首页 > 最新文献

American Psychologist最新文献

英文 中文
Shifting the gaze from racism to healing from racism: A systematic review of selected psychology journals from 1992 to 2022. 将目光从种族主义转向治愈种族主义:对1992年至2022年部分心理学期刊的系统回顾。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001317
Helen A Neville, Mahogany Monette, Jarrett T Lewis, Salman Safir

Using a decolonial approach, we provided a narrative review of the research on racism in psychology and conducted a systematic review of the top five psychology journals publishing research on racism and mental health to identify trends in racism research over time and the research gaps. We examined 372 articles on racism published between 1992 and 2022: American Psychologist, Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, Journal of Black Psychology, Journal of Counseling Psychology, and The Counseling Psychologist. Based on our review, we found that published research examining racism has steadily increased over the past 3 decades, with the greatest spikes in 2021 and 2022. The largest increase was in studies focused on People of Color's experiences with racism. The overwhelming majority of the articles were empirical (86.3%) and most of these studies (87.5%) employed cross-sectional designs. We identified corollary topics by racial/ethnic group, prevalent research designs, and the emergence of strength-based and healing approaches to address racism's impact. There were general racial and ethnic differences in trends, with research on various People of Color groups focused on the harmful effects of racism and research on White populations focused on Whiteness and level of awareness of racism. We conclude with recommendations to enhance the content and methodological rigor of future research while also suggesting policy implications to support advancements in this critical area of study. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们采用非殖民主义方法,对心理学中的种族主义研究进行了叙述性回顾,并对发表种族主义和心理健康研究的五大心理学期刊进行了系统性回顾,以确定种族主义研究的发展趋势和研究差距。我们研究了 1992 年至 2022 年间发表的 372 篇有关种族主义的文章:《美国心理学家》(American Psychologist)、《文化多样性与少数民族心理学》(Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology)、《黑人心理学杂志》(Journal of Black Psychology)、《咨询心理学杂志》(Journal of Counseling Psychology)和《咨询心理学家》(The Counseling Psychologist)。根据我们的回顾,我们发现在过去 30 年里,发表的研究种族主义的文章稳步增加,其中 2021 年和 2022 年的增幅最大。增幅最大的是以有色人种的种族主义经历为重点的研究。绝大多数文章都是实证研究(86.3%),其中大部分研究(87.5%)采用了横断面设计。我们按种族/族裔群体、流行的研究设计以及应对种族主义影响的基于力量和治疗方法的出现确定了相应的主题。在趋势上存在着普遍的种族和民族差异,针对不同有色人种群体的研究侧重于种族主义的有害影响,而针对白人群体的研究侧重于白种人和对种族主义的认识水平。最后,我们就如何提高未来研究的内容和方法的严谨性提出了建议,同时也提出了政策方面的影响,以支持在这一关键研究领域取得进展。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Shifting the gaze from racism to healing from racism: A systematic review of selected psychology journals from 1992 to 2022.","authors":"Helen A Neville, Mahogany Monette, Jarrett T Lewis, Salman Safir","doi":"10.1037/amp0001317","DOIUrl":"10.1037/amp0001317","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Using a decolonial approach, we provided a narrative review of the research on racism in psychology and conducted a systematic review of the top five psychology journals publishing research on racism and mental health to identify trends in racism research over time and the research gaps. We examined 372 articles on racism published between 1992 and 2022: <i>American Psychologist, Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, Journal of Black Psychology, Journal of Counseling Psychology,</i> and <i>The Counseling Psychologist.</i> Based on our review, we found that published research examining racism has steadily increased over the past 3 decades, with the greatest spikes in 2021 and 2022. The largest increase was in studies focused on People of Color's experiences with racism. The overwhelming majority of the articles were empirical (86.3%) and most of these studies (87.5%) employed cross-sectional designs. We identified corollary topics by racial/ethnic group, prevalent research designs, and the emergence of strength-based and healing approaches to address racism's impact. There were general racial and ethnic differences in trends, with research on various People of Color groups focused on the harmful effects of racism and research on White populations focused on Whiteness and level of awareness of racism. We conclude with recommendations to enhance the content and methodological rigor of future research while also suggesting policy implications to support advancements in this critical area of study. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140874958","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The American Psychological Association and antisemitism: Toward equity, diversity, and inclusion. 美国心理学会与反犹太主义:实现公平、多样性和包容性。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-06-06 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001369
Lenore E A Walker, Ester Cole, Sarah L Friedman, Beth Rom-Rymer, Arlene Steinberg, Susan Warshaw

[Correction Notice: An Erratum for this article was reported online in American Psychologist on Jul 15 2024 (see record 2025-04658-001). In the article, three sentences and a reference were redacted related to proceedings against a university concerning its psychology program because appropriate context was not provided in the article. All versions of this article have been corrected.] This article calls for the American Psychological Association (APA) to proactively include the elimination of antisemitism or prejudice against Jewish people in its current mission to disassemble all forms of racism from its organization as well as society. In this article, Jews (estimated as 2.4% of the population) are defined as a people with a common identity, ethnicity, and religion as they experience prejudice; their intersection in Jewish identity; the history and characteristics of antisemitism and its current manifestation in public life, academic institutions, and psychology. Despite Jews having made major contributions to the development of psychology as a profession, historically through the first half of the 20th century, Jews were systematically discriminated against within the discipline of psychology through quotas for acceptance into graduate training, discriminatory employment practices in university psychology departments, and most egregiously through the espousing of "scientific racism" including eugenics by prominent leaders in the APA. We describe how historically leaders in the APA engaged in overt and covert antisemitism while the APA continues to do little or nothing to combat it. We then offer suggestions for the mitigation and elimination of this form of bias, discrimination, and hate as it once again escalates in society. We recommend that the APA engages in research about antisemitism, its predictors, consequences, and power; evaluates the efficacy of intervention programs; encourages contact with various multicultural minoritized groups; and disseminates knowledge to educate about the psychological effects of antisemitism. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本文呼吁美国心理学会(APA)积极主动地将消除反犹太主义或对犹太人的偏见纳入其当前的使命中,从其组织和社会中瓦解一切形式的种族主义。在本文中,犹太人(估计占总人口的 2.4%)被定义为具有共同身份、种族和宗教信仰的民族,因为他们经历过偏见;他们在犹太身份中的交集;反犹太主义的历史和特点及其目前在公共生活、学术机构和心理学中的表现。尽管犹太人为心理学作为一门专业的发展做出了重大贡献,但在 20 世纪上半叶的历史上,犹太人在心理学学科内受到了系统性的歧视,包括研究生培训的录取配额、大学心理学系的歧视性就业措施,以及最令人震惊的美国心理学会(APA)著名领导人对包括优生学在内的 "科学种族主义 "的拥护。我们描述了美国心理学会的领导人如何在历史上公开和隐蔽地进行反犹太主义活动,而美国心理学会却仍然很少或根本没有采取任何措施来打击这种活动。然后,我们提出建议,以减轻和消除这种形式的偏见、歧视和仇恨,因为它再次在社会上升级。我们建议美国心理学会参与有关反犹太主义及其预测因素、后果和力量的研究;评估干预计划的效果;鼓励与各种多元文化的少数群体接触;传播知识,教育人们了解反犹太主义的心理影响。(PsycInfo 数据库记录 (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The American Psychological Association and antisemitism: Toward equity, diversity, and inclusion.","authors":"Lenore E A Walker, Ester Cole, Sarah L Friedman, Beth Rom-Rymer, Arlene Steinberg, Susan Warshaw","doi":"10.1037/amp0001369","DOIUrl":"10.1037/amp0001369","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>[Correction Notice: An Erratum for this article was reported online in <i>American Psychologist</i> on Jul 15 2024 (see record 2025-04658-001). In the article, three sentences and a reference were redacted related to proceedings against a university concerning its psychology program because appropriate context was not provided in the article. All versions of this article have been corrected.] This article calls for the American Psychological Association (APA) to proactively include the elimination of antisemitism or prejudice against Jewish people in its current mission to disassemble all forms of racism from its organization as well as society. In this article, Jews (estimated as 2.4% of the population) are defined as a people with a common identity, ethnicity, and religion as they experience prejudice; their intersection in Jewish identity; the history and characteristics of antisemitism and its current manifestation in public life, academic institutions, and psychology. Despite Jews having made major contributions to the development of psychology as a profession, historically through the first half of the 20th century, Jews were systematically discriminated against within the discipline of psychology through quotas for acceptance into graduate training, discriminatory employment practices in university psychology departments, and most egregiously through the espousing of \"scientific racism\" including eugenics by prominent leaders in the APA. We describe how historically leaders in the APA engaged in overt and covert antisemitism while the APA continues to do little or nothing to combat it. We then offer suggestions for the mitigation and elimination of this form of bias, discrimination, and hate as it once again escalates in society. We recommend that the APA engages in research about antisemitism, its predictors, consequences, and power; evaluates the efficacy of intervention programs; encourages contact with various multicultural minoritized groups; and disseminates knowledge to educate about the psychological effects of antisemitism. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141263245","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What is a mantra? Guidance for practitioners, researchers, and editors. 什么是咒语?为从业人员、研究人员和编辑提供指导。
IF 16.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-30 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001368
Doug Oman

Mantras, sometimes called holy names or prayer words, are increasingly included and studied as components in health and human services interventions. In this emerging field, the term "mantra" has been implicitly defined over several decades in a way that has been useful, largely shared across research teams, and historically resonant. However, confusion has arisen in how "mantra" is defined and used in a small fraction of recent publications that depart from longstanding usage. To provide needed guidance going forward for researchers, editors, reviewers, and practitioners, the present article discusses historical, cross-cultural, conceptual, and empirical background and proposes a definition of "mantra" for use in empirical research on mantra interventions: A mantra is a phrase or sound that has been repeated and sanctified over time within a spiritual tradition. Using this definition, we categorize several dozen empirical studies of mantra repetition interventions. Emphasizing well-established psychosocial processes such as priming and spreading activation, we explain theoretical and empirical bases for expecting repetition of mantras to enlist spiritual resources and provide added value for health and well-being, over and above the repetition of neutral non-mantra sounds or phrases. Although the term mantra should be used carefully in professional discourse, we allow that looser definitions can be acceptable in communications with intervention recipients, clients, and patients, parallel to recent recommendations for how to employ the term "spirituality." Directions for future research are suggested. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

咒语,有时也被称为圣名或祈祷词,作为健康和人类服务干预措施的组成部分,被越来越多地纳入研究范围。在这一新兴领域中,"咒语 "一词在过去几十年中已被隐含地定义为一种有用的方式,在很大程度上为各研究团队所共享,并在历史上产生了共鸣。然而,在最近发表的一小部分出版物中,对 "咒语 "的定义和使用方式出现了混乱,与长期以来的用法大相径庭。为了给研究人员、编辑、审稿人和从业人员提供必要的指导,本文讨论了历史、跨文化、概念和实证背景,并提出了 "咒语 "的定义,供咒语干预实证研究使用:咒语是在精神传统中被长期重复和神圣化的短语或声音。根据这一定义,我们对几十项有关咒语重复干预的实证研究进行了分类。我们强调社会心理过程(如引子和传播激活)的完善,并解释了理论和实证依据,即与重复中性的非咒语声音或短语相比,重复咒语可获得精神资源,并为健康和幸福带来附加价值。虽然在专业讨论中应谨慎使用 "咒语 "一词,但我们认为在与干预对象、客户和患者交流时,可以接受宽松的定义,这与最近关于如何使用 "灵性 "一词的建议是一致的。我们提出了未来的研究方向。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"What is a mantra? Guidance for practitioners, researchers, and editors.","authors":"Doug Oman","doi":"10.1037/amp0001368","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001368","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Mantras, sometimes called holy names or prayer words, are increasingly included and studied as components in health and human services interventions. In this emerging field, the term \"mantra\" has been implicitly defined over several decades in a way that has been useful, largely shared across research teams, and historically resonant. However, confusion has arisen in how \"mantra\" is defined and used in a small fraction of recent publications that depart from longstanding usage. To provide needed guidance going forward for researchers, editors, reviewers, and practitioners, the present article discusses historical, cross-cultural, conceptual, and empirical background and proposes a definition of \"mantra\" for use in empirical research on mantra interventions: A mantra is a phrase or sound that has been repeated and sanctified over time within a spiritual tradition. Using this definition, we categorize several dozen empirical studies of mantra repetition interventions. Emphasizing well-established psychosocial processes such as priming and spreading activation, we explain theoretical and empirical bases for expecting repetition of mantras to enlist spiritual resources and provide added value for health and well-being, over and above the repetition of neutral non-mantra sounds or phrases. Although the term mantra should be used carefully in professional discourse, we allow that looser definitions can be acceptable in communications with intervention recipients, clients, and patients, parallel to recent recommendations for how to employ the term \"spirituality.\" Directions for future research are suggested. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":16.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141179904","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Verbal behavior and the future of social science. 语言行为与社会科学的未来。
IF 16.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-30 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001319
Ryan L Boyd, David M Markowitz

Natural language processing (NLP)-previously the domain of a select few language and computer scientists-is undergoing an unprecedented surge in popularity across disciplines. The ubiquity of language data, alongside extremely rapid methodological innovations, has magnetized the field, attracting researchers with the promise of measuring, forecasting, and understanding the most central questions in business, psychology, biology, sociology, the humanities, and beyond. The power of language analysis to reveal insights into human thought, feeling, and behavior has become a core interest emerging from recent technological advances, which are being probed to unearth deeply embedded truths about the human condition. However, NLP research has reached a critical juncture, sitting at the cusp of societal transformation in many aspects of daily life. The details of how NLP research develops over the next 3-5 years will define this transformation. In this emerging, near-infinite space of NLP-driven research, we provide a critical frame of reference for how, when, and why these technologies should evolve in a particularly transdisciplinary manner. Specifically, we discuss (a) the urgency of pairing existing and emerging NLP research with existing scientific knowledge, theory, and principles from the behavioral sciences; (b) the coevolution of NLP technologies; and (c) the practical implications and ethical consequences of expanding language analysis using broader psychosocial theories of the human condition. While our discussion focuses principally on using language as a window in the individual mind, this topic holds substantial implications for other disciplines and lines of inquiry, including the dynamics of social interaction and beyond. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

自然语言处理(NLP)--以前只是少数语言和计算机科学家的研究领域--正在经历一场前所未有的跨学科普及热潮。语言数据无处不在,加上方法上的快速创新,使这一领域充满磁力,吸引了众多研究人员,有望测量、预测和理解商业、心理学、生物学、社会学、人文学科等领域最核心的问题。语言分析在揭示人类思想、情感和行为方面的洞察力,已成为近年来技术进步所带来的核心兴趣。然而,NLP 研究正处于一个关键时刻,在日常生活的许多方面都处于社会转型的风口浪尖。NLP研究在未来3-5年内的发展细节将决定这场变革的走向。在这一新兴的、近乎无限的NLP研究空间中,我们为这些技术如何、何时以及为何要以一种特别跨学科的方式发展提供了一个重要的参考框架。具体而言,我们将讨论:(a) 将现有和新兴的 NLP 研究与行为科学的现有科学知识、理论和原则相结合的紧迫性;(b) NLP 技术的共同发展;以及 (c) 利用更广泛的人类社会心理理论扩展语言分析的实际意义和伦理后果。虽然我们的讨论主要集中在将语言作为个人心灵的窗口,但这一主题对其他学科和研究领域,包括社会互动动力学及其他领域,都有重大影响。(PsycInfo 数据库记录 (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Verbal behavior and the future of social science.","authors":"Ryan L Boyd, David M Markowitz","doi":"10.1037/amp0001319","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001319","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Natural language processing (NLP)-previously the domain of a select few language and computer scientists-is undergoing an unprecedented surge in popularity across disciplines. The ubiquity of language data, alongside extremely rapid methodological innovations, has magnetized the field, attracting researchers with the promise of measuring, forecasting, and understanding the most central questions in business, psychology, biology, sociology, the humanities, and beyond. The power of language analysis to reveal insights into human thought, feeling, and behavior has become a core interest emerging from recent technological advances, which are being probed to unearth deeply embedded truths about the human condition. However, NLP research has reached a critical juncture, sitting at the cusp of societal transformation in many aspects of daily life. The details of how NLP research develops over the next 3-5 years will define this transformation. In this emerging, near-infinite space of NLP-driven research, we provide a critical frame of reference for how, when, and why these technologies should evolve in a particularly transdisciplinary manner. Specifically, we discuss (a) the urgency of pairing existing and emerging NLP research with existing scientific knowledge, theory, and principles from the behavioral sciences; (b) the coevolution of NLP technologies; and (c) the practical implications and ethical consequences of expanding language analysis using broader psychosocial theories of the human condition. While our discussion focuses principally on using language as a window in the <i>individual mind,</i> this topic holds substantial implications for other disciplines and lines of inquiry, including the dynamics of social interaction and beyond. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":16.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141179899","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social and emotional competency development from fourth to 12th grade: Relations to parental education and gender. 从四年级到十二年级的社交和情感能力发展:与父母教育和性别的关系。
IF 16.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-23 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001357
Sara E Rimm-Kaufman, James Soland, Megan Kuhfeld

Educators have become increasingly committed to social and emotional learning in schools. However, we know too little about the typical growth trajectories of the competencies that schools are striving to improve. We leverage data from the California Office to Reform Education, a consortium of districts in California serving over 1.5 million students, that administers annual surveys to students to measure social and emotional competencies (SECs). This article uses data from six cohorts of approximately 16,000 students each (51% male, 73% Latinx, 11% White, 10% Black, 24% with parents who did not complete high school) in Grades 4-12. Two questions are addressed. First, how much growth occurs in growth mindset, self-efficacy, self-management, and social awareness from Grades 4 to 12? Second, do initial status and growth look different by parental educational attainment and gender? Using accelerated longitudinal design growth models, findings show distinct growth trends among the four SECs with growth mindset increasing, self-management mostly decreasing, and self-efficacy and social awareness decreasing and then increasing. The subgroup analyses show gaps between groups but patterns of growth that are more similar than different. Further, subgroup membership accounts for very little variation in growth or declines. Instead, initial levels of competencies predict growth. Also, variation within groups is greater than variation between groups. The findings have practical implications for educators and psychologists striving to improve SECs. If schools use student-report approaches, predicting steady and consistent positive growth in SECs is unrealistic. Instead, U-shaped patterns for some SECs appear to be normative with notable declines in the sixth grade, requiring new supports. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

教育工作者越来越致力于在学校开展社会和情感学习。然而,我们对学校努力提高的能力的典型成长轨迹知之甚少。我们利用了加州教育改革办公室(California Office to Reform Education)的数据,该机构是加州的一个学区联盟,为超过 150 万名学生提供服务。本文使用的数据来自四至十二年级的六个组群,每个组群约有 16,000 名学生(51% 为男性,73% 为拉丁裔,11% 为白人,10% 为黑人,24% 的父母未完成高中学业)。本研究涉及两个问题。第一,从 4 年级到 12 年级,在成长心态、自我效能感、自我管理和社会意识方面的成长程度如何?其次,初始状态和成长是否因父母的教育程度和性别而有所不同?利用加速纵向设计的成长模型,研究结果显示,四种中等教育证书的成长趋势截然不同:成长心态在增强,自我管理能力主要在减弱,自我效能感和社会意识先减弱后增强。分组分析表明,各组之间存在差距,但成长模式相似多于不同。此外,分组成员资格对增长或下降的影响很小。相反,最初的能力水平预示着增长。此外,组内的差异大于组间的差异。这些研究结果对教育工作者和心理学家努力提高中考成绩具有实际意义。如果学校采用学生报告的方法,预测SEC持续稳定的正增长是不现实的。相反,一些 SEC 的 U 型模式似乎是正常的,但到了六年级就会明显下降,这就需要新的支持。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Social and emotional competency development from fourth to 12th grade: Relations to parental education and gender.","authors":"Sara E Rimm-Kaufman, James Soland, Megan Kuhfeld","doi":"10.1037/amp0001357","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001357","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Educators have become increasingly committed to social and emotional learning in schools. However, we know too little about the typical growth trajectories of the competencies that schools are striving to improve. We leverage data from the California Office to Reform Education, a consortium of districts in California serving over 1.5 million students, that administers annual surveys to students to measure social and emotional competencies (SECs). This article uses data from six cohorts of approximately 16,000 students each (51% male, 73% Latinx, 11% White, 10% Black, 24% with parents who did not complete high school) in Grades 4-12. Two questions are addressed. First, how much growth occurs in growth mindset, self-efficacy, self-management, and social awareness from Grades 4 to 12? Second, do initial status and growth look different by parental educational attainment and gender? Using accelerated longitudinal design growth models, findings show distinct growth trends among the four SECs with growth mindset increasing, self-management mostly decreasing, and self-efficacy and social awareness decreasing and then increasing. The subgroup analyses show gaps between groups but patterns of growth that are more similar than different. Further, subgroup membership accounts for very little variation in growth or declines. Instead, initial levels of competencies predict growth. Also, variation within groups is greater than variation between groups. The findings have practical implications for educators and psychologists striving to improve SECs. If schools use student-report approaches, predicting steady and consistent positive growth in SECs is unrealistic. Instead, U-shaped patterns for some SECs appear to be normative with notable declines in the sixth grade, requiring new supports. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":16.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141080000","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The psychology of sexual and gender diversity in the 21st century: Social technologies and stories of authenticity. 21 世纪性与性别多样性心理学:社会技术与真实性故事。
IF 16.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-23 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001366
Phillip L Hammack, Adriana M Manago

The 21st century has seen shifts in social and scientific understandings of gender and sexuality in the United States. From the legitimization of same-sex marriage to the heightened visibility of transgender identities, nonbinary gender, and forms of intimate diversity such as asexuality, kink, and polyamory, core cultural and scientific assumptions about gender and sexuality have been challenged. This article situates these changes in the historical context of 21st century social technologies, which challenge traditional sources of authority about information and provide enhanced opportunities for individuals to experience authenticity in gender and sexuality. We frame authenticity as a master cultural narrative in the United States characterized by feeling a heightened sense of self-authorship and alignment between inner experience and embodiment of gender and sexuality. Five narratives now circulate in the United States, four of which support sexual and gender diversity: (a) gender as self-constructed; (b) sexuality as plural, playful, flexible, and fluid; (c) sexuality and monogamy as cultural compulsions; and (d) intersectionality as central to the experience of sexuality and gender. A fifth narrative seeking to legitimize hierarchies (e.g., patriarchy) is hostile to sexual and gender diversity but remains anchored in a metanarrative of authenticity and has benefitted equally from the affordances of social technologies. This historical moment provides researchers and practitioners with the opportunity to more intentionally ground their work in lived experience, challenge normative thinking about sexuality and gender, practice affirmation, center the phenomenon of diversity over discrete identity categories in an ever-exclusionary acronym (lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, queer/questioning, and more identities [LGBTQ+]), and embrace fluid and nonlinear narratives of social change. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

21 世纪,美国社会和科学界对性别和性行为的理解发生了转变。从同性婚姻的合法化,到变性身份、非二元性别以及无性恋、变态和多角恋等亲密关系多样性形式的凸显,有关性别和性行为的核心文化和科学假设受到了挑战。本文将这些变化置于 21 世纪社会技术的历史背景下,这些技术挑战了传统的信息权威来源,并为个人体验性别和性行为的真实性提供了更多机会。我们将真实性定义为美国的一种主文化叙事,其特点是自我认同感增强,内心体验与性别和性行为的体现相一致。美国目前流传着五种说法,其中四种支持性与性别的多样性:(a)性别是自我构建的;(b)性行为是多元的、游戏性的、灵活的和多变的;(c)性行为和一夫一妻制是文化强迫;以及(d)交叉性是性与性别体验的核心。寻求等级制度合法化的第五种叙事(如父权制)敌视性和性别多样性,但仍立足于真实性的元叙事,并同样受益于社会技术的能力。这一历史时刻为研究人员和从业人员提供了机会,使他们能够更有意识地将自己的工作立足于生活经验,挑战关于性和性别的规范性思维,践行平权,将多样性现象置于不断排斥的缩写词(女同性恋、男同性恋、双性恋、跨性别者、同性恋者/质疑者以及更多身份[LGBTQ+])中的离散身份类别之上,并接受流动和非线性的社会变革叙事。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The psychology of sexual and gender diversity in the 21st century: Social technologies and stories of authenticity.","authors":"Phillip L Hammack, Adriana M Manago","doi":"10.1037/amp0001366","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001366","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The 21st century has seen shifts in social and scientific understandings of gender and sexuality in the United States. From the legitimization of same-sex marriage to the heightened visibility of transgender identities, nonbinary gender, and forms of intimate diversity such as asexuality, kink, and polyamory, core cultural and scientific assumptions about gender and sexuality have been challenged. This article situates these changes in the historical context of 21st century social technologies, which challenge traditional sources of authority about information and provide enhanced opportunities for individuals to experience authenticity in gender and sexuality. We frame authenticity as a master cultural narrative in the United States characterized by feeling a heightened sense of self-authorship and alignment between inner experience and embodiment of gender and sexuality. Five narratives now circulate in the United States, four of which support sexual and gender diversity: (a) gender as self-constructed; (b) sexuality as plural, playful, flexible, and fluid; (c) sexuality and monogamy as cultural compulsions; and (d) intersectionality as central to the experience of sexuality and gender. A fifth narrative seeking to legitimize hierarchies (e.g., patriarchy) is hostile to sexual and gender diversity but remains anchored in a metanarrative of authenticity and has benefitted equally from the affordances of social technologies. This historical moment provides researchers and practitioners with the opportunity to more intentionally ground their work in lived experience, challenge normative thinking about sexuality and gender, practice affirmation, center the phenomenon of diversity over discrete identity categories in an ever-exclusionary acronym (lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, queer/questioning, and more identities [LGBTQ+]), and embrace fluid and nonlinear narratives of social change. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":16.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141080005","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychologists return to the first question of Western philosophy. 心理学家回到了西方哲学的第一个问题。
IF 16.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-06 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001351
Jeremy D W Clifton

When ancient humans gained the ability to investigate abstract questions, what first question did they pose? This article offers a novel, sweeping, historical analysis with important implications for psychological theory. The story begins with identifying the first question in Ancient Greek philosophy as "Where am I?" with particular interest in the world's overarching basic traits. For example, Pythagoras proposed the world was defined by beauty and Heraclitus suggested change. Though this discourse has traditionally puzzled historians, recent psychological research suggests it might have been largely a debate over primal world beliefs, an emerging research topic that this article introduces and situates historically. Recently, the latent structure of primal world beliefs was mapped statistically, revealing 26 dimensions. Most of these beliefs were new to psychologists, yet already posed by ancient philosophers-including Pythagoras' Beautiful world belief and Heraclitus' Changing world belief. Identifying first questions in early history may have value for psychological theorizing because it hints at something that social psychologists have long suspected: that humans are creatures fundamentally driven to understand their situation and what it calls for. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

当古人类获得研究抽象问题的能力时,他们提出的第一个问题是什么?本文提供了一个新颖、全面的历史分析,对心理学理论具有重要意义。文章首先指出古希腊哲学的第一个问题是 "我在哪里?",并特别关注世界的总体基本特征。例如,毕达哥拉斯提出世界的定义是美,赫拉克利特提出变化。虽然这种讨论历来令历史学家困惑,但最近的心理学研究表明,它可能主要是一场关于原始世界信念的争论,本文介绍了这一新兴研究课题,并对其进行了历史定位。最近,有人对原始世界信念的潜在结构进行了统计,发现了 26 个维度。这些信念中的大多数对心理学家来说都是新的,但古代哲学家已经提出过--包括毕达哥拉斯的 "美丽世界信念 "和赫拉克利特的 "变化世界信念"。确定早期历史中的第一个问题可能对心理学理论研究有价值,因为它暗示了社会心理学家长期以来一直怀疑的一点:人类是一种从根本上被驱使去了解自己的处境及其要求的生物。(PsycInfo 数据库记录 (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Psychologists return to the first question of Western philosophy.","authors":"Jeremy D W Clifton","doi":"10.1037/amp0001351","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001351","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When ancient humans gained the ability to investigate abstract questions, what first question did they pose? This article offers a novel, sweeping, historical analysis with important implications for psychological theory. The story begins with identifying the first question in Ancient Greek philosophy as \"Where am I?\" with particular interest in the world's overarching basic traits. For example, Pythagoras proposed the world was defined by beauty and Heraclitus suggested change. Though this discourse has traditionally puzzled historians, recent psychological research suggests it might have been largely a debate over primal world beliefs, an emerging research topic that this article introduces and situates historically. Recently, the latent structure of primal world beliefs was mapped statistically, revealing 26 dimensions. Most of these beliefs were new to psychologists, yet already posed by ancient philosophers-including Pythagoras' <i>Beautiful</i> world belief and Heraclitus' <i>Changing</i> world belief. Identifying first questions in early history may have value for psychological theorizing because it hints at something that social psychologists have long suspected: that humans are creatures fundamentally driven to understand their situation and what it calls for. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":16.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140868298","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Responsible data sharing: Identifying and remedying possible re-identification of human participants. 负责任的数据共享:识别和纠正可能存在的对人类参与者身份的重新识别。
IF 16.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-06 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001346
Kirsten N Morehouse, Benedek Kurdi, Brian A Nosek

Open data collected from research participants creates a tension between scholarly values of transparency and sharing, on the one hand, and privacy and security, on the other hand. A common solution is to make data sets anonymous by removing personally identifying information (e.g., names or worker IDs) before sharing. However, ostensibly anonymized data sets may be at risk of re-identification if they include demographic information. In the present article, we provide researchers with broadly applicable guidance and tangible tools so that they can engage in open science practices without jeopardizing participants' privacy. Specifically, we (a) review current privacy standards, (b) describe computer science data protection frameworks and their adaptability to the social sciences, (c) provide practical guidance for assessing and addressing re-identification risk, (d) introduce two open-source algorithms developed for psychological scientists-MinBlur and MinBlurLite-to increase privacy while maintaining the integrity of open data, and (e) highlight aspects of ethical data sharing that require further attention. Ultimately, the risk of re-identification should not dissuade engagement with open science practices. Instead, technical innovations should be developed and harnessed so that science can be as open as possible to promote transparency and sharing and as closed as necessary to maintain privacy and security. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

从研究参与者那里收集到的开放数据在学术价值--透明度和共享--与隐私和安全之间产生了矛盾。一种常见的解决方案是在共享前删除个人身份信息(如姓名或工作者 ID),从而使数据集匿名。然而,表面上匿名的数据集如果包含人口统计信息,可能会有被重新识别的风险。在本文中,我们为研究人员提供了广泛适用的指导和切实可行的工具,使他们能够在不损害参与者隐私的情况下参与开放科学实践。具体来说,我们(a)回顾了当前的隐私标准,(b)介绍了计算机科学数据保护框架及其对社会科学的适应性,(c)提供了评估和解决重新识别风险的实用指南,(d)介绍了为心理科学家开发的两种开源算法--MinBlur 和 MinBlurLite--以提高隐私性,同时保持开放数据的完整性,(e)强调了需要进一步关注的道德数据共享方面。归根结底,重新识别的风险不应该阻止人们参与开放科学实践。相反,应开发和利用技术创新,使科学尽可能开放,以促进透明度和共享,并在必要时封闭,以维护隐私和安全。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Responsible data sharing: Identifying and remedying possible re-identification of human participants.","authors":"Kirsten N Morehouse, Benedek Kurdi, Brian A Nosek","doi":"10.1037/amp0001346","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001346","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Open data collected from research participants creates a tension between scholarly values of transparency and sharing, on the one hand, and privacy and security, on the other hand. A common solution is to make data sets anonymous by removing personally identifying information (e.g., names or worker IDs) before sharing. However, ostensibly anonymized data sets may be at risk of <i>re-identification</i> if they include demographic information. In the present article, we provide researchers with broadly applicable guidance and tangible tools so that they can engage in open science practices without jeopardizing participants' privacy. Specifically, we (a) review current privacy standards, (b) describe computer science data protection frameworks and their adaptability to the social sciences, (c) provide practical guidance for assessing and addressing re-identification risk, (d) introduce two open-source algorithms developed for psychological scientists-MinBlur and MinBlurLite-to increase privacy while maintaining the integrity of open data, and (e) highlight aspects of ethical data sharing that require further attention. Ultimately, the risk of re-identification should not dissuade engagement with open science practices. Instead, technical innovations should be developed and harnessed so that science can be as open as possible to promote transparency and sharing and as closed as necessary to maintain privacy and security. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":16.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140870696","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Science of psychological phenomena and their testing. 心理现象及其测试科学。
IF 16.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-06 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001362
Seppo E Iso-Ahola

There is no crisis of replication and generalizability in psychological science, only misunderstanding or forgetting the fundamental nature of psychological phenomena and resultant implications for empirical testing. Stability-variability is the central feature of every psychological phenomenon, meaning that brain-mind interactions can only create stable patterns from which there will always be deviations. Psychological phenomena are not comparable to COVID-19 vaccines that were very effective (95%) initially for almost everyone for a long time. Replications cannot be the gatekeepers of scientific psychological knowledge, only constructive additions and explorations contributing to theory development and measurement improvement. Once a logically justified and theoretically well-developed hypothesis is presented, the phenomenon exists as long as one of the following conditions is true: (1) it has not been shown logically that the phenomenon cannot exist or (2) it has not been shown empirically that the phenomenon does not exist. Like in physics and other sciences, generalization to theory is critical in psychological science, but less important relative to hypothetical (phantom) populations. Initial COVID-19 vaccines were effective because they worked for the right theoretical reason, the mRNA mechanism. This central principle holds true for psychological phenomena as well, with findings generalizing to the theoretical explanation regarding the presence and manifestations of behaviors brought about by the brain-mind interactions, or stated differently, generalization of psychological phenomena to specific behaviors and under specific conditions as proposed by the theory. Instead of the narrow focus on generalization to hypothetical populations, psychological phenomena and associated generalization could more productively be examined from the nine proposed perspectives. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

心理科学不存在可复制性和可推广性的危机,只有误解或遗忘心理现象的基本性质及其对实证检验的影响。稳定性-可变性是每一种心理现象的核心特征,这意味着大脑与心灵的相互作用只能创造出稳定的模式,而这种模式总会出现偏差。心理现象不能与 COVID-19 疫苗相提并论,后者在最初很长一段时间内对几乎所有人都非常有效(95%)。复制不能成为科学心理学知识的守门人,只能是建设性的补充和探索,有助于理论发展和测量改进。一旦提出一个逻辑上合理、理论上完善的假设,只要以下条件之一成立,该现象就存在:(1)逻辑上没有证明该现象不可能存在,或(2)经验上没有证明该现象不存在。与物理学和其他科学一样,理论概括在心理科学中也至关重要,但相对于假设(幽灵)人群来说,就不那么重要了。最初的 COVID-19 疫苗之所以有效,是因为它们有正确的理论依据,即 mRNA 机制。这一核心原则同样适用于心理现象,研究结果可归纳为理论解释中关于脑-心相互作用所带来的行为的存在和表现,或者换一种说法,将心理现象归纳为理论所提出的特定行为和特定条件。与其狭隘地关注对假设人群的概括,不如从提出的九个角度来研究心理现象和相关的概括,这样会更有成效。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Science of psychological phenomena and their testing.","authors":"Seppo E Iso-Ahola","doi":"10.1037/amp0001362","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001362","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>There is no crisis of replication and generalizability in psychological science, only misunderstanding or forgetting the fundamental nature of psychological phenomena and resultant implications for empirical testing. Stability-variability is the central feature of every psychological phenomenon, meaning that brain-mind interactions can only create stable <i>patterns</i> from which there will always be deviations. Psychological phenomena are not comparable to COVID-19 vaccines that were very effective (95%) initially for almost everyone for a long time. Replications cannot be the gatekeepers of scientific psychological knowledge, only constructive additions and explorations contributing to theory development and measurement improvement. Once a logically justified and theoretically well-developed hypothesis is presented, the phenomenon exists as long as one of the following conditions is true: (1) it has not been shown logically that the phenomenon <i>cannot</i> exist or (2) it has not been shown empirically that the phenomenon <i>does not</i> exist. Like in physics and other sciences, generalization to theory is critical in psychological science, but less important relative to hypothetical (phantom) populations. Initial COVID-19 vaccines were effective because they worked for the right theoretical reason, the mRNA mechanism. This central principle holds true for psychological phenomena as well, with findings generalizing to the theoretical explanation regarding the presence and manifestations of behaviors brought about by the brain-mind interactions, or stated differently, <i>generalization of psychological phenomena to specific behaviors and under specific conditions</i> as proposed by the theory. Instead of the narrow focus on generalization to hypothetical populations, psychological phenomena and associated generalization could more productively be examined from the nine proposed perspectives. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":16.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140860999","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of social-evaluative threat for cortisol profiles in response to psychosocial stress: A person-centered approach. 社会评价威胁在应对社会心理压力时对皮质醇特征的作用:以人为本的方法。
IF 16.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-06 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001335
Peggy M Zoccola, Andrew Manigault, Gabrielle Decastro, Courtney Taylor, Sally S Dickerson

Heterogeneity in individuals' physiological stress responses is central to theories linking stress with vulnerability to disease. Although multiple cortisol profiles have been reported in response to acute psychological stress, most prior work focuses on a single, average pattern and relative deviations from it, such as greater or lesser response peaks or reactivity. The present aims were to identify cortisol stress response trajectory classes using a data-driven approach and test whether social-evaluative threat (SET), a reliable elicitor of cortisol, predicted a greater likelihood of membership in the more reactive profiles. Data were pooled from 13 acute laboratory stressor studies from two geographically distinct U.S. university communities. Participants included 1,258 adults ranging from 18 to 52 years (Mage = 20.5; 62% women; 38% men) with diverse racial/ethnic identities and socioeconomic statuses. Studies included a version of the Trier Social Stress Test and at least three salivary cortisol assessments. SET was tested in three ways: study conditions with evaluators present, perceptions of evaluation, and ratings of shame-related emotions. Latent group-based trajectory modeling was applied to identify cortisol response patterns that best fit the data. Results revealed five unique cortisol response profiles. Consistent with hypotheses, SET conditions, greater perceived evaluation, and greater shame-related emotions predicted membership in the most reactive response trajectories. The findings highlight the high degree of heterogeneity that characterizes cortisol stress response profiles, which has important implications for theories of stress and health and methodological approaches in future research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

个体生理压力反应的异质性是压力与疾病易感性相关理论的核心。虽然有报告称急性心理应激反应中存在多种皮质醇特征,但之前的大多数研究都集中在单一的平均模式以及与之相对的偏差上,例如反应峰值或反应性的高低。本研究的目的是利用数据驱动法确定皮质醇应激反应轨迹类别,并测试社会评价威胁(SET)作为皮质醇的可靠诱导因子是否能预测出更有可能属于反应性更强的类别。研究数据来自两个地理位置不同的美国大学社区的 13 项急性实验室压力源研究。参与者包括 1,258 名成年人,年龄从 18 岁到 52 岁不等(Mage = 20.5;62% 为女性;38% 为男性),具有不同的种族/民族身份和社会经济地位。研究包括一个版本的特里尔社会压力测试和至少三次唾液皮质醇评估。社会压力测试从三个方面进行测试:有评估者在场的研究条件、对评估的感知以及对羞耻相关情绪的评级。应用基于潜伏组的轨迹模型来确定最适合数据的皮质醇反应模式。结果发现了五种独特的皮质醇反应模式。与假设一致的是,SET 条件、更高的感知评价和更高的羞耻相关情绪预示着最反应性反应轨迹中的成员。研究结果凸显了皮质醇压力反应特征的高度异质性,这对压力和健康理论以及未来研究的方法论具有重要意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The role of social-evaluative threat for cortisol profiles in response to psychosocial stress: A person-centered approach.","authors":"Peggy M Zoccola, Andrew Manigault, Gabrielle Decastro, Courtney Taylor, Sally S Dickerson","doi":"10.1037/amp0001335","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001335","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Heterogeneity in individuals' physiological stress responses is central to theories linking stress with vulnerability to disease. Although multiple cortisol profiles have been reported in response to acute psychological stress, most prior work focuses on a single, average pattern and relative deviations from it, such as greater or lesser response peaks or reactivity. The present aims were to identify cortisol stress response trajectory classes using a data-driven approach and test whether social-evaluative threat (SET), a reliable elicitor of cortisol, predicted a greater likelihood of membership in the more reactive profiles. Data were pooled from 13 acute laboratory stressor studies from two geographically distinct U.S. university communities. Participants included 1,258 adults ranging from 18 to 52 years (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 20.5; 62% women; 38% men) with diverse racial/ethnic identities and socioeconomic statuses. Studies included a version of the Trier Social Stress Test and at least three salivary cortisol assessments. SET was tested in three ways: study conditions with evaluators present, perceptions of evaluation, and ratings of shame-related emotions. Latent group-based trajectory modeling was applied to identify cortisol response patterns that best fit the data. Results revealed five unique cortisol response profiles. Consistent with hypotheses, SET conditions, greater perceived evaluation, and greater shame-related emotions predicted membership in the most reactive response trajectories. The findings highlight the high degree of heterogeneity that characterizes cortisol stress response profiles, which has important implications for theories of stress and health and methodological approaches in future research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":16.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140858922","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
American Psychologist
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1