首页 > 最新文献

American Psychologist最新文献

英文 中文
Professional practice guidelines for operational psychology: An executive summary.
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-02-27 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001499
Mark A Staal, David M Corey, Paul J Dean, David DeMatteo, Daniel A Krauss, Larry K Lewis, Christopher A Myers, Daniel J Neller, James A Stephenson, Philip S Trompetter, Jeffrey N Younggren

Operational psychologists provide a variety of psychological services in support of national security, national defense, and public safety. Their work may include the assessment of personnel for high-risk positions, consultation to investigations and crisis negotiations, support to military or intelligence training and operations, or other types of psychological and behavioral assessments. The practice of operational psychology differs in important ways from other practice areas and has developed significantly over the past 20 years. Given developments in the field, debate about the proper roles of psychologists in national security settings, and psychologists' ongoing need for guidance, these Professional Practice Guidelines for Operational Psychology are provided to benefit operational psychologists, the recipients of their services, and other affected parties. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

作战心理学家为支持国家安全、国防和公共安全提供各种心理服务。他们的工作可能包括对高风险岗位人员进行评估、为调查和危机谈判提供咨询、为军事或情报培训和行动提供支持,或进行其他类型的心理和行为评估。行动心理学的实践与其他实践领域有着重要的不同,在过去 20 年中得到了长足的发展。鉴于该领域的发展、有关心理学家在国家安全环境中的适当角色的争论,以及心理学家对指导的持续需求,本《作战心理学专业实践指南》的提供将使作战心理学家、其服务的接受者以及其他受影响的各方受益。(PsycInfo 数据库记录 (c) 2025 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Professional practice guidelines for operational psychology: An executive summary.","authors":"Mark A Staal, David M Corey, Paul J Dean, David DeMatteo, Daniel A Krauss, Larry K Lewis, Christopher A Myers, Daniel J Neller, James A Stephenson, Philip S Trompetter, Jeffrey N Younggren","doi":"10.1037/amp0001499","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001499","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Operational psychologists provide a variety of psychological services in support of national security, national defense, and public safety. Their work may include the assessment of personnel for high-risk positions, consultation to investigations and crisis negotiations, support to military or intelligence training and operations, or other types of psychological and behavioral assessments. The practice of operational psychology differs in important ways from other practice areas and has developed significantly over the past 20 years. Given developments in the field, debate about the proper roles of psychologists in national security settings, and psychologists' ongoing need for guidance, these <i>Professional Practice Guidelines for Operational Psychology</i> are provided to benefit operational psychologists, the recipients of their services, and other affected parties. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143524832","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sudhir Kakar (1938-2024).
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-02-27 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001498
Dinesh Sharma, John Munder Ross

Sudhir Kakar, the father of Indian psychoanalysis, died on April 22, 2024, at the age of 85 years. Born in Nainital in 1938, he grew up in Sargodha (now in Pakistan), Rohtak, and Delhi. Kakar belonged to an upper caste Hindu family, well connected to the industrialist elites in India. Kakar became a psychoanalyst after meeting his guru Erik Erikson. After Girindrasekhar Bose, Kakar was one of the most creative psychoanalysts of modern India. He wrote and edited 25 books of nonfiction and seven novels. He is survived by his son, daughter, and his partner. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

{"title":"Sudhir Kakar (1938-2024).","authors":"Dinesh Sharma, John Munder Ross","doi":"10.1037/amp0001498","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001498","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Sudhir Kakar, the father of Indian psychoanalysis, died on April 22, 2024, at the age of 85 years. Born in Nainital in 1938, he grew up in Sargodha (now in Pakistan), Rohtak, and Delhi. Kakar belonged to an upper caste Hindu family, well connected to the industrialist elites in India. Kakar became a psychoanalyst after meeting his guru Erik Erikson. After Girindrasekhar Bose, Kakar was one of the most creative psychoanalysts of modern India. He wrote and edited 25 books of nonfiction and seven novels. He is survived by his son, daughter, and his partner. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143524835","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Veterans health administration leads the way in population mental health science: Commentary on Dodge et al. (2024). 退伍军人健康管理局引领人口心理健康科学发展:对 Dodge 等人(2024 年)的评论。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-12-12 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001428
Brian P Marx, Denise M Sloan, Terence M Keane, Stacey Pollack, Paula P Schnurr

Recently, Dodge et al. (2024) published an article in American Psychologist offering recommendations to the mental health field for changing from an individual-level to a population-level focus. These recommendations included scaling up evidence-based programs, innovating and evaluating population-level interventions, and creating a primary system of care to promote mental health and well-being. For the past 2 decades, the Veterans Health Administration has been successfully engaged in these activities. In this commentary, we describe some of these ongoing efforts to demonstrate that Dodge et al.'s (2024) recommendations are indeed feasible with the proper infrastructure and resources and that the Veterans Health Administration's efforts can serve as a model for the field. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

最近,Dodge等人(2024)在《美国心理学家》上发表了一篇文章,为心理健康领域提供了从个人层面转向人群层面的建议。这些建议包括扩大以证据为基础的规划,创新和评估人口水平的干预措施,以及建立初级保健系统以促进精神健康和福祉。在过去的20年里,退伍军人健康管理局成功地参与了这些活动。在这篇评论中,我们描述了一些正在进行的努力,以证明Dodge等人(2024)的建议在适当的基础设施和资源下确实是可行的,退伍军人健康管理局的努力可以作为该领域的典范。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Veterans health administration leads the way in population mental health science: Commentary on Dodge et al. (2024).","authors":"Brian P Marx, Denise M Sloan, Terence M Keane, Stacey Pollack, Paula P Schnurr","doi":"10.1037/amp0001428","DOIUrl":"10.1037/amp0001428","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recently, Dodge et al. (2024) published an article in <i>American Psychologist</i> offering recommendations to the mental health field for changing from an individual-level to a population-level focus. These recommendations included scaling up evidence-based programs, innovating and evaluating population-level interventions, and creating a primary system of care to promote mental health and well-being. For the past 2 decades, the Veterans Health Administration has been successfully engaged in these activities. In this commentary, we describe some of these ongoing efforts to demonstrate that Dodge et al.'s (2024) recommendations are indeed feasible with the proper infrastructure and resources and that the Veterans Health Administration's efforts can serve as a model for the field. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":" ","pages":"279-281"},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142819381","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychologists return to the first question of Western philosophy. 心理学家回到了西方哲学的第一个问题。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-06 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001351
Jeremy D W Clifton

When ancient humans gained the ability to investigate abstract questions, what first question did they pose? This article offers a novel, sweeping, historical analysis with important implications for psychological theory. The story begins with identifying the first question in Ancient Greek philosophy as "Where am I?" with particular interest in the world's overarching basic traits. For example, Pythagoras proposed the world was defined by beauty and Heraclitus suggested change. Though this discourse has traditionally puzzled historians, recent psychological research suggests it might have been largely a debate over primal world beliefs, an emerging research topic that this article introduces and situates historically. Recently, the latent structure of primal world beliefs was mapped statistically, revealing 26 dimensions. Most of these beliefs were new to psychologists, yet already posed by ancient philosophers-including Pythagoras' Beautiful world belief and Heraclitus' Changing world belief. Identifying first questions in early history may have value for psychological theorizing because it hints at something that social psychologists have long suspected: that humans are creatures fundamentally driven to understand their situation and what it calls for. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

当古人类获得研究抽象问题的能力时,他们提出的第一个问题是什么?本文提供了一个新颖、全面的历史分析,对心理学理论具有重要意义。文章首先指出古希腊哲学的第一个问题是 "我在哪里?",并特别关注世界的总体基本特征。例如,毕达哥拉斯提出世界的定义是美,赫拉克利特提出变化。虽然这种讨论历来令历史学家困惑,但最近的心理学研究表明,它可能主要是一场关于原始世界信念的争论,本文介绍了这一新兴研究课题,并对其进行了历史定位。最近,有人对原始世界信念的潜在结构进行了统计,发现了 26 个维度。这些信念中的大多数对心理学家来说都是新的,但古代哲学家已经提出过--包括毕达哥拉斯的 "美丽世界信念 "和赫拉克利特的 "变化世界信念"。确定早期历史中的第一个问题可能对心理学理论研究有价值,因为它暗示了社会心理学家长期以来一直怀疑的一点:人类是一种从根本上被驱使去了解自己的处境及其要求的生物。(PsycInfo 数据库记录 (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Psychologists return to the first question of Western philosophy.","authors":"Jeremy D W Clifton","doi":"10.1037/amp0001351","DOIUrl":"10.1037/amp0001351","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When ancient humans gained the ability to investigate abstract questions, what first question did they pose? This article offers a novel, sweeping, historical analysis with important implications for psychological theory. The story begins with identifying the first question in Ancient Greek philosophy as \"Where am I?\" with particular interest in the world's overarching basic traits. For example, Pythagoras proposed the world was defined by beauty and Heraclitus suggested change. Though this discourse has traditionally puzzled historians, recent psychological research suggests it might have been largely a debate over primal world beliefs, an emerging research topic that this article introduces and situates historically. Recently, the latent structure of primal world beliefs was mapped statistically, revealing 26 dimensions. Most of these beliefs were new to psychologists, yet already posed by ancient philosophers-including Pythagoras' <i>Beautiful</i> world belief and Heraclitus' <i>Changing</i> world belief. Identifying first questions in early history may have value for psychological theorizing because it hints at something that social psychologists have long suspected: that humans are creatures fundamentally driven to understand their situation and what it calls for. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":" ","pages":"232-246"},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140868298","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Approaching psychology's current crises by exploring the vagueness of psychological concepts: Recommendations for advancing the discipline. 通过探讨心理学概念的模糊性来应对心理学当前的危机:推动学科发展的建议。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-25 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001300
Fabian Hutmacher, David J Franz

Psychology is currently facing a multilayered crisis stemming from the fact that the results of many psychological studies cannot be replicated (replication crisis), that psychological research has neglected cross-cultural and cross-temporal variation (universality crisis), and that many psychological theories are ill-developed and underspecified (theory crisis). In the present article, we use ideas derived from debates in theoretical and philosophical psychology as a basis for responding to all three crises. In short, we claim that psychological concepts are inherently vague in the sense that their meanings and the rules for their application are indeterminate. This does not imply that psychological concepts are ineffable or lack meaning. It implies, however, that hoping to arrive at a finite set of necessary and sufficient criteria that define psychological concepts once and for all is an illusion. From this, we deduce four recommendations for responding to psychology's crises. First, we argue that the replication crisis could be approached by paying more attention to the context conditions under which psychological realities and knowledge about these realities are being created. Second, we claim that the universality crisis can be alleviated by putting more effort into exploring variability across times and cultures. Third, we contend that acknowledging the language dependence of psychological research could be a fruitful way of addressing the theory crisis. Last, we show that embracing theoretical and methodological pluralism would be an antidote against psychology's crises in general. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

心理学目前正面临着多层次的危机,其根源在于许多心理学研究的结果无法复制(复制危机),心理学研究忽视了跨文化和跨时空的差异(普遍性危机),以及许多心理学理论发展不完善和不够具体(理论危机)。在本文中,我们以理论心理学和哲学心理学辩论中的观点为基础,对这三种危机做出回应。简而言之,我们认为心理学概念本身是模糊的,因为它们的含义和应用规则是不确定的。这并不意味着心理学概念不可言传或缺乏意义。然而,这意味着希望得出一套有限的必要和充分标准来一劳永逸地定义心理学概念是一种幻想。由此,我们推导出四条应对心理学危机的建议。首先,我们认为可以通过更多地关注心理学现实和关于这些现实的知识是在怎样的背景条件下产生的,来应对复制危机。其次,我们认为可以通过加大力度探索不同时代和不同文化之间的差异性来缓解普遍性危机。第三,我们认为,承认心理学研究的语言依赖性是解决理论危机的有效途径。最后,我们表明,接受理论和方法的多元化将是解决心理学总体危机的一剂良药。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Approaching psychology's current crises by exploring the vagueness of psychological concepts: Recommendations for advancing the discipline.","authors":"Fabian Hutmacher, David J Franz","doi":"10.1037/amp0001300","DOIUrl":"10.1037/amp0001300","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Psychology is currently facing a multilayered crisis stemming from the fact that the results of many psychological studies cannot be replicated (replication crisis), that psychological research has neglected cross-cultural and cross-temporal variation (universality crisis), and that many psychological theories are ill-developed and underspecified (theory crisis). In the present article, we use ideas derived from debates in theoretical and philosophical psychology as a basis for responding to all three crises. In short, we claim that psychological concepts are inherently vague in the sense that their meanings and the rules for their application are indeterminate. This does not imply that psychological concepts are ineffable or lack meaning. It implies, however, that hoping to arrive at a finite set of necessary and sufficient criteria that define psychological concepts once and for all is an illusion. From this, we deduce four recommendations for responding to psychology's crises. First, we argue that the replication crisis could be approached by paying more attention to the context conditions under which psychological realities and knowledge about these realities are being created. Second, we claim that the universality crisis can be alleviated by putting more effort into exploring variability across times and cultures. Third, we contend that acknowledging the language dependence of psychological research could be a fruitful way of addressing the theory crisis. Last, we show that embracing theoretical and methodological pluralism would be an antidote against psychology's crises in general. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":" ","pages":"220-231"},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139565128","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Daniel Kahneman (1934-2024).
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-03 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001431
Barbara Tversky

This article memorializes Daniel Kahneman (1934-2024), Danny was always fascinated by the complexities and inconsistencies of human behavior, beliefs, values, and tastes, if also by the grand problems of philosophy. His pioneering work led to his influential theory of attention as a limited resource that could be allocated to various tasks presented in his first book, Attention and Effort (1974). Prospect theory remained his most cited work by far and was the central work cited in his Nobel Prize award in 2002. Danny's Nobel Prize Lecture, carried the seeds of his highly influential book, Thinking Fast and Slow (2011). His research influenced policy and program development in economics, philosophy, psychology, and artificial intelligence. Highlights of Kahneman's career and professional contributions are noted. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

{"title":"Daniel Kahneman (1934-2024).","authors":"Barbara Tversky","doi":"10.1037/amp0001431","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001431","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This article memorializes Daniel Kahneman (1934-2024), Danny was always fascinated by the complexities and inconsistencies of human behavior, beliefs, values, and tastes, if also by the grand problems of philosophy. His pioneering work led to his influential theory of attention as a limited resource that could be allocated to various tasks presented in his first book, <i>Attention and Effort</i> (1974). Prospect theory remained his most cited work by far and was the central work cited in his Nobel Prize award in 2002. Danny's Nobel Prize Lecture, carried the seeds of his highly influential book, <i>Thinking Fast and Slow</i> (2011). His research influenced policy and program development in economics, philosophy, psychology, and artificial intelligence. Highlights of Kahneman's career and professional contributions are noted. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":"80 2","pages":"285-286"},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143442398","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Jean Maria Arrigo (1944-2024).
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-03 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001402
Roy Eidelson

This article memorializes Jean Maria Arrigo (1944-2024). Jean Maria's dedication to pursuing truth and accountability led to ethical reforms to the world's largest organization of psychologists. Despite her success in reforming APA policy, until her death, Jean Maria remained deeply concerned about the weaponization of psychology and the dangers posed by ties between professional associations and the national security establishment. Highlights of Arrigo's career and professional contributions are noted. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

{"title":"Jean Maria Arrigo (1944-2024).","authors":"Roy Eidelson","doi":"10.1037/amp0001402","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001402","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This article memorializes Jean Maria Arrigo (1944-2024). Jean Maria's dedication to pursuing truth and accountability led to ethical reforms to the world's largest organization of psychologists. Despite her success in reforming APA policy, until her death, Jean Maria remained deeply concerned about the weaponization of psychology and the dangers posed by ties between professional associations and the national security establishment. Highlights of Arrigo's career and professional contributions are noted. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":"80 2","pages":"288"},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143442401","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
WEIRD-Confucian comparisons: Ongoing cultural biases in psychology's evidence base and some recommendations for improving global representation. WEIRD-Confucian comparisons:心理学证据基础中持续存在的文化偏见以及改善全球代表性的一些建议。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-25 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001298
Kuba Krys, Igor de Almeida, Arkadiusz Wasiel, Vivian L Vignoles

The realization that most behavioral science research focuses on cultures labeled as WEIRD-Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic (Arnett, 2008; Henrich et al., 2010; Thalmayer et al., 2021)-has given an impetus to extend the research to more diverse populations. Confucian East Asian societies have relatively strong social and technological infrastructure to advance science and thus have gained much prominence in cross-cultural studies. This has inadvertently fostered another bias: the dominance of WEIRD-Confucian comparisons and a tendency to draw conclusions about "non-WEIRD" cultures in general based on data from Confucian societies. Here, analyzing 1,466,019 scientific abstracts and, separately, coverage of 60 large-scale cross-cultural psychological projects (Nsamples = 2,668 from Ncountries = 153 covering nparticipants = 3,722,940), we quantify the dominance of Confucian over other non-WEIRD cultures in psychological research. Our analysis also reveals the underrepresentation of non-European Union postcommunist societies and the almost total invisibility of Pacific Island, Caribbean, Middle African, and Central Asian societies within the research database of psychology. We call for a shift in cross-cultural studies toward midsize (7+ countries) and ideally large-scale (50+ countries) cross-cultural studies, and we propose mitigations that we believe could aid the inclusion of diverse researchers as well as participants from underrepresented cultures in our field. People in all world regions and cultures deserve psychological knowledge that applies to them. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

大多数行为科学研究都集中在被称为WEIRD--西方的、受过教育的、工业化的、富裕的和民主的文化(Arnett, 2008; Henrich et al.东亚儒家社会拥有相对强大的社会和技术基础设施来推动科学的发展,因此在跨文化研究中占据了重要地位。这无意中助长了另一种偏见:以 "伟人 "与 "儒家 "的比较为主导,并倾向于根据儒家社会的数据得出有关 "非伟人 "文化的一般性结论。在此,我们分析了1,466,019份科学摘要,并分别分析了60个大型跨文化心理学项目的覆盖范围(Nsamples = 2,668 from Ncountries = 153 covering nparticipants = 3,722,940),量化了心理学研究中儒家文化相对于其他非WEIRD文化的主导地位。我们的分析还揭示了非欧盟后共产主义社会的代表性不足,以及太平洋岛屿、加勒比海、中非和中亚社会在心理学研究数据库中几乎完全被忽视。我们呼吁将跨文化研究转向中型(7 个以上国家),最好是大型(50 个以上国家)跨文化研究,我们还提出了一些缓解措施,我们认为这些措施有助于将来自代表性不足的文化的不同研究人员和参与者纳入我们的研究领域。世界各地和各种文化背景的人们都应该获得适用于他们的心理学知识。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"WEIRD-Confucian comparisons: Ongoing cultural biases in psychology's evidence base and some recommendations for improving global representation.","authors":"Kuba Krys, Igor de Almeida, Arkadiusz Wasiel, Vivian L Vignoles","doi":"10.1037/amp0001298","DOIUrl":"10.1037/amp0001298","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The realization that most behavioral science research focuses on cultures labeled as WEIRD-Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic (Arnett, 2008; Henrich et al., 2010; Thalmayer et al., 2021)-has given an impetus to extend the research to more diverse populations. Confucian East Asian societies have relatively strong social and technological infrastructure to advance science and thus have gained much prominence in cross-cultural studies. This has inadvertently fostered another bias: the dominance of WEIRD-Confucian comparisons and a tendency to draw conclusions about \"non-WEIRD\" cultures in general based on data from Confucian societies. Here, analyzing 1,466,019 scientific abstracts and, separately, coverage of 60 large-scale cross-cultural psychological projects (<i>N</i><sub>samples</sub> = 2,668 from <i>N</i><sub>countries</sub> = 153 covering <i>n</i><sub>participants</sub> = 3,722,940), we quantify the dominance of Confucian over other non-WEIRD cultures in psychological research. Our analysis also reveals the underrepresentation of non-European Union postcommunist societies and the almost total invisibility of Pacific Island, Caribbean, Middle African, and Central Asian societies within the research database of psychology. We call for a shift in cross-cultural studies toward midsize (7+ countries) and ideally large-scale (50+ countries) cross-cultural studies, and we propose mitigations that we believe could aid the inclusion of diverse researchers as well as participants from underrepresented cultures in our field. People in all world regions and cultures deserve psychological knowledge that applies to them. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":" ","pages":"247-263"},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139565133","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neural specialization with generalizable representations underlies children's cognitive development of attention. 具有可概括表征的神经特化是儿童注意力认知发展的基础。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001283
Lei Hao, Siya Peng, Ying Zhou, Xu Chen, Jiang Qiu, Wenbo Luo, Liping Zhuang, Jiahua Xu, Yanpei Wang, Haowen Su, Haoran Guan, Jing Luo, Shuping Tan, Jia-Hong Gao, Yong He, Tanya M Evans, Jin Fan, Sha Tao, Qi Dong, Shaozheng Qin

From childhood to adulthood, the human brain develops highly specialized yet interacting neural modules that give rise to nuanced attention and other cognitive functions. Each module can specialize over development to support specific functions, yet also coexist in multiple neurobiological modes to support distinct processes. Advances in cognitive neuroscience have conceptualized human attention as a set of cognitive processes anchored in highly specialized yet interacting neural systems. The underlying mechanisms of how these systems interplay to support children's cognitive development of multiple attention processes remain unknown. Leveraging developmental functional magnetic resonance imaging with attention network test paradigm, we demonstrate differential neurocognitive development of three core attentional processes from childhood to adulthood, with alerting reaching adult-like level earlier, followed by orienting and executive attention with more protracted development throughout middle and late childhood. Relative to adults, young children exhibit immature specialization with less pronounced dissociation of neural systems specific to each attentional process. Children manifest adult-like distributed representations in the ventral attention and cingulo-opercular networks, but less stable and weaker generalizable representations across multiple processes in the dorsal attention network. Our findings provide insights into the functional specialization and generalization of neural representations scaffolding cognitive development of core attentional processes from childhood to adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

从童年到成年,人类大脑发展出高度专业化但又相互作用的神经模块,从而产生了细致入微的注意力和其他认知功能。每个模块在发育过程中都会专门化,以支持特定的功能,但也会以多种神经生物学模式共存,以支持不同的过程。认知神经科学的进步将人类的注意力概念化为一系列认知过程,这些认知过程以高度特化但又相互作用的神经系统为基础。这些系统如何相互作用以支持儿童多种注意过程的认知发展,其基本机制仍然未知。利用发育功能磁共振成像和注意力网络测试范式,我们证明了从童年到成年三个核心注意力过程的不同神经认知发展,其中警觉较早达到成人水平,随后是定向和执行注意力,在童年中期和晚期发展更为持久。与成人相比,幼儿表现出不成熟的专业化,每个注意过程所特有的神经系统的分离不那么明显。儿童在腹侧注意网络和丘脑-小脑网络中表现出类似成人的分布式表征,但在背侧注意网络中,儿童在多个过程中的表征不太稳定,通用性也较弱。我们的研究结果为神经表征的功能特化和泛化提供了见解,这些神经表征为儿童期到成年期核心注意过程的认知发展提供了支架。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Neural specialization with generalizable representations underlies children's cognitive development of attention.","authors":"Lei Hao, Siya Peng, Ying Zhou, Xu Chen, Jiang Qiu, Wenbo Luo, Liping Zhuang, Jiahua Xu, Yanpei Wang, Haowen Su, Haoran Guan, Jing Luo, Shuping Tan, Jia-Hong Gao, Yong He, Tanya M Evans, Jin Fan, Sha Tao, Qi Dong, Shaozheng Qin","doi":"10.1037/amp0001283","DOIUrl":"10.1037/amp0001283","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>From childhood to adulthood, the human brain develops highly specialized yet interacting neural modules that give rise to nuanced attention and other cognitive functions. Each module can specialize over development to support specific functions, yet also coexist in multiple neurobiological modes to support distinct processes. Advances in cognitive neuroscience have conceptualized human attention as a set of cognitive processes anchored in highly specialized yet interacting neural systems. The underlying mechanisms of how these systems interplay to support children's cognitive development of multiple attention processes remain unknown. Leveraging developmental functional magnetic resonance imaging with attention network test paradigm, we demonstrate differential neurocognitive development of three core attentional processes from childhood to adulthood, with alerting reaching adult-like level earlier, followed by orienting and executive attention with more protracted development throughout middle and late childhood. Relative to adults, young children exhibit immature specialization with less pronounced dissociation of neural systems specific to each attentional process. Children manifest adult-like distributed representations in the ventral attention and cingulo-opercular networks, but less stable and weaker generalizable representations across multiple processes in the dorsal attention network. Our findings provide insights into the functional specialization and generalization of neural representations scaffolding cognitive development of core attentional processes from childhood to adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":" ","pages":"148-164"},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139651907","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Move past adversity or bite through it? Diet quality, physical activity, and sedentary behavior in relation to resilience. 走出逆境还是咬牙挺过?饮食质量、体育锻炼和久坐行为与复原力的关系。
IF 12.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-07 DOI: 10.1037/amp0001423
Alea Ruf, Kira F Ahrens, Judith R Gruber, Rebecca J Neumann, Bianca Kollmann, Raffael Kalisch, Klaus Lieb, Oliver Tüscher, Michael M Plichta, Ute Nöthlings, Ulrich Ebner-Priemer, Andreas Reif, Silke Matura

Adverse life experiences are associated with an increased risk of mental disorders. The successful adaptation to adversity and maintenance or quick restoration of mental health despite adversity is referred to as resilience. Identifying factors that promote resilience can contribute to the prevention of mental disorders. Lifestyle behaviors, increasingly recognized for their impact on mental health, are discussed as potential resilience factors. Several studies found that healthy eating and physical activity (PA) are positively associated with resilience. However, most of these studies assessed resilience through questionnaires, which is unsatisfactory given that resilience research is moving toward conceptualizing resilience as the outcome of a dynamic process, which can only be assessed prospectively and longitudinally. The present study is the first to assess the relationship between diet quality, PA, sedentary behavior (SB), and resilience, captured prospectively and longitudinally in a sample of 145 individuals (75.17% female; Mage = 28.88, SDage = 7.80; MBMI = 24.11, SDBMI = 3.97). Resilience was assessed as the relationship between stressor exposure and mental health (i.e., the stressor reactivity score: higher scores indicate lower resilience and vice versa). Diet quality (i.e., the Healthy Eating Index) was assessed on the basis of app-based food records and 24-hr dietary recalls. PA and SB were objectively recorded through accelerometers. Regression analysis showed that neither diet quality nor PA and SB predicted resilience (ps > .30). Profound differences in the conceptualization and operationalization of resilience might explain the contrary findings. Prospective longitudinal studies are needed to replicate the findings of the present study. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

不利的生活经历与精神失常风险的增加有关。成功地适应逆境、在逆境中保持或迅速恢复心理健康被称为抗逆力。找出促进恢复力的因素有助于预防精神障碍。生活方式行为对心理健康的影响日益得到认可,被视为潜在的抗逆力因素。一些研究发现,健康饮食和体育锻炼(PA)与抗逆力呈正相关。然而,这些研究大多通过问卷调查来评估抗逆力,这并不令人满意,因为抗逆力研究正朝着将抗逆力概念化的方向发展,认为抗逆力是一个动态过程的结果,只能对其进行前瞻性和纵向评估。本研究首次对饮食质量、PA、久坐行为(SB)和复原力之间的关系进行了前瞻性和纵向评估,样本为 145 人(75.17% 为女性;Mage = 28.88,SDage = 7.80;MBMI = 24.11,SDBMI = 3.97)。复原力被评估为压力暴露与心理健康之间的关系(即压力反应性得分:得分越高,复原力越低,反之亦然)。饮食质量(即健康饮食指数)根据基于应用程序的食物记录和 24 小时饮食回忆进行评估。PA和SB则通过加速度计进行客观记录。回归分析表明,无论是饮食质量还是 PA 和 SB 都不能预测复原力(ps > .30)。复原力的概念化和操作化方面的巨大差异可能解释了相反的研究结果。需要进行前瞻性纵向研究,以复制本研究的结果。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Move past adversity or bite through it? Diet quality, physical activity, and sedentary behavior in relation to resilience.","authors":"Alea Ruf, Kira F Ahrens, Judith R Gruber, Rebecca J Neumann, Bianca Kollmann, Raffael Kalisch, Klaus Lieb, Oliver Tüscher, Michael M Plichta, Ute Nöthlings, Ulrich Ebner-Priemer, Andreas Reif, Silke Matura","doi":"10.1037/amp0001423","DOIUrl":"10.1037/amp0001423","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Adverse life experiences are associated with an increased risk of mental disorders. The successful adaptation to adversity and maintenance or quick restoration of mental health despite adversity is referred to as resilience. Identifying factors that promote resilience can contribute to the prevention of mental disorders. Lifestyle behaviors, increasingly recognized for their impact on mental health, are discussed as potential resilience factors. Several studies found that healthy eating and physical activity (PA) are positively associated with resilience. However, most of these studies assessed resilience through questionnaires, which is unsatisfactory given that resilience research is moving toward conceptualizing resilience as the outcome of a dynamic process, which can only be assessed prospectively and longitudinally. The present study is the first to assess the relationship between diet quality, PA, sedentary behavior (SB), and resilience, captured prospectively and longitudinally in a sample of 145 individuals (75.17% female; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 28.88, <i>SD</i><sub>age</sub> = 7.80; <i>M</i><sub>BMI</sub> = 24.11, <i>SD</i><sub>BMI</sub> = 3.97). Resilience was assessed as the relationship between stressor exposure and mental health (i.e., the stressor reactivity score: higher scores indicate lower resilience and vice versa). Diet quality (i.e., the Healthy Eating Index) was assessed on the basis of app-based food records and 24-hr dietary recalls. PA and SB were objectively recorded through accelerometers. Regression analysis showed that neither diet quality nor PA and SB predicted resilience (<i>p</i>s > .30). Profound differences in the conceptualization and operationalization of resilience might explain the contrary findings. Prospective longitudinal studies are needed to replicate the findings of the present study. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48468,"journal":{"name":"American Psychologist","volume":" ","pages":"180-192"},"PeriodicalIF":12.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142606559","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
American Psychologist
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1