survey on a subsample of 12,713 respondents older than 65 from 29 European countries. Our model predicted preferences for four of the Schwartz higher order value types (Self-Transcendence, Self-Enhancement, Openness to Change, and Conservation) among the three categories of older adults based on their offspring status, controlling for individual-level (gender, education level, general health) and macro-level variables (Human Development Index). All but Openness to Change values are significantly predicted by older adults' offspring status. Having grandchildren is positively associated with Self-Transcendence and Conservation values (socially oriented values) and negatively with Self-Enhancement (values with self-oriented focus). The article discusses the theoretical implications of the obtained findings, focusing on plausible mechanisms connecting the offspring status and value orientations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

{"title":"Outliving oneself through the next generations: (grand)parenthood and values in later life.","authors":"Suzana Ignjatović, Zoran Pavlović, Bojan Todosijević","doi":"10.1037/pag0000879","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000879","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The article explores personal values among older adults in relation to their \"offspring status.\" Erikson's theory of psychosocial development and Schwartz's theory of human values suggest a positive relationship between having offspring and prosocial values. We tested this hypothesis by comparing older adults who have none, one, or two generations of descendants: childless (with no descendants), grandchildless (with adult children and no grandchildren), and grandparents. We conducted a hierarchical multilevel regression analysis using data from Round 9 of the European Social Survey on a subsample of 12,713 respondents older than 65 from 29 European countries. Our model predicted preferences for four of the Schwartz higher order value types (Self-Transcendence, Self-Enhancement, Openness to Change, and Conservation) among the three categories of older adults based on their offspring status, controlling for individual-level (gender, education level, general health) and macro-level variables (Human Development Index). All but Openness to Change values are significantly predicted by older adults' offspring status. Having grandchildren is positively associated with Self-Transcendence and Conservation values (socially oriented values) and negatively with Self-Enhancement (values with self-oriented focus). The article discusses the theoretical implications of the obtained findings, focusing on plausible mechanisms connecting the offspring status and value orientations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2025-01-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143025338","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Self-perceptions of aging and social goals.
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-23 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000881
Christina Ristl, Selma Korlat, Fiona S Rupprecht, Alexandra Burgstaller, Jana Nikitin

Existing research shows a clear link between the way people experience the aging process (i.e., self-perceptions of aging) and their health, cognitive abilities, and well-being. In contrast, there is little research on the association between self-perceptions of aging and social functioning. This study examines the relationship between awareness of age-related gains and losses as two facets of self-perceptions of aging and social approach and avoidance goals. Social approach goals (i.e., goals that focus on positive, desirable social outcomes) and social avoidance goals (i.e., goals that focus on negative, undesirable social outcomes) play a fundamental role in shaping individuals' social experiences and behaviors. We hypothesize that awareness of age-related gains is associated with social approach goals, whereas awareness of age-related losses is associated with social avoidance goals. In an experience sampling study with N = 117 participants aged 18-85 years (M = 40.4, SD = 17.1), we examined interindividual differences in participants' awareness of age-related gains and losses and how these relate to social goals. We focused on social goals in everyday social situations (aggregated state) and as individual dispositions (trait). As hypothesized, awareness of age-related gains was positively associated with social approach (but not avoidance) goals, while awareness of age-related losses was positively associated with social avoidance (but not approach) goals. These associations largely persisted even after controlling for age. These findings suggest that perceptions of age-related gains and losses are important factors in how people navigate their social lives. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

{"title":"Self-perceptions of aging and social goals.","authors":"Christina Ristl, Selma Korlat, Fiona S Rupprecht, Alexandra Burgstaller, Jana Nikitin","doi":"10.1037/pag0000881","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000881","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Existing research shows a clear link between the way people experience the aging process (i.e., self-perceptions of aging) and their health, cognitive abilities, and well-being. In contrast, there is little research on the association between self-perceptions of aging and social functioning. This study examines the relationship between awareness of age-related gains and losses as two facets of self-perceptions of aging and social approach and avoidance goals. Social approach goals (i.e., goals that focus on positive, desirable social outcomes) and social avoidance goals (i.e., goals that focus on negative, undesirable social outcomes) play a fundamental role in shaping individuals' social experiences and behaviors. We hypothesize that awareness of age-related gains is associated with social approach goals, whereas awareness of age-related losses is associated with social avoidance goals. In an experience sampling study with <i>N</i> = 117 participants aged 18-85 years (<i>M</i> = 40.4, <i>SD</i> = 17.1), we examined interindividual differences in participants' awareness of age-related gains and losses and how these relate to social goals. We focused on social goals in everyday social situations (aggregated state) and as individual dispositions (trait). As hypothesized, awareness of age-related gains was positively associated with social approach (but not avoidance) goals, while awareness of age-related losses was positively associated with social avoidance (but not approach) goals. These associations largely persisted even after controlling for age. These findings suggest that perceptions of age-related gains and losses are important factors in how people navigate their social lives. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2025-01-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143025252","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Younger adults may be faster at making semantic predictions, but older adults are more efficient. 年轻人在进行语义预测方面可能更快,但老年人的效率更高。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-16 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000872
Leigh B Fernandez, Muzna Shehzad, Lauren V Hadley

While there is strong evidence that younger adults use contextual information to generate semantic predictions, findings from older adults are less clear. Age affects cognition in a variety of different ways that may impact prediction mechanisms; while the efficiency of memory systems and processing speed decrease, life experience leads to complementary increases in vocabulary size, real-world knowledge, and even inhibitory control. Using the visual world paradigm, we tested prediction in younger (n = 30, between 18 and 35 years of age) and older adults (n = 30, between 53 and 78 years of age). Importantly, we differentiated early stage predictions based on simple spreading activation from the more resource-intensive tailoring of predictions when additional constraining information is provided. We found that older adults were slower than younger adults in generating early stage predictions but then quicker than younger adults to tailor those predictions given additional information. This suggests that while age may lead to delays in first activating relevant lexical items when listening to speech, increased linguistic experience nonetheless increases the efficiency with which contextual information is used. These findings are consistent with reports of age having positive as well as negative impacts on cognition and suggest conflation of different stages of prediction as a basis for the inconsistency in the aging-related literature to date. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

虽然有强有力的证据表明,年轻人使用上下文信息来产生语义预测,但老年人的研究结果不太清楚。年龄以各种不同的方式影响认知,可能影响预测机制;当记忆系统的效率和处理速度下降时,生活经验会导致词汇量、现实世界知识甚至抑制控制的互补增加。使用视觉世界范式,我们测试了年轻人(n = 30,年龄在18到35岁之间)和老年人(n = 30,年龄在53到78岁之间)的预测。重要的是,我们区分了基于简单扩散激活的早期预测和当提供额外约束信息时更资源密集的预测剪裁。我们发现,老年人在做出早期预测方面比年轻人慢,但在提供额外信息的情况下,他们在调整预测方面比年轻人快。这表明,虽然年龄可能导致在听演讲时首次激活相关词汇项目的时间延迟,但语言经验的增加却提高了使用上下文信息的效率。这些发现与年龄对认知有积极和消极影响的报告相一致,并建议将不同阶段的预测合并为迄今为止与衰老相关的文献中不一致的基础。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Younger adults may be faster at making semantic predictions, but older adults are more efficient.","authors":"Leigh B Fernandez, Muzna Shehzad, Lauren V Hadley","doi":"10.1037/pag0000872","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000872","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>While there is strong evidence that younger adults use contextual information to generate semantic predictions, findings from older adults are less clear. Age affects cognition in a variety of different ways that may impact prediction mechanisms; while the efficiency of memory systems and processing speed decrease, life experience leads to complementary increases in vocabulary size, real-world knowledge, and even inhibitory control. Using the visual world paradigm, we tested prediction in younger (<i>n</i> = 30, between 18 and 35 years of age) and older adults (<i>n</i> = 30, between 53 and 78 years of age). Importantly, we differentiated early stage predictions based on simple spreading activation from the more resource-intensive tailoring of predictions when additional constraining information is provided. We found that older adults were slower than younger adults in generating early stage predictions but then quicker than younger adults to tailor those predictions given additional information. This suggests that while age may lead to delays in first activating relevant lexical items when listening to speech, increased linguistic experience nonetheless increases the efficiency with which contextual information is used. These findings are consistent with reports of age having positive as well as negative impacts on cognition and suggest conflation of different stages of prediction as a basis for the inconsistency in the aging-related literature to date. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2025-01-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143014041","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring semantic expression disparities in intragenerational and intergenerational communication: A novel perspective on socioemotional selectivity theory. 探讨代际和代际交际中的语义表达差异:社会情感选择理论的新视角。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-13 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000877
Peng-Yu Zeng, Su-Ling Yeh

The Socioemotional Selectivity Theory (SST) posits that older and younger adults have different life goals due to differences in perceived remaining lifetime. Younger adults focus more on future-oriented knowledge exploration and forming new friendships, while older adults prioritize present-focused emotional regulation and maintaining close relationships. While previous research has found these age differences manifest in autobiographical textual expressions, their presence in verbal communication remains unexplored. We recruited 36 older adults and 36 younger adults to form 12 younger adults-younger adults dyads, 12 older adults-older adults dyads, and 12 younger adults-older adults dyads engaging in three conversational scenarios. Analysis of communication transcripts using Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count revealed that in intragenerational communication, older adults used more present-focused and family-related words, and fewer future-focused, negative emotion, and knowledge acquisition words compared to younger adults, aligning with SST predictions. However, in intergenerational communication, younger and older adults showed similar patterns in future-focused, cognitive, and family-related language use. This suggests that participants adjusted their goal orientations to accommodate each other, mitigating the age differences proposed by SST and partially supporting the Interpersonal Cognitive Consistency Model. These findings indicate that while SST explains semantic expressions in intragenerational communication, group dynamics toward verbal consistency play a more vital role in intergenerational communication. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

社会情绪选择理论(SST)认为,老年人和年轻人由于感知剩余寿命的差异而具有不同的生活目标。年轻人更注重面向未来的知识探索和建立新的友谊,而老年人更注重以现在为中心的情绪调节和保持亲密关系。虽然之前的研究发现,这些年龄差异在自传体文本表达中表现出来,但它们在口头交流中的存在仍未得到探索。我们招募了36名老年人和36名年轻人,组成12名年轻人-年轻人组,12名老年人-老年人组,以及12名年轻人-老年人组,参与三种对话场景。使用语言调查和字数统计对交流记录进行分析发现,在代际交流中,与年轻人相比,老年人使用更多以现在为中心和家庭相关的词汇,而较少使用以未来为中心、消极情绪和知识获取相关的词汇,这与SST预测一致。然而,在代际交流中,年轻人和老年人在关注未来、认知和家庭相关的语言使用方面表现出相似的模式。这表明被试调整了目标取向以适应彼此,减轻了SST提出的年龄差异,部分支持了人际认知一致性模型。这些发现表明,虽然SST解释了代际交际中的语义表达,但语言一致性的群体动力学在代际交际中起着更为重要的作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Exploring semantic expression disparities in intragenerational and intergenerational communication: A novel perspective on socioemotional selectivity theory.","authors":"Peng-Yu Zeng, Su-Ling Yeh","doi":"10.1037/pag0000877","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000877","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Socioemotional Selectivity Theory (SST) posits that older and younger adults have different life goals due to differences in perceived remaining lifetime. Younger adults focus more on future-oriented knowledge exploration and forming new friendships, while older adults prioritize present-focused emotional regulation and maintaining close relationships. While previous research has found these age differences manifest in autobiographical textual expressions, their presence in verbal communication remains unexplored. We recruited 36 older adults and 36 younger adults to form 12 younger adults-younger adults dyads, 12 older adults-older adults dyads, and 12 younger adults-older adults dyads engaging in three conversational scenarios. Analysis of communication transcripts using Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count revealed that in intragenerational communication, older adults used more present-focused and family-related words, and fewer future-focused, negative emotion, and knowledge acquisition words compared to younger adults, aligning with SST predictions. However, in intergenerational communication, younger and older adults showed similar patterns in future-focused, cognitive, and family-related language use. This suggests that participants adjusted their goal orientations to accommodate each other, mitigating the age differences proposed by SST and partially supporting the Interpersonal Cognitive Consistency Model. These findings indicate that while SST explains semantic expressions in intragenerational communication, group dynamics toward verbal consistency play a more vital role in intergenerational communication. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2025-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142972784","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Developmental invariance in deep distortions. 深度扭曲中的发展不变性。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-13 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000869
C J Brainerd, D M Bialer, X Liu, M Chang

Recently, a distinction has been drawn between conventional false memories, which misrepresent specific facts, and deep distortions, which misrepresent relations that connect facts. We report the first study of adult developmental trends in deep distortions, using a paradigm in which people make conjoint recognition judgments about incompatible facts (e.g., Was Einstein born in Austria, Germany, or Switzerland?). As conventional false memories increase over the adult lifespan, it is natural to expect that deep distortions will do likewise. Surprisingly, however, the modal explanation of adult increases in false memory predicts that deep distortions will be developmentally invariant. We tested that prediction in two experiments that measured three deep distortions (violations of the logical laws of additivity, countable additivity, and universal event) in memory for real-world incompatibility relations (e.g., size of planets, geographical location of companies, people in historical events). In Experiment 1, robust violations of all three laws were detected in younger adults (N = 105; Mage = 20), and as predicted, those violations did not increase in adults (N = 182; Mage = 33) or older adults (N = 176; Mage = 62). Experiment 2 was designed to test whether deep distortions would increase with age when there was stronger support for retrieving verbatim memories, but once again, deep distortion levels were the same in young adults (N = 81; Mage = 19), adults (N = 167; Mage = 34), and older adults (N = 170; Mage = 62). Conjoint recognition analyses revealed that throughout the adult lifespan, verbatim memory played no role in deep distortions. Other analyses revealed that although incompatible facts are perfectly compensatory in the real world (Einstein could only be born in Germany to the extent that he was not born in Austria or Switzerland), memory for incompatible facts is noncompensatory throughout the adult lifespan. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

最近,人们对传统错误记忆和深度扭曲记忆进行了区分,前者歪曲了具体事实,后者歪曲了联系事实的关系。我们报告了第一个关于深度扭曲的成人发展趋势的研究,使用了一个范式,在这个范式中,人们对不相容的事实做出共同的识别判断(例如,爱因斯坦是出生在奥地利、德国还是瑞士?)由于传统的错误记忆会在成年人的一生中不断增加,我们很自然地预计深度扭曲也会如此。然而,令人惊讶的是,对成人错误记忆增加的模态解释预测,深度扭曲将是发展不变的。我们在两个实验中测试了这一预测,这些实验测量了现实世界不相容关系(例如,行星的大小、公司的地理位置、历史事件中的人物)记忆中的三种深度扭曲(违反可加性、可数可加性和普遍事件的逻辑规律)。在实验1中,年轻人明显违反了这三条定律(N = 105;Mage = 20),正如预测的那样,这些违规行为在成人中没有增加(N = 182;33)或老年人(N = 176;法师= 62)。实验2旨在测试深度扭曲是否会随着年龄的增长而增加,当有更强的逐字记忆支持检索时,但同样,深度扭曲水平在年轻人中是相同的(N = 81;法师= 19),成人(N = 167;老年人(N = 170;法师= 62)。联合识别分析显示,在整个成年人的一生中,逐字记忆在深度扭曲中没有作用。其他分析显示,尽管不相容的事实在现实世界中是完全补偿的(爱因斯坦只有在他不是出生在奥地利或瑞士的情况下才可能出生在德国),但对不相容事实的记忆在整个成年人的一生中都是不补偿的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Developmental invariance in deep distortions.","authors":"C J Brainerd, D M Bialer, X Liu, M Chang","doi":"10.1037/pag0000869","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000869","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recently, a distinction has been drawn between conventional false memories, which misrepresent specific facts, and deep distortions, which misrepresent relations that connect facts. We report the first study of adult developmental trends in deep distortions, using a paradigm in which people make conjoint recognition judgments about incompatible facts (e.g., Was Einstein born in Austria, Germany, or Switzerland?). As conventional false memories increase over the adult lifespan, it is natural to expect that deep distortions will do likewise. Surprisingly, however, the modal explanation of adult increases in false memory predicts that deep distortions will be developmentally invariant. We tested that prediction in two experiments that measured three deep distortions (violations of the logical laws of additivity, countable additivity, and universal event) in memory for real-world incompatibility relations (e.g., size of planets, geographical location of companies, people in historical events). In Experiment 1, robust violations of all three laws were detected in younger adults (<i>N</i> = 105; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 20), and as predicted, those violations did not increase in adults (<i>N</i> = 182; Mage = 33) or older adults (<i>N</i> = 176; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 62). Experiment 2 was designed to test whether deep distortions would increase with age when there was stronger support for retrieving verbatim memories, but once again, deep distortion levels were the same in young adults (<i>N</i> = 81; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 19), adults (<i>N</i> = 167; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 34), and older adults (<i>N</i> = 170; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 62). Conjoint recognition analyses revealed that throughout the adult lifespan, verbatim memory played no role in deep distortions. Other analyses revealed that although incompatible facts are perfectly compensatory in the real world (Einstein could only be born in Germany to the extent that he was not born in Austria or Switzerland), memory for incompatible facts is noncompensatory throughout the adult lifespan. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2025-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142972782","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The contribution of general intelligence to cognitive performance across the lifespan: A differentiation analysis of the wechsler tests. 在整个生命周期中,一般智力对认知表现的贡献:韦氏测验的差异分析。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-13 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000875
Moritz Breit, Martin Brunner, Julian Preuß, Monika Daseking, Franz Pauls, Franziska Walter, Franzis Preckel

Human cognitive abilities exhibit positive interrelationships that can be represented by a latent general intelligence factor (g). Differentiation hypotheses propose that there are systematic interindividual differences in the strength of g, specifically along the dimensions of ability level (ability differentiation) and age (age differentiation). Despite the potential implications for cognitive theory and assessment, the available evidence on the matter is inconclusive. We present comprehensive analyses of differentiation effects across the lifespan, drawing on the meta-analytic integration of nonlinear factor analyses with German standardization samples (N = 4,129) of the most widely used intelligence tests worldwide (i.e., the Wechsler tests). Results support ability differentiation at all ages, with particularly large effect sizes in young adults, and suggest a complex pattern of age differentiation and dedifferentiation across the lifespan. These findings challenge the uniformity of g, highlighting the need to account for differentiation effects in cognitive theories and assessment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

人类的认知能力表现出积极的相互关系,可以用潜在的一般智力因素(g)来表示。分化假说认为,g 的强度在个体间存在系统性差异,特别是在能力水平(能力分化)和年龄(年龄分化)两个维度上。尽管这对认知理论和评估有潜在的影响,但现有的相关证据还没有定论。我们通过对全球使用最广泛的智力测验(即韦氏测验)的德国标准化样本(N = 4 129)进行非线性因子分析的元分析整合,对整个生命周期的分化效应进行了全面分析。结果表明,所有年龄段都存在能力分化,尤其是青壮年的效应大小更大,并表明整个生命周期都存在复杂的年龄分化和去分化模式。这些发现对 g 的统一性提出了挑战,强调了在认知理论和评估中考虑分化效应的必要性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The contribution of general intelligence to cognitive performance across the lifespan: A differentiation analysis of the wechsler tests.","authors":"Moritz Breit, Martin Brunner, Julian Preuß, Monika Daseking, Franz Pauls, Franziska Walter, Franzis Preckel","doi":"10.1037/pag0000875","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000875","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Human cognitive abilities exhibit positive interrelationships that can be represented by a latent general intelligence factor (g). Differentiation hypotheses propose that there are systematic interindividual differences in the strength of g, specifically along the dimensions of ability level (ability differentiation) and age (age differentiation). Despite the potential implications for cognitive theory and assessment, the available evidence on the matter is inconclusive. We present comprehensive analyses of differentiation effects across the lifespan, drawing on the meta-analytic integration of nonlinear factor analyses with German standardization samples (<i>N</i> = 4,129) of the most widely used intelligence tests worldwide (i.e., the Wechsler tests). Results support ability differentiation at all ages, with particularly large effect sizes in young adults, and suggest a complex pattern of age differentiation and dedifferentiation across the lifespan. These findings challenge the uniformity of g, highlighting the need to account for differentiation effects in cognitive theories and assessment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2025-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142972786","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Selectivity in prosociality among older adults: The moderation effect of self- and other-oriented motivation. 老年人亲社会行为的选择性:自我和他人导向动机的调节作用。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-06 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000873
Hongmei Lin, Yi-Long Lu, Li Li, Jian Li, Xin Zhang, Helene H Fung

Prosociality tends to increase with age, but whether older adults (OA) are more willing than younger adults (YA) to extend their prosocial behaviors beyond close social circles remains a topic of debate. This study aimed to address this controversy and explore the underlying mechanisms of age-related differences in prosociality through the lens of social discounting and gain-and-loss framing. One hundred twenty-three younger adults and 135 older adults participated in a social discounting task (measuring prosocial tendencies toward various social relationships) with various framings (self-oriented framing, other-oriented framing, and control condition). Compared to younger adults, older adults exhibited higher overall prosociality and treated socially close and distant others more evenly, indicating lower levels of selectivity in prosociality. Notably, the interaction effect between age and framing revealed that other-oriented framing amplified the prosocial tendencies of older adults, particularly toward socially distant others but not younger adults. These findings suggest other-oriented framing specifically reduced older adults' selectivity in prosociality, highlighting their prioritization of others' welfare and aversion to others' losses. This supports the notion that older adults' prosociality may be driven by other-oriented motivation rather than self-interest. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

亲社会倾向随着年龄的增长而增加,但老年人(OA)是否比年轻人(YA)更愿意将他们的亲社会行为扩展到亲密的社交圈之外,仍然是一个有争议的话题。本研究旨在解决这一争议,并通过社会折现和得失框架的视角探讨亲社会性年龄相关差异的潜在机制。在不同的框架(自我导向框架、他人导向框架和控制条件)下,123名年轻人和135名老年人参与了社会贴现任务(测量对各种社会关系的亲社会倾向)。与年轻人相比,老年人表现出更高的整体亲社会性,对待社会亲密和疏远的人更均匀,表明亲社会性的选择性水平较低。值得注意的是,年龄和框架之间的交互作用表明,其他导向框架放大了老年人的亲社会倾向,特别是对社会疏远的其他人,而不是年轻人。这些发现表明,以他人为导向的框架特别降低了老年人亲社会的选择性,突出了他们对他人福利的优先考虑和对他人损失的厌恶。这支持了这样一种观点,即老年人的亲社会行为可能是由他人导向的动机而不是自身利益驱动的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Selectivity in prosociality among older adults: The moderation effect of self- and other-oriented motivation.","authors":"Hongmei Lin, Yi-Long Lu, Li Li, Jian Li, Xin Zhang, Helene H Fung","doi":"10.1037/pag0000873","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000873","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prosociality tends to increase with age, but whether older adults (OA) are more willing than younger adults (YA) to extend their prosocial behaviors beyond close social circles remains a topic of debate. This study aimed to address this controversy and explore the underlying mechanisms of age-related differences in prosociality through the lens of social discounting and gain-and-loss framing. One hundred twenty-three younger adults and 135 older adults participated in a social discounting task (measuring prosocial tendencies toward various social relationships) with various framings (self-oriented framing, other-oriented framing, and control condition). Compared to younger adults, older adults exhibited higher overall prosociality and treated socially close and distant others more evenly, indicating lower levels of selectivity in prosociality. Notably, the interaction effect between age and framing revealed that other-oriented framing amplified the prosocial tendencies of older adults, particularly toward socially distant others but not younger adults. These findings suggest other-oriented framing specifically reduced older adults' selectivity in prosociality, highlighting their prioritization of others' welfare and aversion to others' losses. This supports the notion that older adults' prosociality may be driven by other-oriented motivation rather than self-interest. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2025-01-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142933138","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Trajectories of episodic memory in midlife: Historical change from a cross-country perspective. 中年记忆的轨迹:从跨国视角看历史变迁。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-16 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000870
Markus Wettstein, Frank J Infurna, Nutifafa E Y Dey, Yesenia Cruz-Carrillo, Kevin J Grimm, Margie E Lachman, Denis Gerstorf

According to the Flynn effect, performance on cognitive ability tests has improved over the past decades. However, we know very little about whether such historical improvements generalize to middle-aged adults (aged 45-65) and differ across nations. We used harmonized data on episodic memory from nationally representative longitudinal panel surveys across a total of 16 countries (United States, Mexico, China, England, and countries in Continental, Mediterranean, and Nordic Europe). We compared historical change in age-related trajectories of episodic memory among middle-aged adults. Our sample included 117,231 participants who provided 330,390 observations. Longitudinal multilevel regression models revealed that today's middle-aged adults in the United States perform worse on episodic memory tests than their peers in the past. By contrast, today's middle-aged adults in most other countries perform better on these tests than their peers in the past. However, later-born cohorts of U.S. and Chinese middle-aged adults experienced less steep within-person decrements-or even increments-in episodic memory than earlier born cohorts. Historical change trends persisted when controlling for sociodemographic factors, as well as for indicators of physical and mental health. Differences in episodic memory by gender and education became smaller over historical time across all nations. Our findings suggest that countries differ considerably in episodic memory performance, by more than half a standard deviation, and in the direction and size of how midlife episodic memory trajectories have changed over historical time. Further factors related to historical changes in midlife episodic memory need to be identified by future research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

根据弗林效应,在过去几十年里,人们在认知能力测试中的表现有所提高。然而,我们对这种历史上的改善是否适用于中年人(45-65岁)以及不同国家之间的差异知之甚少。我们使用了来自16个国家(美国、墨西哥、中国、英国以及欧洲大陆、地中海和北欧国家)具有全国代表性的纵向小组调查的情景记忆的统一数据。我们比较了中年人情景记忆的年龄相关轨迹的历史变化。我们的样本包括117,231名参与者,他们提供了330,390次观察。纵向多水平回归模型显示,今天的美国中年人在情景记忆测试中的表现比过去的同龄人要差。相比之下,今天大多数其他国家的中年人在这些测试中比过去的同龄人表现得更好。然而,晚出生的美国和中国中年人在情景记忆方面的下降幅度比早出生的人要小,甚至有所增加。在控制社会人口因素以及身心健康指标的情况下,历史变化趋势仍然存在。在所有国家中,性别和教育程度在情景记忆方面的差异在历史上都变小了。我们的研究结果表明,不同国家在情景记忆表现上的差异很大,超过了一半的标准差,而且在中年情景记忆轨迹的方向和大小上也随着历史时间的推移而变化。与中年情景记忆的历史变化相关的进一步因素需要通过未来的研究来确定。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Trajectories of episodic memory in midlife: Historical change from a cross-country perspective.","authors":"Markus Wettstein, Frank J Infurna, Nutifafa E Y Dey, Yesenia Cruz-Carrillo, Kevin J Grimm, Margie E Lachman, Denis Gerstorf","doi":"10.1037/pag0000870","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000870","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>According to the Flynn effect, performance on cognitive ability tests has improved over the past decades. However, we know very little about whether such historical improvements generalize to middle-aged adults (aged 45-65) and differ across nations. We used harmonized data on episodic memory from nationally representative longitudinal panel surveys across a total of 16 countries (United States, Mexico, China, England, and countries in Continental, Mediterranean, and Nordic Europe). We compared historical change in age-related trajectories of episodic memory among middle-aged adults. Our sample included 117,231 participants who provided 330,390 observations. Longitudinal multilevel regression models revealed that today's middle-aged adults in the United States perform worse on episodic memory tests than their peers in the past. By contrast, today's middle-aged adults in most other countries perform better on these tests than their peers in the past. However, later-born cohorts of U.S. and Chinese middle-aged adults experienced less steep within-person decrements-or even increments-in episodic memory than earlier born cohorts. Historical change trends persisted when controlling for sociodemographic factors, as well as for indicators of physical and mental health. Differences in episodic memory by gender and education became smaller over historical time across all nations. Our findings suggest that countries differ considerably in episodic memory performance, by more than half a standard deviation, and in the direction and size of how midlife episodic memory trajectories have changed over historical time. Further factors related to historical changes in midlife episodic memory need to be identified by future research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-12-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142830362","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychology and Aging
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1