首页 > 最新文献

American Psychologist最新文献

英文 中文
Beliefs that influence personality likely concern a situation humans never leave. 影响个性的信念很可能与人类从未离开过的环境有关。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-07 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001436
Jeremy D W Clifton, Alia J Crum

Many of us-60% of humanity, according to one study-would like to change some of our personality traits, such as decreasing pessimism or neuroticism. Dweck (2008) proposed that traits might be altered by changing beliefs. However, novel beliefs must be identified, she contends, because currently studied beliefs are empirically inadequate (e.g., low correlations to broad personality traits) and because a belief's influence on behavior is usually confined to a particular situation or topic. When psychologists refer to the psychological impact of beliefs about situations, they typically mean local situations: situations individuals can enter and leave (e.g., "This neighborhood is dangerous"). The novel theoretical suggestion of this article is that a person's basic beliefs about a situation they never leave such as the world (e.g., "This world is dangerous") are uniquely suited to impact cross-situational behavior patterns often associated with broad personality traits. Historically, general beliefs about the world (termed "primal world beliefs") were understudied, and many remained unknown, rendering systematic investigation infeasible. However, using several methods that helped identify Big Five traits decades ago, a recent effort seeking to map primal world beliefs found a structure of 26 dimensions (most clustering into the beliefs that the world is Safe, Enticing, and Alive) suggesting promising avenues for primals-personality research. This article presents a nuanced, working, speculative hypothesis future research can explore: Average behavioral tendencies that persist wherever the individual goes (personality traits) theoretically should result from beliefs about the broader situation the individual never leaves (the world). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

根据一项研究,我们中的许多人--占人类的 60%--都希望改变自己的某些人格特质,比如减少悲观主义或神经质。德韦克(2008 年)提出,可以通过改变信念来改变性格特征。然而,她认为必须确定新的信念,因为目前研究的信念在经验上并不充分(例如,与广泛的人格特质的相关性较低),而且信念对行为的影响通常仅限于特定的情况或主题。当心理学家提到信念对情境的心理影响时,他们通常指的是局部情境:个人可以进入和离开的情境(例如,"这个街区很危险")。本文提出的新颖理论建议是,一个人对其从未离开过的情境(如世界)的基本信念(如 "这个世界很危险"),是影响跨情境行为模式的独特因素,而跨情境行为模式通常与广泛的人格特质有关。一直以来,人们对世界的一般信念(称为 "原始世界信念")研究不足,许多信念仍不为人所知,因此不可能进行系统的研究。然而,利用几十年前帮助确定大五特质的几种方法,最近一项试图绘制原始世界信念图的工作发现了一个由 26 个维度组成的结构(大部分聚集在 "世界是安全的"、"诱人的 "和 "活生生的 "等信念中),这为原始世界信念-人格研究提供了很有希望的途径。本文提出了一个细微的、可行的、推测性的假设,供未来研究探索:从理论上讲,个人无论走到哪里都会持续存在的平均行为倾向(人格特质)应该来自于对个人从未离开过的更广阔环境(世界)的信念。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Beliefs that influence personality likely concern a situation humans never leave.","authors":"Jeremy D W Clifton, Alia J Crum","doi":"10.1037/amp0001436","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001436","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many of us-60% of humanity, according to one study-would like to change some of our personality traits, such as decreasing pessimism or neuroticism. Dweck (2008) proposed that traits might be altered by changing beliefs. However, novel beliefs must be identified, she contends, because currently studied beliefs are empirically inadequate (e.g., low correlations to broad personality traits) and because a belief's influence on behavior is usually confined to a particular situation or topic. When psychologists refer to the psychological impact of beliefs about situations, they typically mean local situations: situations individuals can enter and leave (e.g., \"This neighborhood is dangerous\"). The novel theoretical suggestion of this article is that a person's basic beliefs about a situation <i>they never leave</i> such as the world (e.g., \"This world is dangerous\") are uniquely suited to impact cross-situational behavior patterns often associated with broad personality traits. Historically, general beliefs about the world (termed \"primal world beliefs\") were understudied, and many remained unknown, rendering systematic investigation infeasible. However, using several methods that helped identify Big Five traits decades ago, a recent effort seeking to map primal world beliefs found a structure of 26 dimensions (most clustering into the beliefs that the world is <i>Safe, Enticing,</i> and <i>Alive</i>) suggesting promising avenues for primals-personality research. This article presents a nuanced, working, speculative hypothesis future research can explore: Average behavioral tendencies that persist wherever the individual goes (personality traits) theoretically should result from beliefs about the broader situation the individual never leaves (the world). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142606551","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Frans B. M. de Waal (1948-2024). 弗兰斯-德瓦尔(Frans B. M. de Waal,1948-2024 年)。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-07 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001456
Stephanie D Preston

Article memorializes Frans B. M. de Waal (1948-2024). Franciscus (Frans) Bernardus Maria de Waal was a Dutch-American primatologist and ethologist who was born on October 29, 1948, in Hertogenbosch, in the southern Netherlands. Frans was taken by stomach cancer in March 2024, at the age of 75. Frans's long and storied career and life touched the lives of so many, all around the world. Through 13 books (in 20 languages), courses, lectures, field visits (to nearly every continent), collaborations, and many abiding friendships, Frans shared his love of animals and the natural world with us all. May we honor his legacy by caring for those around us, large and small. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

文章纪念弗兰斯-B.M.-德瓦尔(1948-2024)。弗朗西斯库斯(弗兰斯)-伯纳德斯-玛丽亚-德瓦尔是美籍荷兰灵长类动物学家和人种学家,1948 年 10 月 29 日出生于荷兰南部的赫托亨博斯。2024 年 3 月,弗朗斯因胃癌去世,享年 75 岁。弗兰斯漫长而传奇的职业生涯和人生影响了世界各地许多人的生活。通过 13 本著作(20 种语言)、课程、讲座、实地考察(几乎遍及各大洲)、合作以及许多深厚的友谊,弗兰斯与我们所有人分享了他对动物和自然世界的热爱。愿我们关爱身边大大小小的人,以此纪念他的遗产。 (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Frans B. M. de Waal (1948-2024).","authors":"Stephanie D Preston","doi":"10.1037/amp0001456","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001456","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Article memorializes Frans B. M. de Waal (1948-2024). Franciscus (Frans) Bernardus Maria de Waal was a Dutch-American primatologist and ethologist who was born on October 29, 1948, in Hertogenbosch, in the southern Netherlands. Frans was taken by stomach cancer in March 2024, at the age of 75. Frans's long and storied career and life touched the lives of so many, all around the world. Through 13 books (in 20 languages), courses, lectures, field visits (to nearly every continent), collaborations, and many abiding friendships, Frans shared his love of animals and the natural world with us all. May we honor his legacy by caring for those around us, large and small. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142606555","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
There is nothing WEIRD about basic research: The critical role of convenience samples in psychological science. 基础研究并不奇怪:便利样本在心理科学中的关键作用。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-07 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001443
Jeffrey W Sherman

Attention to issues of sample diversity and generalizability has increased dramatically in the past 15 years, as psychological scientists have confronted the limitations of relatively homogeneous samples. Though this reckoning was perhaps overdue and has undoubtedly shined a light on some poor research practices, recommendations surrounding sample diversity are sometimes applied to research that does not aim for generalizability across peoples. In this article, I seek to promote discussion about when and why sample diversity and generalizability matter. In doing so, I address problems with language surrounding generalizability, the broader question of generalizability beyond samples, challenges for determining sufficient generalizability, and the inherent question of moderation in psychological science, given the reality of limited time and resources. I then discuss the important roles that basic research plays in understanding group differences, producing generalizable knowledge, and developing applied interventions. Finally, I address issues of equity surrounding sample diversity, emphasizing the distinction between WEIRD samples and convenience samples and the importance of convenience samples for globalizing psychological science. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在过去的 15 年里,随着心理科学家们正视相对单一样本的局限性,对样本多样性和可推广性问题的关注急剧增加。尽管这种反思也许早就应该进行,而且无疑也揭示了一些不良的研究实践,但围绕样本多样性的建议有时却被应用于那些不以在不同民族间实现可推广性为目标的研究中。在本文中,我试图推动关于样本多样性和可推广性何时以及为何重要的讨论。在此过程中,我将讨论有关可推广性的语言问题、样本之外的更广泛的可推广性问题、确定充分的可推广性所面临的挑战,以及在时间和资源有限的现实情况下,心理科学中固有的节制问题。然后,我讨论了基础研究在理解群体差异、产生可推广的知识和开发应用干预措施方面的重要作用。最后,我讨论了与样本多样性相关的公平问题,强调了 WEIRD 样本与便利样本之间的区别,以及便利样本对于心理科学全球化的重要性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"There is nothing WEIRD about basic research: The critical role of convenience samples in psychological science.","authors":"Jeffrey W Sherman","doi":"10.1037/amp0001443","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001443","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Attention to issues of sample diversity and generalizability has increased dramatically in the past 15 years, as psychological scientists have confronted the limitations of relatively homogeneous samples. Though this reckoning was perhaps overdue and has undoubtedly shined a light on some poor research practices, recommendations surrounding sample diversity are sometimes applied to research that does not aim for generalizability across peoples. In this article, I seek to promote discussion about when and why sample diversity and generalizability matter. In doing so, I address problems with language surrounding generalizability, the broader question of generalizability beyond samples, challenges for determining sufficient generalizability, and the inherent question of moderation in psychological science, given the reality of limited time and resources. I then discuss the important roles that basic research plays in understanding group differences, producing generalizable knowledge, and developing applied interventions. Finally, I address issues of equity surrounding sample diversity, emphasizing the distinction between WEIRD samples and convenience samples and the importance of convenience samples for globalizing psychological science. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142606733","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to "The narcissistic appeal of leadership theories" by Steffens et al. (2022). 对 Steffens 等人(2022 年)撰写的 "领导力理论的自恋魅力 "的更正。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001418

Reports an error in "The narcissistic appeal of leadership theories" by Niklas K. Steffens, Mark S. P. Chong and S. Alexander Haslam (American Psychologist, 2022[Feb-Mar], Vol 77[2], 234-248). In the article, Mark S. P. Chong was incorrectly omitted from the author list. The online version of this article has been corrected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2020-81554-001.) Leadership is one of the most researched topics in psychological and other social and behavioral sciences. It is routinely seen as vital to the success and vitality of various forms of collaborative activity not only in organizations but in society at large. This has provided the stimulus for a massive amount of theoretical and applied research and also supports a huge industry. But to whom does this body of work appeal? More specifically, does it appeal to people with a broad interest in advancing groups and society or to people who are primarily interested in promoting themselves? To answer this question, we explore the extent to which individuals' narcissism predicts their endorsement of leadership theories. Results provide empirical evidence that the more narcissistic people are, the more they find leadership theories appealing and the more interest they have in learning about the ideas behind particular theories. The predictive power of narcissism also holds when accounting for other variables (including demographic, Big Five traits, and ideological and motivational variables). We conclude that psychological theorizing about leadership can be a double-edged sword in so far as the lionization of leaders(hip) appeals to, and legitimizes, the tastes of a narcissistic audience. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

报告 Niklas K. Steffens、Mark S. P. Chong 和 S. Alexander Haslam 所著《领导力理论的自恋魅力》(《美国心理学家》,2022 年[2-3 月],第 77[2] 卷,234-248 页)中的一处错误。文章中,Mark S. P. Chong 被错误地从作者名单中删除。本文网络版已作更正。(以下为 2020-81554-001 号记录中的原文摘要)。领导力是心理学及其他社会和行为科学领域研究最多的课题之一。不仅在组织中,而且在整个社会中,人们都认为领导力对于各种形式的合作活动的成功和活力至关重要。这为大量的理论和应用研究提供了动力,也为一个庞大的产业提供了支持。但是,这些工作对谁有吸引力呢?更具体地说,它是吸引那些对促进团体和社会发展有着广泛兴趣的人,还是吸引那些主要对提升自己感兴趣的人?为了回答这个问题,我们探讨了个人的自恋在多大程度上能预测他们对领导力理论的认可程度。结果提供的经验证据表明,越是自恋的人,越觉得领导力理论有吸引力,越有兴趣了解特定理论背后的思想。当考虑到其他变量(包括人口统计学、五大特质、意识形态和动机变量)时,自恋的预测能力也是成立的。我们的结论是,关于领导力的心理学理论研究可能是一把双刃剑,因为对领导者(时髦人物)的赞美迎合了自恋受众的口味,并使之合法化。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Correction to \"The narcissistic appeal of leadership theories\" by Steffens et al. (2022).","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/amp0001418","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001418","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Reports an error in \"The narcissistic appeal of leadership theories\" by Niklas K. Steffens, Mark S. P. Chong and S. Alexander Haslam (<i>American Psychologist</i>, 2022[Feb-Mar], Vol 77[2], 234-248). In the article, Mark S. P. Chong was incorrectly omitted from the author list. The online version of this article has been corrected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2020-81554-001.) Leadership is one of the most researched topics in psychological and other social and behavioral sciences. It is routinely seen as vital to the success and vitality of various forms of collaborative activity not only in organizations but in society at large. This has provided the stimulus for a massive amount of theoretical and applied research and also supports a huge industry. But to whom does this body of work appeal? More specifically, does it appeal to people with a broad interest in advancing groups and society or to people who are primarily interested in promoting themselves? To answer this question, we explore the extent to which individuals' narcissism predicts their endorsement of leadership theories. Results provide empirical evidence that the more narcissistic people are, the more they find leadership theories appealing and the more interest they have in learning about the ideas behind particular theories. The predictive power of narcissism also holds when accounting for other variables (including demographic, Big Five traits, and ideological and motivational variables). We conclude that psychological theorizing about leadership can be a double-edged sword in so far as the lionization of leaders(hip) appeals to, and legitimizes, the tastes of a narcissistic audience. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":"79 8","pages":"1048"},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630824","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Resilience to major life events: Advancing trajectory modeling and resilience factor identification by controlling for background stressor exposure. 对重大生活事件的复原力:通过控制背景压力暴露,推进轨迹建模和复原力因素识别。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001315
Kira F Ahrens, Charlotte Schenk, Bianca Kollmann, Lara M C Puhlmann, Rebecca J Neumann, Sarah K Schäfer, Dorota Reis, Ulrike Basten, Danuta Weichert, Christian J Fiebach, Beat Lutz, Michèle Wessa, Jonathan Repple, Klaus Lieb, Oliver Tüscher, Andreas Reif, Raffael Kalisch, Michael M Plichta

Resilience has been defined as the maintenance or quick recovery of mental health during and after stressor exposure. One popular operationalization of this concept is to model prototypical trajectories of mental health in response to an adverse event, where trajectories of undisturbed low or rapidly recovering symptoms both comply with the resilience definition. However, mental health responses are likely also influenced by other stressors occurring before or during the observation time window. These "background" stressors may affect a person's assignment to a trajectory class. When using these classes as dependent variables to identify resilience-predictive factors, this may lead to false estimates. A new method to build exposure-controlled trajectories based on time courses of stressor reactivity (SR), rather than pure mental health scores, is demonstrated on a data set of 707 initially healthy participants living in Germany (67.33% female; Mage = 29.20, SD = 8.27). SR scores express individual deviations from the sample's normative mental health reaction to observed real-life stressors during the observation time window, thus accounting for individual differences in exposure to background stressors. The resulting trajectory models are plausible. In analyses additionally controlling for background stressors occurring before the observation time window (past life events), low SR trajectories are predicted by the well-documented resilience factor sense of coherence, suggesting construct validity. Further, they are associated with lower odds of developing categorical mental health conditions, suggesting predictive validity. Our study provides the first proof of principle for a refined method to identify predictors of resilience to major stressor events. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

复原力被定义为在受到压力时或压力过后心理健康的维持或快速恢复。对这一概念的一种流行的操作方法是,模拟心理健康在应对不利事件时的原型轨迹,其中不受干扰的低症状或快速恢复的症状轨迹都符合复原力的定义。然而,心理健康反应也可能受到观察时间窗口之前或期间发生的其他压力因素的影响。这些 "背景 "压力可能会影响一个人的轨迹分类。当使用这些类别作为因变量来识别复原力预测因素时,可能会导致错误的估计。我们在一个由 707 名居住在德国、初始健康的参与者(67.33% 为女性;年龄 = 29.20,标准差 = 8.27)组成的数据集上,展示了一种基于压力源反应性(SR)时间历程而非纯粹的心理健康评分来构建暴露控制轨迹的新方法。SR 分数表示在观察时间窗口内,个体对观察到的现实生活压力的心理健康反应与样本正常心理健康反应之间的偏差,从而反映出个体在背景压力下的暴露差异。由此得出的轨迹模型是可信的。在对观察时间窗之前发生的背景压力因素(过去的生活事件)进行额外控制的分析中,低 SR 轨迹是由已被充分证明的复原力因素一致性感预测的,这表明了建构的有效性。此外,低 SR 轨迹还与较低的分类精神健康状况发生几率相关,这也表明了预测的有效性。我们的研究首次证明了确定重大压力事件复原力预测因子的改进方法的原理。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Resilience to major life events: Advancing trajectory modeling and resilience factor identification by controlling for background stressor exposure.","authors":"Kira F Ahrens, Charlotte Schenk, Bianca Kollmann, Lara M C Puhlmann, Rebecca J Neumann, Sarah K Schäfer, Dorota Reis, Ulrike Basten, Danuta Weichert, Christian J Fiebach, Beat Lutz, Michèle Wessa, Jonathan Repple, Klaus Lieb, Oliver Tüscher, Andreas Reif, Raffael Kalisch, Michael M Plichta","doi":"10.1037/amp0001315","DOIUrl":"10.1037/amp0001315","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Resilience has been defined as the maintenance or quick recovery of mental health during and after stressor exposure. One popular operationalization of this concept is to model prototypical trajectories of mental health in response to an adverse event, where trajectories of undisturbed low or rapidly recovering symptoms both comply with the resilience definition. However, mental health responses are likely also influenced by other stressors occurring before or during the observation time window. These \"background\" stressors may affect a person's assignment to a trajectory class. When using these classes as dependent variables to identify resilience-predictive factors, this may lead to false estimates. A new method to build exposure-controlled trajectories based on time courses of stressor reactivity (SR), rather than pure mental health scores, is demonstrated on a data set of 707 initially healthy participants living in Germany (67.33% female; Mage = 29.20, SD = 8.27). SR scores express individual deviations from the sample's normative mental health reaction to observed real-life stressors during the observation time window, thus accounting for individual differences in exposure to background stressors. The resulting trajectory models are plausible. In analyses additionally controlling for background stressors occurring before the observation time window (past life events), low SR trajectories are predicted by the well-documented resilience factor sense of coherence, suggesting construct validity. Further, they are associated with lower odds of developing categorical mental health conditions, suggesting predictive validity. Our study provides the first proof of principle for a refined method to identify predictors of resilience to major stressor events. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":"79 8","pages":"1076-1091"},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142629666","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Early adolescents' ethnic-racial discrimination and pubertal development: Parents' ethnic-racial identities promote adolescents' resilience. 早期青少年的民族-种族歧视与青春期发育:父母的种族认同促进青少年的适应能力。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001284
Juan Del Toro, Riana E Anderson, Xiaoran Sun, Richard M Lee

Ethnically and racially underrepresented adolescents are experiencing pubertal development earlier in life than prior cohorts and their White American peers. This early onset of puberty is partly attributable to ethnic-racial discrimination. To contribute to adolescents' resilience and posttraumatic growth in the face of ethnic-racial discrimination, parents' ethnic-racial identities may spill over into their parenting beliefs and practices. Parents who have a sense of belonging with and commitment to their ethnic-racial identities may be aware of discrimination and actively and consistently engage in practices that build supportive home environments to support their children's development in the context of ethnic-racial discrimination. To assess whether parents' ethnic-racial identity commitment predicted adolescents' resilience against ethnic-racial discrimination, we used multiple waves of survey data from adolescent siblings and their parents participating in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development study (N-adolescents = 1,651; N-families = 805; 35% Black, 37% Latinx, 3% Asian, 25% other ethnically and racially underrepresented youth; 49% boys, 50% girls, 1% gender nonconforming youth; Mage = 11.49, SD = 0.51). Results indicated that adolescents who experienced more frequent ethnic-racial discrimination than their siblings showed more advanced pubertal development. Parental ethnic-racial identity commitment reduced the relation between discrimination and pubertal development within a family. Results suggest that ethnic-racial identity commitment in parents can protect children when they experience ethnic-racial discrimination. Building on extant propositions related to resilience (Infurna & Luthar, 2018), the present study amplifies the depiction of resilience, yields recommendations for analysis of future research, and provides implications regarding the role of ethnicity-race in familial practices. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

与以前的同龄人和美国白人同龄人相比,在族裔和种族上代表性不足的青少年青春期发育较早。青春期发育提前的部分原因是种族歧视。为了促进青少年在面对族裔-种族歧视时的复原力和创伤后成长,父母的族裔-种族身份可能会影响到他们的育儿信念和做法。对自己的民族-种族身份有归属感和承诺的父母可能会意识到歧视,并积极、持续地参与到营造支持性家庭环境的实践中,以支持孩子在民族-种族歧视背景下的发展。为了评估父母的民族-种族身份承诺是否能预测青少年抵御民族-种族歧视的能力,我们使用了参与青少年大脑认知发展研究的青少年兄弟姐妹及其父母的多波调查数据(青少年人数 = 1,651;家庭人数 = 805;35% 为黑人,37% 为拉丁裔,3% 为亚裔,25% 为其他民族和种族代表性不足的青少年;49% 为男孩,50% 为女孩,1% 为性别不符青少年;Mage = 11.49,SD = 0.51)。结果表明,与兄弟姐妹相比,遭受更多民族-种族歧视的青少年的青春期发育更快。父母的种族认同承诺降低了家庭中歧视与青春期发育之间的关系。研究结果表明,父母的种族认同承诺可以在孩子遭受种族歧视时保护他们。本研究在与复原力相关的现有命题(Infurna & Luthar, 2018)的基础上,扩大了对复原力的描述,提出了对未来研究分析的建议,并就民族-种族在家庭实践中的作用提供了启示。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Early adolescents' ethnic-racial discrimination and pubertal development: Parents' ethnic-racial identities promote adolescents' resilience.","authors":"Juan Del Toro, Riana E Anderson, Xiaoran Sun, Richard M Lee","doi":"10.1037/amp0001284","DOIUrl":"10.1037/amp0001284","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Ethnically and racially underrepresented adolescents are experiencing pubertal development earlier in life than prior cohorts and their White American peers. This early onset of puberty is partly attributable to ethnic-racial discrimination. To contribute to adolescents' resilience and posttraumatic growth in the face of ethnic-racial discrimination, parents' ethnic-racial identities may spill over into their parenting beliefs and practices. Parents who have a sense of belonging with and commitment to their ethnic-racial identities may be aware of discrimination and actively and consistently engage in practices that build supportive home environments to support their children's development in the context of ethnic-racial discrimination. To assess whether parents' ethnic-racial identity commitment predicted adolescents' resilience against ethnic-racial discrimination, we used multiple waves of survey data from adolescent siblings and their parents participating in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development study (N-adolescents = 1,651; N-families = 805; 35% Black, 37% Latinx, 3% Asian, 25% other ethnically and racially underrepresented youth; 49% boys, 50% girls, 1% gender nonconforming youth; Mage = 11.49, SD = 0.51). Results indicated that adolescents who experienced more frequent ethnic-racial discrimination than their siblings showed more advanced pubertal development. Parental ethnic-racial identity commitment reduced the relation between discrimination and pubertal development within a family. Results suggest that ethnic-racial identity commitment in parents can protect children when they experience ethnic-racial discrimination. Building on extant propositions related to resilience (Infurna & Luthar, 2018), the present study amplifies the depiction of resilience, yields recommendations for analysis of future research, and provides implications regarding the role of ethnicity-race in familial practices. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":"79 8","pages":"1109-1122"},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630826","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Beyond "post," "traumatic," "growth," and prediction in research on posttraumatic growth. 超越 "创伤后"、"创伤"、"成长 "以及创伤后成长研究中的预测。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001398
Jonathan M Adler, Ted Schwaba

Thirty years after the introduction of posttraumatic growth (PTG), research on the concept has expanded dramatically. Novel theoretical perspectives included in this special issue, however, demonstrate that nearly every element of PTG requires revision. "Post" implies a definitive before and after adversity that simply does not exist, either empirically or in the everyday navigation of adversity, especially for marginalized people. "Trauma" is appropriately scaled to the gravity of some forms of adversity, yet the term is often overly pathologizing or flattening of individual experience. And "growth" is often misleading, difficult to operationalize, and always value-laden. Studying PTG requires grappling with these claims in a way that can inspire pessimism. What is left in PTG after we question the P, T, and G? In asking this question, we ultimately encounter the limits of empiricism. Drawing insights from contemporary research in lifespan development, we suggest that it may be impossible to prospectively predict, using individual-level variables, how people grapple with adversity and develop after it. There are limits to our understanding of PTG that may simply be insurmountable. But complementary perspectives in narrative research, especially those espoused in this issue, as well as in the humanities and the arts, offer a way forward. Retrospectively understanding adverse events and taking an idiographic and qualitative perspective on the ways in which people navigate them can both humanize and bolster inclusivity in PTG research. We conclude by suggesting a period of enhanced divergent exploration, one that embraces disciplinary humility and epistemological and methodological pluralism to further understand PTG. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

创伤后成长(Posttraumatic Growth,PTG)提出 30 年后,对这一概念的研究有了显著的扩展。然而,本特刊所收录的新理论观点表明,创伤后成长的几乎所有要素都需要修正。后 "意味着逆境之前和之后的确定性,但无论是在经验上还是在日常应对逆境的过程中,尤其是对于边缘化人群来说,这种确定性都是不存在的。"创伤 "一词与某些形式的逆境的严重程度相称,但往往过于病态化或扁平化个人经历。而 "成长 "往往具有误导性,难以操作,而且总是带有价值色彩。研究 PTG 需要以一种可能引发悲观情绪的方式来应对这些说法。在我们对 P、T 和 G 提出质疑之后,PTG 还剩下什么?在提出这个问题时,我们最终会遇到经验主义的局限。从当代生命发展研究中汲取灵感,我们认为,利用个人层面的变量来前瞻性地预测人们如何应对逆境以及逆境后的发展可能是不可能的。我们对PTG的理解可能存在根本无法克服的局限性。但是,叙事研究中的互补性视角,尤其是本期所支持的视角,以及人文和艺术视角,为我们提供了一条前进的道路。回顾性地理解不利事件,并从特异性和定性的角度看待人们应对不利事件的方式,既能使 PTG 研究人性化,又能增强 PTG 研究的包容性。最后,我们建议加强发散性探索,以学科的谦逊、认识论和方法论的多元化来进一步理解 PTG。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Beyond \"post,\" \"traumatic,\" \"growth,\" and prediction in research on posttraumatic growth.","authors":"Jonathan M Adler, Ted Schwaba","doi":"10.1037/amp0001398","DOIUrl":"10.1037/amp0001398","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Thirty years after the introduction of posttraumatic growth (PTG), research on the concept has expanded dramatically. Novel theoretical perspectives included in this special issue, however, demonstrate that nearly every element of PTG requires revision. \"Post\" implies a definitive before and after adversity that simply does not exist, either empirically or in the everyday navigation of adversity, especially for marginalized people. \"Trauma\" is appropriately scaled to the gravity of some forms of adversity, yet the term is often overly pathologizing or flattening of individual experience. And \"growth\" is often misleading, difficult to operationalize, and always value-laden. Studying PTG requires grappling with these claims in a way that can inspire pessimism. What is left in PTG after we question the P, T, and G? In asking this question, we ultimately encounter the limits of empiricism. Drawing insights from contemporary research in lifespan development, we suggest that it may be impossible to prospectively predict, using individual-level variables, how people grapple with adversity and develop after it. There are limits to our understanding of PTG that may simply be insurmountable. But complementary perspectives in narrative research, especially those espoused in this issue, as well as in the humanities and the arts, offer a way forward. Retrospectively understanding adverse events and taking an idiographic and qualitative perspective on the ways in which people navigate them can both humanize and bolster inclusivity in PTG research. We conclude by suggesting a period of enhanced divergent exploration, one that embraces disciplinary humility and epistemological and methodological pluralism to further understand PTG. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":"79 8","pages":"1227-1240"},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630821","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do many hands make light work? The role of romantic partners and close relationships in posttraumatic growth. 人多力量大吗?恋爱伴侣和亲密关系在创伤后成长中的作用。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001331
Mariah F Purol, William J Chopik

Our relationships are an important resource for health and well-being in times of need, often buffering the negative effects of stressful situations. Recent research has expanded on these buffering effects, exploring the role of close others in the experience of posttraumatic growth (PTG), or positive personality change that occurs after someone has experienced trauma. In the current review, we examine how much of a role partners play in PTG for individuals, summarizing the existing evidence suggesting that partners can influence the experience of PTG. Additionally, we examine which partner traits or behaviors may facilitate this growth for individuals, discussing relationship-relevant mechanisms, facilitators, and suppressors of PTG. Finally, and perhaps most importantly, we also discuss the quality of existing evidence for the influence of social relationships on PTG, how can we improve the quality of future research, and what is needed for a comprehensive examination of partner-influenced PTG. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在需要的时候,我们的人际关系是健康和幸福的重要资源,往往能缓冲压力环境的负面影响。最近的研究扩展了这些缓冲作用,探讨了亲密的人在创伤后成长(PTG)经历中的作用,或一个人经历创伤后发生的积极人格变化。在本综述中,我们研究了伴侣在个人创伤后成长中的作用,总结了现有证据表明伴侣可以影响创伤后成长的经历。此外,我们还研究了伴侣的哪些特质或行为可能会促进个人的这种成长,讨论了与伴侣关系相关的机制、促进因素以及 PTG 的抑制因素。最后,也许也是最重要的一点,我们还讨论了社会关系对 PTG 影响的现有证据的质量、如何提高未来研究的质量,以及全面研究受伴侣影响的 PTG 所需的条件。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Do many hands make light work? The role of romantic partners and close relationships in posttraumatic growth.","authors":"Mariah F Purol, William J Chopik","doi":"10.1037/amp0001331","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001331","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Our relationships are an important resource for health and well-being in times of need, often buffering the negative effects of stressful situations. Recent research has expanded on these buffering effects, exploring the role of close others in the experience of posttraumatic growth (PTG), or positive personality change that occurs after someone has experienced trauma. In the current review, we examine how much of a role partners play in PTG for individuals, summarizing the existing evidence suggesting that partners can influence the experience of PTG. Additionally, we examine which partner traits or behaviors may facilitate this growth for individuals, discussing relationship-relevant mechanisms, facilitators, and suppressors of PTG. Finally, and perhaps most importantly, we also discuss the quality of existing evidence for the influence of social relationships on PTG, how can we improve the quality of future research, and what is needed for a comprehensive examination of partner-influenced PTG. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":"79 8","pages":"1214-1226"},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630825","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The social determinants of resilience: A conceptual framework to integrate psychological and policy research. 复原力的社会决定因素:整合心理和政策研究的概念框架。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001308
Briana S Last, Noah S Triplett, Emma E McGinty, Claire R Waller, Gabriela Kattan Khazanov, Rinad S Beidas

The psychological study of resilience has increasingly underscored the need for children and families to access material and psychological resources to positively adapt to significant stress. Redistributive policies-policies that downwardly reallocate society's social and economic resources-can offer economically disadvantaged families sustained access to these resources and mitigate the harmful impacts of adversity. This conceptual article builds upon and integrates insights from psychological and policy research to develop a unifying multilevel resilience framework, which we call the Social Determinants of Resilience. We examine four U.S. redistributive policies that have been extensively studied for their effects on child and family outcomes as case studies: (1) Medicaid expansion; (2) the Earned Income Tax Credit; (3) childcare subsidies; and (4) Temporary Assistance for Needy Families. Informed by a scoping review of each policy, we propose that redistributive policies promote children's resilience through three mechanisms by (1) increasing families' resource and service access; (2) reducing family stress; and (3) enhancing adaptive cognitions, emotions, behaviors, and interpersonal processes that protect against the development of psychopathology and promote positive mental health outcomes. Highlighting current evidence for these resilience mechanisms as well as gaps in knowledge, we conclude by setting a multidisciplinary research agenda that can leverage this conceptual framework to advance the science on how redistributive policies enable children and families to thrive. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

对复原力的心理学研究日益强调,儿童和家庭需要获得物质和心理资源,以积极适应重大压力。再分配政策--向下重新分配社会和经济资源的政策--可以让经济上处于不利地位的家庭持续获得这些资源,并减轻逆境的有害影响。这篇概念性文章基于并整合了心理学和政策研究的见解,建立了一个统一的多层次复原力框架,我们称之为 "复原力的社会决定因素"。作为案例研究,我们考察了四项美国再分配政策,这些政策对儿童和家庭结果的影响已得到广泛研究:(1) 扩大医疗补助;(2) 收入所得税抵免;(3) 儿童保育补贴;(4) 贫困家庭临时援助。根据对每项政策的范围审查,我们建议再分配政策通过以下三种机制促进儿童的复原力:(1)增加家庭获得资源和服务的机会;(2)减少家庭压力;(3)增强适应性认知、情感、行为和人际交往过程,从而防止心理病理学的发展并促进积极的心理健康结果。最后,我们强调了这些复原机制的现有证据以及知识差距,并制定了一个多学科研究议程,该议程可利用这一概念框架来推动有关再分配政策如何使儿童和家庭茁壮成长的科学研究。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"The social determinants of resilience: A conceptual framework to integrate psychological and policy research.","authors":"Briana S Last, Noah S Triplett, Emma E McGinty, Claire R Waller, Gabriela Kattan Khazanov, Rinad S Beidas","doi":"10.1037/amp0001308","DOIUrl":"10.1037/amp0001308","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The psychological study of resilience has increasingly underscored the need for children and families to access material and psychological resources to positively adapt to significant stress. Redistributive policies-policies that downwardly reallocate society's social and economic resources-can offer economically disadvantaged families sustained access to these resources and mitigate the harmful impacts of adversity. This conceptual article builds upon and integrates insights from psychological and policy research to develop a unifying multilevel resilience framework, which we call the Social Determinants of Resilience. We examine four U.S. redistributive policies that have been extensively studied for their effects on child and family outcomes as case studies: (1) Medicaid expansion; (2) the Earned Income Tax Credit; (3) childcare subsidies; and (4) Temporary Assistance for Needy Families. Informed by a scoping review of each policy, we propose that redistributive policies promote children's resilience through three mechanisms by (1) increasing families' resource and service access; (2) reducing family stress; and (3) enhancing adaptive cognitions, emotions, behaviors, and interpersonal processes that protect against the development of psychopathology and promote positive mental health outcomes. Highlighting current evidence for these resilience mechanisms as well as gaps in knowledge, we conclude by setting a multidisciplinary research agenda that can leverage this conceptual framework to advance the science on how redistributive policies enable children and families to thrive. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":"79 8","pages":"1049-1062"},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142629682","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Donald N. Bersoff (1939-2024). 唐纳德-N-贝尔索夫(1939-2024)。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-05 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001401
Kirk Heilbrun, David DeMatteo, Naomi E Goldstein

This article memorializes Donald N. Bersoff (1939-2024), who provided a foundational influence on the development of American law-psychology and served as the 2013 president of the American Psychological Association (APA). Don had a long and meaningful career as a psychologist and a lawyer. One of Don's greatest interests was in training psychologist-lawyers who would make meaningful contributions to either field. After a decade as APA general counsel, he was recruited to direct the law-psychology program at Hahnemann University and Villanova School of Law. He was elected as the president of the APA in 2013; two of the major themes of his presidency involved encouraging service to military veterans and their families, and promoting diversity within the ranks of psychology to better serve an increasingly diverse population. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本文纪念唐纳德-贝尔索夫(Donald N. Bersoff,1939-2024 年),他对美国法律心理学的发展产生了奠基性的影响,并曾担任美国心理学会 (APA) 2013 年主席。作为一名心理学家和律师,唐的职业生涯漫长而有意义。唐最大的兴趣之一是培养能在这两个领域做出有意义贡献的心理学家-律师。在担任美国心理学会总顾问十年后,他受聘指导哈内曼大学和维拉诺瓦法学院的法律心理学课程。他于 2013 年当选为美国心理学会主席;他担任主席期间的两大主题是鼓励为退伍军人及其家人提供服务,以及促进心理学队伍的多元化,以便更好地为日益多元化的人群服务。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Donald N. Bersoff (1939-2024).","authors":"Kirk Heilbrun, David DeMatteo, Naomi E Goldstein","doi":"10.1037/amp0001401","DOIUrl":"10.1037/amp0001401","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This article memorializes Donald N. Bersoff (1939-2024), who provided a foundational influence on the development of American law-psychology and served as the 2013 president of the American Psychological Association (APA). Don had a long and meaningful career as a psychologist and a lawyer. One of Don's greatest interests was in training psychologist-lawyers who would make meaningful contributions to either field. After a decade as APA general counsel, he was recruited to direct the law-psychology program at Hahnemann University and Villanova School of Law. He was elected as the president of the APA in 2013; two of the major themes of his presidency involved encouraging service to military veterans and their families, and promoting diversity within the ranks of psychology to better serve an increasingly diverse population. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":" ","pages":"1241-1242"},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141890588","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
American Psychologist
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1