首页 > 最新文献

Psychology and Aging最新文献

英文 中文
Are depressive symptoms associated with biological aging in a cross-sectional analysis of adults over age 50 in the United States. 对美国 50 岁以上成年人进行的横断面分析显示,抑郁症状是否与生理衰老有关。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-14 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000860
Herong Wang, Kelly M Bakulski, Freida Blostein, Brittany R Porath, John Dou, César Higgins Tejera, Lindsay H Ryan, Erin B Ware

Major depressive disorder accelerates DNA methylation age, a biological aging marker. Subclinical depressive symptoms are common, but their link to DNA methylation aging in older adults remains unexplored. This study analyzed the cross-sectional relationship between depressive symptoms and accelerated DNA methylation aging, considering gender and race/ethnicity in U.S. adults aged over 50. We used data from 3,882 diverse participants in the 2016 Health and Retirement Study wave, measuring blood DNA methylation age against chronologic age for acceleration. Depressive symptoms were assessed using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D) scale. Multiple linear regression evaluated the association between depressive symptoms and DNA methylation age acceleration, adjusting for sociodemographic factors, blood cell proportions, and health behaviors (physical activity, alcohol use, smoking, and chronic conditions). Gender and race/ethnicity modifications were also tested. Depressive symptoms, measured by continuous CES-D score, high depressive symptoms (CES-D ≥ 4), or any symptoms (CES-D ≥ 1), significantly correlated with increased GrimAge DNA methylation age acceleration (all p ≤ .001) in unadjusted and sociodemographic-adjusted models but were nonsignificant in fully adjusted models. No significant gender or race/ethnicity effect modifications were found in fully adjusted models. Health behaviors significantly influence DNA methylation age acceleration and depressive phenotypes, underscoring the need to understand their roles in assessing psychological factors related to DNA methylation age acceleration. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

重度抑郁症会加速生物衰老标志 DNA 甲基化的衰老。亚临床抑郁症状很常见,但它们与老年人 DNA 甲基化老化之间的联系仍未得到研究。本研究分析了美国 50 岁以上成年人中抑郁症状与 DNA 甲基化加速老化之间的横断面关系,同时考虑了性别和种族/民族因素。我们使用了 2016 年健康与退休研究(Health and Retirement Study)波中 3882 名不同参与者的数据,测量了血液 DNA 甲基化年龄与计时年龄的加速度。抑郁症状采用流行病学研究中心抑郁(CES-D)量表进行评估。多元线性回归评估了抑郁症状与 DNA 甲基化年龄加速之间的关联,并对社会人口因素、血细胞比例和健康行为(体育锻炼、饮酒、吸烟和慢性病)进行了调整。此外,还测试了性别和种族/人种修正情况。在未调整和社会人口因素调整模型中,以连续 CES-D 评分、高抑郁症状(CES-D ≥ 4)或任何症状(CES-D ≥ 1)衡量的抑郁症状与 GrimAge DNA 甲基化年龄加速度的增加显著相关(所有 p 均小于 .001),但在完全调整模型中不显著。在完全调整模型中,没有发现明显的性别或种族/人种效应修正。健康行为对DNA甲基化年龄加速和抑郁表型有重大影响,这说明在评估与DNA甲基化年龄加速有关的心理因素时,需要了解健康行为的作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Are depressive symptoms associated with biological aging in a cross-sectional analysis of adults over age 50 in the United States.","authors":"Herong Wang, Kelly M Bakulski, Freida Blostein, Brittany R Porath, John Dou, César Higgins Tejera, Lindsay H Ryan, Erin B Ware","doi":"10.1037/pag0000860","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000860","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Major depressive disorder accelerates DNA methylation age, a biological aging marker. Subclinical depressive symptoms are common, but their link to DNA methylation aging in older adults remains unexplored. This study analyzed the cross-sectional relationship between depressive symptoms and accelerated DNA methylation aging, considering gender and race/ethnicity in U.S. adults aged over 50. We used data from 3,882 diverse participants in the 2016 Health and Retirement Study wave, measuring blood DNA methylation age against chronologic age for acceleration. Depressive symptoms were assessed using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D) scale. Multiple linear regression evaluated the association between depressive symptoms and DNA methylation age acceleration, adjusting for sociodemographic factors, blood cell proportions, and health behaviors (physical activity, alcohol use, smoking, and chronic conditions). Gender and race/ethnicity modifications were also tested. Depressive symptoms, measured by continuous CES-D score, high depressive symptoms (CES-D ≥ 4), or any symptoms (CES-D ≥ 1), significantly correlated with increased GrimAge DNA methylation age acceleration (all <i>p</i> ≤ .001) in unadjusted and sociodemographic-adjusted models but were nonsignificant in fully adjusted models. No significant gender or race/ethnicity effect modifications were found in fully adjusted models. Health behaviors significantly influence DNA methylation age acceleration and depressive phenotypes, underscoring the need to understand their roles in assessing psychological factors related to DNA methylation age acceleration. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-10-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142478019","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Affective response to daily physical activity in younger and older adults. 年轻人和老年人对日常体育活动的情绪反应。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-07 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000852
Marta Stojanovic, Arthi Venkatesan, Tammy English, Denise Head

Many older adults do not meet the physical activity recommendations of the American Heart Association; hence, it is important to understand the factors that can facilitate regular physical activity in older adults. Notably, the role of affective response has been understudied. Mixed findings have been reported in terms of age effects in affective response to daily physical activity. This study aimed to determine age differences in affective response to daily physical activity and whether these differences are associated with overall physical activity levels. Further, the role of contextual factors in age differences in affective response following daily physical activity was examined. Younger (n = 59) and older adults (n = 60) completed 1 week of experience sampling during which they responded to daily prompts about their affect and physical activity. Overall physical activity levels were estimated via actigraphy. In both age groups, daily physical activity was similarly associated with greater high-arousal positive (HAP) affect relative to other activities. Across age groups, participants reported more HAP affect when engaging in daily physical activity in a group compared to alone. Greater duration and enjoyment of daily physical activity were more strongly associated with greater HAP affect in younger adults relative to older adults. Affective responses following bouts of daily physical activity did not predict overall physical activity levels for either age group. Overall, older adults may experience similar positive affective response to younger adults following daily physical activity relative to other activities. These affective responses may have a limited role in physical activity engagement in daily life. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

许多老年人达不到美国心脏协会推荐的体育锻炼量;因此,了解促进老年人定期进行体育锻炼的因素非常重要。值得注意的是,人们对情感反应的作用研究不足。关于年龄对日常体育锻炼的情感反应的影响,研究结果不一。本研究旨在确定老年人对日常体育锻炼的情感反应的年龄差异,以及这些差异是否与总体体育锻炼水平相关。此外,研究还考察了情境因素在日常体育锻炼后情感反应的年龄差异中的作用。年轻人(n = 59)和老年人(n = 60)分别完成了为期一周的经验取样,在此期间,他们每天都会对自己的情绪和体育活动作出反应。总体体力活动水平是通过运动记录仪估算出来的。在这两个年龄组中,与其他活动相比,日常体育活动同样与更大的高唤醒积极情绪(HAP)相关。在各个年龄组中,与单独参加日常体育活动相比,参加者在集体参加日常体育活动时会产生更多的高唤醒积极情感。与老年人相比,年轻人参加日常体育活动的时间更长、更喜欢参加日常体育活动与更大的高唤醒积极情感更密切相关。日常体育锻炼后的情绪反应并不能预测两个年龄组的总体体育锻炼水平。总体而言,相对于其他活动,老年人在日常体育活动后可能会出现与年轻人相似的积极情绪反应。这些情绪反应在日常生活中参与体育锻炼的作用可能有限。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Affective response to daily physical activity in younger and older adults.","authors":"Marta Stojanovic, Arthi Venkatesan, Tammy English, Denise Head","doi":"10.1037/pag0000852","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000852","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many older adults do not meet the physical activity recommendations of the American Heart Association; hence, it is important to understand the factors that can facilitate regular physical activity in older adults. Notably, the role of affective response has been understudied. Mixed findings have been reported in terms of age effects in affective response to daily physical activity. This study aimed to determine age differences in affective response to daily physical activity and whether these differences are associated with overall physical activity levels. Further, the role of contextual factors in age differences in affective response following daily physical activity was examined. Younger (<i>n</i> = 59) and older adults (<i>n</i> = 60) completed 1 week of experience sampling during which they responded to daily prompts about their affect and physical activity. Overall physical activity levels were estimated via actigraphy. In both age groups, daily physical activity was similarly associated with greater high-arousal positive (HAP) affect relative to other activities. Across age groups, participants reported more HAP affect when engaging in daily physical activity in a group compared to alone. Greater duration and enjoyment of daily physical activity were more strongly associated with greater HAP affect in younger adults relative to older adults. Affective responses following bouts of daily physical activity did not predict overall physical activity levels for either age group. Overall, older adults may experience similar positive affective response to younger adults following daily physical activity relative to other activities. These affective responses may have a limited role in physical activity engagement in daily life. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-10-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142394339","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reminders eliminate age-related declines in prospective memory. 提醒可消除与年龄有关的前瞻性记忆衰退。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000844
B Hunter Ball, Phil Peper, Matthew K Robison

Prospective memory (PM)-the process of establishing intentions for future action and remembering to fulfill these intentions at the appropriate time-is necessary for many instrumental activities of daily living and for maintaining functional independence with increased age. Offloading PM demands onto the environment, such as setting a reminder alarm to take medication, offers an easy and effective way to mitigate age-related PM declines. However, a lack of basic knowledge about the cognitive and metacognitive processes that drive offloading decisions presents barriers to successful implementation. The present study addresses these issues by examining age differences in PM for offloaded (i.e., with reminders) and nonoffloaded (i.e., without reminders) intentions under low and high memory demands. With highly specific intentions that can be retrieved via bottom-up processes (Experiment 1), there were no age differences in PM without reminders, and younger and older adults equally benefitted from reminders under high memory load. With nonspecific intentions that require top-down attention for retrieval (Experiment 2), older adults had worse PM under high load without reminders. Critically, this age difference was eliminated with the use of reminders, likely due to increased reminder checking for older adults under high load. These findings suggest that offloading can circumvent cognitive capacity limitations and minimize computational effort to improve intention fulfillment in older adults. The theoretical and applied ramifications of these findings are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

前瞻性记忆(PM)--为未来行动建立意图并在适当的时候记住实现这些意图的过程--是许多日常生活工具性活动的必要条件,也是随着年龄增长保持功能独立性的必要条件。将对 PM 的要求转移到环境中,例如设置提醒服药的闹钟,为缓解与年龄相关的 PM 衰退提供了一种简单有效的方法。然而,由于缺乏对驱动卸载决策的认知和元认知过程的基本了解,成功实施卸载决策面临障碍。本研究通过研究在低记忆需求和高记忆需求下,卸载(即有提醒)和非卸载(即无提醒)意向的PM的年龄差异来解决这些问题。对于可以通过自下而上的过程进行检索的高度特定的意图(实验 1),在没有提醒的情况下,PM 没有年龄差异,在高记忆负荷下,年轻人和老年人同样从提醒中获益。对于需要自上而下注意才能检索的非特定意图(实验 2),在没有提醒的情况下,老年人在高负荷下的 PM 更差。重要的是,这种年龄差异在使用提醒后被消除了,这可能是由于老年人在高负荷下对提醒的检查增加了。这些发现表明,"卸载 "可以规避认知能力的限制,最大限度地减少计算工作量,从而改善老年人的意图实现情况。本文讨论了这些发现的理论和应用影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Reminders eliminate age-related declines in prospective memory.","authors":"B Hunter Ball, Phil Peper, Matthew K Robison","doi":"10.1037/pag0000844","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000844","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prospective memory (PM)-the process of establishing intentions for future action and remembering to fulfill these intentions at the appropriate time-is necessary for many instrumental activities of daily living and for maintaining functional independence with increased age. Offloading PM demands onto the environment, such as setting a reminder alarm to take medication, offers an easy and effective way to mitigate age-related PM declines. However, a lack of basic knowledge about the cognitive and metacognitive processes that drive offloading decisions presents barriers to successful implementation. The present study addresses these issues by examining age differences in PM for offloaded (i.e., with reminders) and nonoffloaded (i.e., without reminders) intentions under low and high memory demands. With highly specific intentions that can be retrieved via bottom-up processes (Experiment 1), there were no age differences in PM without reminders, and younger and older adults equally benefitted from reminders under high memory load. With nonspecific intentions that require top-down attention for retrieval (Experiment 2), older adults had worse PM under high load without reminders. Critically, this age difference was eliminated with the use of reminders, likely due to increased reminder checking for older adults under high load. These findings suggest that offloading can circumvent cognitive capacity limitations and minimize computational effort to improve intention fulfillment in older adults. The theoretical and applied ramifications of these findings are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337182","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Retirement and life satisfaction among middle-aged and older adults: A piecewise growth mixture analysis. 中老年人的退休和生活满意度:片断增长混合分析。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-23 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000853
Jonathan L Chia, Andree Hartanto, William Tov

Retirement represents a significant life transition typically occurring in later adulthood, often accompanied by substantial lifestyle changes. Several theoretical frameworks suggest that these changes present both opportunities and challenges for well-being, and the extent to which individuals experience positive versus negative well-being outcomes may be influenced by various factors. To study such heterogeneity in retirement experiences, researchers have embraced person-centered methodologies. Yet, some previous studies have not robustly delineated retirement- from age-related changes in well-being, accounted for statistical uncertainties, or examined these diverse experiences outside of a Western context. These limitations preclude conclusions about the diverse experience of retirement. Using both person- and variable-centered approaches, this study examined life satisfaction trajectories before and after retirement among 532 retired middle-aged and older adults from the Singapore Life Panel. Controlling for age-related changes, latent growth mixture analysis was employed to identify retirement subgroups with varying life satisfaction trajectories. Three distinct trajectories were revealed-decreasingly satisfied, stable postretirement, and increasingly satisfied. As compared to those increasingly satisfied, decreasingly satisfied individuals tended to have lower social support, were higher on neuroticism, and had higher income. While expressed to a similar magnitude across profiles, education and religious activity also emerged as important predictors of well-being in retirement transition. Findings from the present study highlight the importance of recognizing heterogeneity in retirement experiences and opportunities for targeted interventions to support retirees' well-being. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

退休是人生的一次重大转变,通常发生在成年晚期,往往伴随着生活方式的重大改变。一些理论框架认为,这些变化对幸福感来说既是机遇也是挑战,而个人在幸福感方面的积极与消极程度可能会受到各种因素的影响。为了研究退休经历的这种异质性,研究人员采用了以人为本的方法。然而,之前的一些研究并没有将退休与年龄相关的幸福感变化有力地划分开来,也没有考虑统计上的不确定性,或在西方背景之外研究这些不同的体验。这些局限性使得我们无法对退休后的不同经历得出结论。本研究采用以人为中心和以变量为中心的方法,对新加坡生活小组中 532 名退休中老年人退休前后的生活满意度轨迹进行了研究。在控制了与年龄相关的变化后,采用了潜在增长混合分析法来识别具有不同生活满意度轨迹的退休亚群。结果显示了三种不同的轨迹--满意度下降、退休后稳定和满意度上升。与越来越满意的人相比,越来越不满意的人往往社会支持较少,神经质程度较高,收入较高。教育和宗教活动也是预测退休过渡期幸福感的重要因素,但在不同情况下的表现程度相似。本研究的结果凸显了认识到退休经历异质性的重要性,以及采取有针对性的干预措施以支持退休人员幸福感的机会。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Retirement and life satisfaction among middle-aged and older adults: A piecewise growth mixture analysis.","authors":"Jonathan L Chia, Andree Hartanto, William Tov","doi":"10.1037/pag0000853","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000853","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Retirement represents a significant life transition typically occurring in later adulthood, often accompanied by substantial lifestyle changes. Several theoretical frameworks suggest that these changes present both opportunities and challenges for well-being, and the extent to which individuals experience positive versus negative well-being outcomes may be influenced by various factors. To study such heterogeneity in retirement experiences, researchers have embraced person-centered methodologies. Yet, some previous studies have not robustly delineated retirement- from age-related changes in well-being, accounted for statistical uncertainties, or examined these diverse experiences outside of a Western context. These limitations preclude conclusions about the diverse experience of retirement. Using both person- and variable-centered approaches, this study examined life satisfaction trajectories before and after retirement among 532 retired middle-aged and older adults from the Singapore Life Panel. Controlling for age-related changes, latent growth mixture analysis was employed to identify retirement subgroups with varying life satisfaction trajectories. Three distinct trajectories were revealed-decreasingly satisfied, stable postretirement, and increasingly satisfied. As compared to those increasingly satisfied, decreasingly satisfied individuals tended to have lower social support, were higher on neuroticism, and had higher income. While expressed to a similar magnitude across profiles, education and religious activity also emerged as important predictors of well-being in retirement transition. Findings from the present study highlight the importance of recognizing heterogeneity in retirement experiences and opportunities for targeted interventions to support retirees' well-being. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298840","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Interplay of aging and practice in conflict processing: A big-data diffusion-model analysis. 冲突处理中老龄化与实践的相互作用:大数据扩散模型分析。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000848
Paul Kelber, Victor Mittelstädt, Rolf Ulrich

We are continually required to exercise cognitive control in order to separate relevant and irrelevant information. Previous studies have produced mixed results as to whether cognitive control declines across adulthood and improves with practice. Moreover, little is known about the influences of aging and practice on the automatic and controlled processes underlying performance in conflict situations. This calls for analyses of extensive data using process models for conflict tasks, akin to earlier drift-diffusion model analyses of performance in cognitive nonconflict tasks. Thus, to understand how aging and practice influence cognitive control at the process level, we analyzed a large-scale data set (1,800 participants aged 21-80 years completing 60 blocklike online games of an arrow-based Eriksen flanker task). At the coarse-grained level of mean response times, the congruency effect increased across adulthood and decreased with practice following an initial increase. The finer-grained distributional response time and error rate data were closely fitted by the diffusion model for conflict tasks, which captures the dynamic interplay of automatic and controlled processing. Best-fitting parameter values revealed multiple, partially counteracting influences of aging and practice: Aging across adulthood slowed down both controlled and automatic processing (besides slowing down nondecisional processes and increasing decision caution). By contrast, practice selectively speeded up controlled processing (besides speeding up nondecisional processes and decreasing decision caution). Taken together, these findings suggest that aging and practice primarily alter the speed of controlled (aging and practice) and automatic processing (aging), rather than causing inhibitory adjustments in the strength of automatic processing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们需要不断进行认知控制,以区分相关信息和无关信息。关于认知控制能力是否会随着成年而下降,以及是否会随着练习而提高,以往的研究结果不一。此外,人们对衰老和练习对冲突情况下自动和受控过程的影响知之甚少。这就需要使用冲突任务的过程模型对大量数据进行分析,类似于早先对认知非冲突任务的表现进行的漂移-扩散模型分析。因此,为了了解衰老和练习如何在过程水平上影响认知控制,我们分析了一个大规模的数据集(1800 名年龄在 21-80 岁之间的参与者完成了 60 个基于箭头的埃里克森侧翼任务的块状在线游戏)。在平均反应时间的粗粒度水平上,一致性效应在整个成年期都在增加,并在最初增加后随着练习而减少。冲突任务的扩散模型能够紧密拟合反应时间和错误率的细粒度分布数据,该模型捕捉了自动处理和受控处理的动态相互作用。最佳拟合参数值揭示了衰老和练习的多重、部分抵消影响:成年期的衰老会减慢受控处理和自动处理的速度(此外还会减慢非决策过程并增加决策的谨慎性)。与此相反,练习则有选择性地加速了控制处理过程(除了加速非决策过程和降低决策谨慎性之外)。综上所述,这些研究结果表明,衰老和练习主要改变了控制处理(衰老和练习)和自动处理(衰老)的速度,而不是导致自动处理强度的抑制性调整。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Interplay of aging and practice in conflict processing: A big-data diffusion-model analysis.","authors":"Paul Kelber, Victor Mittelstädt, Rolf Ulrich","doi":"10.1037/pag0000848","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000848","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We are continually required to exercise cognitive control in order to separate relevant and irrelevant information. Previous studies have produced mixed results as to whether cognitive control declines across adulthood and improves with practice. Moreover, little is known about the influences of aging and practice on the automatic and controlled processes underlying performance in conflict situations. This calls for analyses of extensive data using process models for conflict tasks, akin to earlier drift-diffusion model analyses of performance in cognitive nonconflict tasks. Thus, to understand how aging and practice influence cognitive control at the process level, we analyzed a large-scale data set (1,800 participants aged 21-80 years completing 60 blocklike online games of an arrow-based Eriksen flanker task). At the coarse-grained level of mean response times, the congruency effect increased across adulthood and decreased with practice following an initial increase. The finer-grained distributional response time and error rate data were closely fitted by the diffusion model for conflict tasks, which captures the dynamic interplay of automatic and controlled processing. Best-fitting parameter values revealed multiple, partially counteracting influences of aging and practice: Aging across adulthood slowed down both controlled and automatic processing (besides slowing down nondecisional processes and increasing decision caution). By contrast, practice selectively speeded up controlled processing (besides speeding up nondecisional processes and decreasing decision caution). Taken together, these findings suggest that aging and practice primarily alter the speed of controlled (aging and practice) and automatic processing (aging), rather than causing inhibitory adjustments in the strength of automatic processing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298838","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Delayed onset of cognitive terminal decline in later born cohorts: Evidence from a longitudinal study of two cohorts born 29-years apart. 出生较晚的人群认知能力终末衰退的起始时间较晚:对相隔 29 年出生的两个群体进行纵向研究的证据。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-05 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000846
Valgeir Thorvaldsson, Ingmar Skoog, Boo Johansson

In this study, we evaluated birth cohort (i.e., generational) differences in the onset and rate of acceleration in cognitive decline prior to death (i.e., terminal decline [TD]). We obtained data from two cohorts, born in 1901-1902 (n = 755, 64% females) and 1930 (n = 347, 48% females), identified and sampled at age 70 from the same city population and assessed on the same cognitive tests at ages 70, 75, 79, 85, and 88. The 1901-1902 cohort was additionally assessed at ages 90, 92, 95, 97, 99, and 100. The outcome was defined at each measurement occasion by a composite score of three cognitive tests assessing spatial ability, perceptual and motor speed, and reasoning. Date of death was obtained from population register, with the last update in April 2023, covering over 99% and 38% of the cohort members, respectively. We fitted a random TD change point model to the data while accounting for sex and education. Findings revealed expected TD onset (as conditioned on male with formal education) 5.17 (95% highest density interval [2.54, 8.05]) years prior to death in the 1901-1902 cohort, with an acceleration in rate of decline by a factor of 4.43 within the TD phase. This estimate was delayed by 2.53 (95% highest density interval [5.68, 0.10]) years in the 1930 cohort, with an acceleration by a factor of 5.16 within the TD phase. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to present evidence indicating that today's birth cohort experience, on average, a shorter TD phase in comparison to earlier born cohorts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在这项研究中,我们评估了出生队列(即世代)在死亡前认知能力衰退(即终末衰退 [TD])的开始和加速率方面的差异。我们从 1901-1902 年(n = 755,64% 为女性)和 1930 年(n = 347,48% 为女性)出生的两个队列中获得了数据,这两个队列在 70 岁时从同一个城市人口中识别和抽样,并在 70、75、79、85 和 88 岁时接受相同的认知测试评估。此外,还在 90、92、95、97、99 和 100 岁时对 1901-1902 年组群进行了评估。每次测量的结果均由三项认知测试的综合得分确定,这三项测试分别评估空间能力、感知和运动速度以及推理能力。死亡日期从人口登记册中获得,最近一次更新是在2023年4月,分别覆盖了队列中超过99%和38%的成员。我们对数据进行了随机 TD 变点模型拟合,同时考虑了性别和教育程度。结果显示,在 1901-1902 年队列中,预计 TD 发病时间(以受过正规教育的男性为条件)比死亡时间早 5.17 年(95% 最高密度区间 [2.54, 8.05]),在 TD 阶段,下降速度加快了 4.43 倍。在 1930 年队列中,这一估计值延迟了 2.53 年(95% 最高密度区间 [5.68, 0.10]),在 TD 阶段加速了 5.16 倍。据我们所知,这是首次有证据表明,与较早出生的人群相比,今天出生的人群平均经历的 TD 阶段较短的研究。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Delayed onset of cognitive terminal decline in later born cohorts: Evidence from a longitudinal study of two cohorts born 29-years apart.","authors":"Valgeir Thorvaldsson, Ingmar Skoog, Boo Johansson","doi":"10.1037/pag0000846","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000846","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this study, we evaluated birth cohort (i.e., generational) differences in the onset and rate of acceleration in cognitive decline prior to death (i.e., terminal decline [TD]). We obtained data from two cohorts, born in 1901-1902 (<i>n</i> = 755, 64% females) and 1930 (<i>n</i> = 347, 48% females), identified and sampled at age 70 from the same city population and assessed on the same cognitive tests at ages 70, 75, 79, 85, and 88. The 1901-1902 cohort was additionally assessed at ages 90, 92, 95, 97, 99, and 100. The outcome was defined at each measurement occasion by a composite score of three cognitive tests assessing spatial ability, perceptual and motor speed, and reasoning. Date of death was obtained from population register, with the last update in April 2023, covering over 99% and 38% of the cohort members, respectively. We fitted a random TD change point model to the data while accounting for sex and education. Findings revealed expected TD onset (as conditioned on male with formal education) 5.17 (95% highest density interval [2.54, 8.05]) years prior to death in the 1901-1902 cohort, with an acceleration in rate of decline by a factor of 4.43 within the TD phase. This estimate was delayed by 2.53 (95% highest density interval [5.68, 0.10]) years in the 1930 cohort, with an acceleration by a factor of 5.16 within the TD phase. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to present evidence indicating that today's birth cohort experience, on average, a shorter TD phase in comparison to earlier born cohorts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141375","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Which predicts longevity better: Satisfaction with life or purpose in life? 哪个更能预测长寿?生活满意度还是人生目标?
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-15 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000802
Frank Martela, Elmeri Laitinen, Christian Hakulinen

Life satisfaction and purpose in life are fundamental yet separate ways to evaluate one's life. Both positively predict physical health and longevity, making them key factors for length and quality of life. However, we do not know which of them predicts mortality, when controlling for the influence of each other. Given that purpose in life involves a more active engagement with life and can help to cope with suffering, we hypothesize that purpose in life could be a more direct prospective predictor of longevity, overshadowing any effect of life satisfaction, when the two are pitted against each other as prospective predictors of longevity. To examine these hypotheses, we utilized Midlife in the U.S. survey, which is a 23-year follow-up study, (N = 5,993) and Cox proportional hazards models, repeating the analyses both without covariates and when controlling for various demographic and health-related variables. We show that both life satisfaction and purpose in life predict mortality when modeled separately. When life satisfaction, purpose in life and self-rated health were entered as simultaneous predictors of mortality, purpose in life remained a slightly more robust predictor of mortality, while life satisfaction became only marginally significant, suggesting that some of the factors that connect it to mortality are covered by the other two subjective evaluations. Overall, the results demonstrate that purpose in life is a robust predictor of mortality, and thus a key dimension of well-being to attend to as people age, while the predictive power of life satisfaction is more dependent on the choice of covariates. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

生活满意度和人生目标是评价一个人生活的基本但又不同的方式。两者都能积极预测身体健康和长寿,是决定生命长度和质量的关键因素。然而,我们并不知道在控制两者相互影响的情况下,哪一个能预测死亡率。鉴于生活目的涉及更积极地参与生活,并有助于应对痛苦,我们假设,当生活目的和生活满意度作为长寿的前瞻性预测因素相互对立时,生活目的可能是更直接的长寿前瞻性预测因素,其作用会盖过生活满意度。为了检验这些假设,我们利用了美国中年调查(一项为期 23 年的跟踪研究)(N = 5993)和 Cox 比例危险模型,在不考虑协变量以及控制各种人口和健康相关变量的情况下重复进行了分析。我们的研究表明,如果将生活满意度和生活目的分别建模,它们都能预测死亡率。当生活满意度、生活目的和自评健康同时作为死亡率的预测因子时,生活目的仍然是死亡率的一个略微稳健的预测因子,而生活满意度仅有轻微的显著性,这表明生活满意度与死亡率相关的一些因素已被其他两个主观评价所涵盖。总之,研究结果表明,生活目标是预测死亡率的有力指标,因此也是随着人们年龄的增长而需要关注的幸福感的一个关键维度,而生活满意度的预测能力则更多地取决于协变量的选择。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Which predicts longevity better: Satisfaction with life or purpose in life?","authors":"Frank Martela, Elmeri Laitinen, Christian Hakulinen","doi":"10.1037/pag0000802","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000802","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Life satisfaction and purpose in life are fundamental yet separate ways to evaluate one's life. Both positively predict physical health and longevity, making them key factors for length and quality of life. However, we do not know which of them predicts mortality, when controlling for the influence of each other. Given that purpose in life involves a more active engagement with life and can help to cope with suffering, we hypothesize that purpose in life could be a more direct prospective predictor of longevity, overshadowing any effect of life satisfaction, when the two are pitted against each other as prospective predictors of longevity. To examine these hypotheses, we utilized Midlife in the U.S. survey, which is a 23-year follow-up study, (<i>N</i> = 5,993) and Cox proportional hazards models, repeating the analyses both without covariates and when controlling for various demographic and health-related variables. We show that both life satisfaction and purpose in life predict mortality when modeled separately. When life satisfaction, purpose in life and self-rated health were entered as simultaneous predictors of mortality, purpose in life remained a slightly more robust predictor of mortality, while life satisfaction became only marginally significant, suggesting that some of the factors that connect it to mortality are covered by the other two subjective evaluations. Overall, the results demonstrate that purpose in life is a robust predictor of mortality, and thus a key dimension of well-being to attend to as people age, while the predictive power of life satisfaction is more dependent on the choice of covariates. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139736406","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are social interactions perceived as more meaningful in older adulthood? 成年后的社会交往是否更有意义?
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-23 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000827
Jana Nikitin, Martina Luchetti, Nejla Alimanovic, Fiona S Rupprecht, Angelina R Sutin

Social relationships accompany us throughout adulthood and are among the most important sources of meaning in a person's life. However, little is known about age differences in meaningfulness of social interactions across adulthood. According to socioemotional selectivity theory, as people age, they develop relatively stronger preferences for social relationships that are emotionally meaningful. Consequently, older adults may perceive social interactions in everyday life as more meaningful than younger adults. To test this hypothesis, the present study examined age-related differences in the perceived meaningfulness of daily social interactions using experience sampling data. Three-hundred six participants (56.9% women, 18-88 years) completed a total of 6,407 entries over 3 days. Results of the multilevel analyses showed that age was positively associated with perceived meaningfulness of daily social interactions, controlling for relationship closeness and situation valence. In addition, the perceived meaningfulness of daily social interactions was positively related to subjective well-being both between and within all participants, indicating that meaningful social interactions are beneficial for well-being regardless of age. Thus, perceiving social interactions as meaningful could be one way that older people maintain a high level of well-being in their daily lives. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

社会关系伴随着我们整个成年期,是人一生中最重要的意义来源之一。然而,人们对整个成年期社会交往意义的年龄差异知之甚少。根据社会情感选择理论,随着年龄的增长,人们会对有情感意义的社会关系产生相对较强的偏好。因此,老年人可能会认为日常生活中的社会交往比年轻人更有意义。为了验证这一假设,本研究利用经验抽样数据,考察了与年龄相关的日常社会交往意义感知差异。三百六名参与者(56.9% 为女性,18-88 岁)在三天内共完成了 6,407 个条目。多层次分析的结果表明,年龄与日常社交互动的意义感呈正相关,与关系亲密程度和情境价值相关。此外,在所有参与者之间和参与者内部,日常社交互动的意义感都与主观幸福感呈正相关,这表明无论年龄大小,有意义的社交互动都对幸福感有益。因此,认为社交互动有意义可能是老年人在日常生活中保持高水平幸福感的一种方式。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Are social interactions perceived as more meaningful in older adulthood?","authors":"Jana Nikitin, Martina Luchetti, Nejla Alimanovic, Fiona S Rupprecht, Angelina R Sutin","doi":"10.1037/pag0000827","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000827","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Social relationships accompany us throughout adulthood and are among the most important sources of meaning in a person's life. However, little is known about age differences in meaningfulness of social interactions across adulthood. According to socioemotional selectivity theory, as people age, they develop relatively stronger preferences for social relationships that are emotionally meaningful. Consequently, older adults may perceive social interactions in everyday life as more meaningful than younger adults. To test this hypothesis, the present study examined age-related differences in the perceived meaningfulness of daily social interactions using experience sampling data. Three-hundred six participants (56.9% women, 18-88 years) completed a total of 6,407 entries over 3 days. Results of the multilevel analyses showed that age was positively associated with perceived meaningfulness of daily social interactions, controlling for relationship closeness and situation valence. In addition, the perceived meaningfulness of daily social interactions was positively related to subjective well-being both between and within all participants, indicating that meaningful social interactions are beneficial for well-being regardless of age. Thus, perceiving social interactions as meaningful could be one way that older people maintain a high level of well-being in their daily lives. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141082803","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do early midlife work characteristics predict 20-year change in control beliefs? 中年早期的工作特征能否预测 20 年后控制信念的变化?
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-11 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000830
Johanna Hartung, Lena Stahlhofen, Oliver K Schilling, Hans-Werner Wahl, Gizem Hülür

Previous research showed the importance of control beliefs for many life outcomes. The present study examines associations between subjectively perceived work environment and objectively measured work activities at the beginning of midlife as a central developmental phase in the context of work, with control beliefs across the subsequent 20 years. We analyzed four-wave longitudinal data from N = 374 participants (born 1950-1952; Mage baseline = 44 years, SD = 1; 44% women) from the Interdisciplinary Longitudinal Study of Adult Development and Aging within a structural equation modeling framework. Over 20 years and overall, internal control beliefs were stable, while external control beliefs decreased. Individuals who reported higher task variety and better social integration at work at baseline also reported higher levels of control beliefs for positive life outcomes. In addition, higher social integration at work at baseline was related to lower levels of external control beliefs. Work characteristics at baseline were not associated with individual differences in change in control beliefs across the 20-year observational interval. In summary, our findings suggest that work experiences at the prime of job-related development around the midst of the fifth decade of life do not play a major role in subsequent control beliefs development across 20 years. However, investigations measuring control beliefs as well as work characteristics continuously over a long period of time are necessary to shed light on reciprocal influences between work and personality. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

以往的研究表明,控制信念对许多人生结果都很重要。本研究探讨了中年之初的主观感知工作环境和客观测量的工作活动与随后 20 年的控制信念之间的关联,中年是工作方面的一个核心发展阶段。我们在结构方程建模框架内分析了 "成人发展与老龄化跨学科纵向研究"(Interdisciplinary Longitudinal Study of Adult Development and Aging)中 N = 374 名参与者(1950-1952 年出生;年龄基线 = 44 岁,SD = 1;44% 为女性)的四波纵向数据。在 20 年的总体时间里,内部控制信念保持稳定,而外部控制信念则有所下降。那些在基线时报告工作任务多样性更高、社会融合度更好的人,也报告他们对积极生活结果的控制信念水平更高。此外,基线时工作中社会融合度较高的人与外部控制信念水平较低有关。基线时的工作特征与 20 年观察期间控制信念变化的个体差异无关。总之,我们的研究结果表明,在人生的第五个十年中期左右,在与工作相关的发展的黄金时期的工作经历并不会对随后跨越 20 年的控制信念的发展起主要作用。然而,有必要对控制信念以及工作特征进行长期持续的测量,以揭示工作与人格之间的相互影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Do early midlife work characteristics predict 20-year change in control beliefs?","authors":"Johanna Hartung, Lena Stahlhofen, Oliver K Schilling, Hans-Werner Wahl, Gizem Hülür","doi":"10.1037/pag0000830","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000830","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research showed the importance of control beliefs for many life outcomes. The present study examines associations between subjectively perceived work environment and objectively measured work activities at the beginning of midlife as a central developmental phase in the context of work, with control beliefs across the subsequent 20 years. We analyzed four-wave longitudinal data from <i>N</i> = 374 participants (born 1950-1952; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> baseline = 44 years, <i>SD</i> = 1; 44% women) from the Interdisciplinary Longitudinal Study of Adult Development and Aging within a structural equation modeling framework. Over 20 years and overall, internal control beliefs were stable, while external control beliefs decreased. Individuals who reported higher task variety and better social integration at work at baseline also reported higher levels of control beliefs for positive life outcomes. In addition, higher social integration at work at baseline was related to lower levels of external control beliefs. Work characteristics at baseline were not associated with individual differences in change in control beliefs across the 20-year observational interval. In summary, our findings suggest that work experiences at the prime of job-related development around the midst of the fifth decade of life do not play a major role in subsequent control beliefs development across 20 years. However, investigations measuring control beliefs as well as work characteristics continuously over a long period of time are necessary to shed light on reciprocal influences between work and personality. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141591778","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to "Do caregiver interventions improve outcomes in relatives with dementia and mild cognitive impairment? A comprehensive systematic review and meta-analysis" by Cheng et al. (2022). Correction to "Do caregiver interventions improve outcomes in relatives with dementia and mild cognitive impairment?Cheng等人(2022年)所作的 "全面系统回顾和荟萃分析"。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-25 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000840

Reports an error in "Do caregiver interventions improve outcomes in relatives with dementia and mild cognitive impairment? A comprehensive systematic review and meta-analysis" by Sheung-Tak Cheng, Kin-Kit Li, Peggy P. L. Or and Andrés Losada (Psychology and Aging, 2022[Dec], Vol 37[8], 929-953). In the original article, two of the labels in the first column of Table 6 were incorrect. The first Recommended intervention should have been "Education with psychotherapeutic components (psychoeducation-b)," and the first Moderately recommended intervention should have been "Education with probable psychological components (psychoeducation-a)." Despite the misnomers, the nature of the intervention type can be clearly discerned from the description in the far-right column. The online version of this article has been corrected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2022-76749-001). Some reviews suggest benefits of nonpharmacological interventions for informal caregivers of people with dementia and mild cognitive impairment. These benefits may transfer to the care-recipients (CRs) through increased caregiving capability, reduced burden and depression among caregivers, and decreased negative mood contagion. However, large-scale review on these effects on the CRs is lacking. We searched PsycINFO, CINAHL with Full Text, MEDLINE, and PubMed from inception to end of 2020 and found 142 articles that reported randomized controlled trials (RCTs) of caregiver interventions using CR outcomes. Interventions were found to reduce neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS) in general and behavioral and mood disturbance specifically, enhance cognition and quality of life, and delay institutionalization and mortality, with care coordination/case management, educational intervention with psychotherapeutic components (psychoeducation-b), and direct training of the CR (with caregiver involvement) being the more potent interventions. The kinds of benefit depend on the types of intervention. NPS was reduced by psychoeducation-b, care coordination/case management, and CR training. Cognition and quality of life were enhanced by CR training and care coordination/case management, respectively. Institutionalization was delayed by multicomponent interventions and respite (based on one study). However, the effects were generally small to very small. Together with existing findings on caregiver outcomes, a tripartite scaffolding model of caregiver support is proposed. The model is composed of three components: (a) care coordination/case management (i.e., enhanced usual care), (b) psychoeducation-b, and (c) CR training. Future directions in terms of developing consensual guidelines, a registry of intervention manuals, and family-centered programs with flexibility in delivery are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

报告了 Sheung-Tak Cheng、Kin-Kit Li、Peggy P.L. Or 和 Andrés Losada 的 "护理干预是否能改善痴呆症和轻度认知障碍患者亲属的预后?A comprehensive systematic review and meta-analysis"(《心理学与老龄化》,2022 年 12 月,第 37 卷[8],929-953 页)中的错误。在原文中,表 6 第一栏中有两个标签不正确。第一个推荐干预应该是 "具有心理治疗成分的教育(心理教育-b)",第一个中度推荐干预应该是 "可能具有心理成分的教育(心理教育-a)"。尽管用词不当,但从最右边一栏的描述中可以清楚地看出干预类型的性质。本文网络版已作更正。(原文摘要如下,载于 2022-76749-001 号记录)。一些综述表明,非药物干预对痴呆症和轻度认知障碍患者的非正式照顾者有益。这些益处可能会通过提高护理能力、减轻护理者的负担和抑郁以及减少负面情绪传染等方式传递给护理对象(CRs)。然而,目前还缺乏有关这些对护理对象影响的大规模研究。我们检索了 PsycINFO、CINAHL(含全文)、MEDLINE 和 PubMed(从开始到 2020 年底),发现有 142 篇文章报道了使用 CR 结果对照顾者进行干预的随机对照试验 (RCT)。研究发现,干预措施可以减少一般的神经精神症状(NPS),特别是行为和情绪障碍,提高认知能力和生活质量,延迟入院时间和死亡率,其中护理协调/病例管理、包含心理治疗成分的教育干预(心理教育-b)和直接培训 CR(有护理者参与)是更有效的干预措施。获益的种类取决于干预的类型。心理教育-b、护理协调/个案管理和 CR 培训降低了 NPS。CR 培训和护理协调/个案管理分别提高了认知能力和生活质量。多组分干预和暂休(基于一项研究)推迟了入院时间。然而,效果一般较小或非常小。结合现有的关于照护者成果的研究结果,提出了照护者支持的三方支架模式。该模式由三个部分组成:(a) 护理协调/个案管理(即增强型常规护理),(b) 心理教育-b,以及 (c) CR 培训。本文讨论了未来的发展方向,即制定共识指南、登记干预手册以及以家庭为中心、灵活实施的项目。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Correction to \"Do caregiver interventions improve outcomes in relatives with dementia and mild cognitive impairment? A comprehensive systematic review and meta-analysis\" by Cheng et al. (2022).","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/pag0000840","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000840","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Reports an error in \"Do caregiver interventions improve outcomes in relatives with dementia and mild cognitive impairment? A comprehensive systematic review and meta-analysis\" by Sheung-Tak Cheng, Kin-Kit Li, Peggy P. L. Or and Andrés Losada (<i>Psychology and Aging</i>, 2022[Dec], Vol 37[8], 929-953). In the original article, two of the labels in the first column of Table 6 were incorrect. The first Recommended intervention should have been \"Education with psychotherapeutic components (psychoeducation-b),\" and the first Moderately recommended intervention should have been \"Education with probable psychological components (psychoeducation-a).\" Despite the misnomers, the nature of the intervention type can be clearly discerned from the description in the far-right column. The online version of this article has been corrected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2022-76749-001). Some reviews suggest benefits of nonpharmacological interventions for informal caregivers of people with dementia and mild cognitive impairment. These benefits may transfer to the care-recipients (CRs) through increased caregiving capability, reduced burden and depression among caregivers, and decreased negative mood contagion. However, large-scale review on these effects on the CRs is lacking. We searched PsycINFO, CINAHL with Full Text, MEDLINE, and PubMed from inception to end of 2020 and found 142 articles that reported randomized controlled trials (RCTs) of caregiver interventions using CR outcomes. Interventions were found to reduce neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS) in general and behavioral and mood disturbance specifically, enhance cognition and quality of life, and delay institutionalization and mortality, with care coordination/case management, educational intervention with psychotherapeutic components (psychoeducation-b), and direct training of the CR (with caregiver involvement) being the more potent interventions. The kinds of benefit depend on the types of intervention. NPS was reduced by psychoeducation-b, care coordination/case management, and CR training. Cognition and quality of life were enhanced by CR training and care coordination/case management, respectively. Institutionalization was delayed by multicomponent interventions and respite (based on one study). However, the effects were generally small to very small. Together with existing findings on caregiver outcomes, a tripartite scaffolding model of caregiver support is proposed. The model is composed of three components: (a) care coordination/case management (i.e., enhanced usual care), (b) psychoeducation-b, and (c) CR training. Future directions in terms of developing consensual guidelines, a registry of intervention manuals, and family-centered programs with flexibility in delivery are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141761790","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychology and Aging
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1