首页 > 最新文献

Perception最新文献

英文 中文
Obituary: Wim van de Grind (23 April 1936-14 July 2024). 讣告维姆-范-德-格林德(1936 年 4 月 23 日-2024 年 7 月 14 日)。
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-14 DOI: 10.1177/03010066241274439
Frans Verstraten
{"title":"Obituary: Wim van de Grind (23 April 1936-14 July 2024).","authors":"Frans Verstraten","doi":"10.1177/03010066241274439","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/03010066241274439","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142057084","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The psychometrics of rating facial attractiveness using different response scales. 使用不同反应量表评定面部吸引力的心理测量学。
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-23 DOI: 10.1177/03010066241256221
Robin S S Kramer, Kay L Ritchie, Tessa R Flack, Michael O Mireku, Alex L Jones

Perceiving facial attractiveness is an important behaviour across psychological science due to these judgments having real-world consequences. However, there is little consensus on the measurement of this behaviour, and practices differ widely. Research typically asks participants to provide ratings of attractiveness across a multitude of different response scales, with little consideration of the psychometric properties of these scales. Here, we make psychometric comparisons across nine different response scales. Specifically, we analysed the psychometric properties of a binary response, a 0-100 scale, a visual analogue scale, and a set of Likert scales (1-3, 1-5, 1-7, 1-8, 1-9, 1-10) as tools to measure attractiveness, calculating a range of commonly used statistics for each. While certain properties suggested researchers might choose to favour the 1-5, 1-7 and 1-8 scales, we generally found little evidence of an advantage for one scale over any other. Taken together, our investigation provides consideration of currently used techniques for measuring facial attractiveness and makes recommendations for researchers in this field.

感知面部吸引力是心理科学中的一项重要行为,因为这些判断会对现实世界产生影响。然而,关于这种行为的测量方法却鲜有共识,而且做法也大相径庭。研究通常会要求参与者用多种不同的反应量表对吸引力进行评分,但却很少考虑这些量表的心理测量特性。在这里,我们对九种不同的反应量表进行了心理计量学比较。具体来说,我们分析了作为吸引力测量工具的二元反应量表、0-100 量表、视觉模拟量表和一组李克特量表(1-3、1-5、1-7、1-8、1-9、1-10)的心理测量特性,并计算了每种量表的一系列常用统计数据。虽然某些特性表明研究人员可能会偏爱 1-5、1-7 和 1-8 等量表,但我们总体上没有发现任何证据表明某一种量表比其他量表更有优势。总之,我们的调查对目前使用的面部吸引力测量技术进行了考虑,并为该领域的研究人员提出了建议。
{"title":"The psychometrics of rating facial attractiveness using different response scales.","authors":"Robin S S Kramer, Kay L Ritchie, Tessa R Flack, Michael O Mireku, Alex L Jones","doi":"10.1177/03010066241256221","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066241256221","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Perceiving facial attractiveness is an important behaviour across psychological science due to these judgments having real-world consequences. However, there is little consensus on the measurement of this behaviour, and practices differ widely. Research typically asks participants to provide ratings of attractiveness across a multitude of different response scales, with little consideration of the psychometric properties of these scales. Here, we make psychometric comparisons across nine different response scales. Specifically, we analysed the psychometric properties of a binary response, a 0-100 scale, a visual analogue scale, and a set of Likert scales (1-3, 1-5, 1-7, 1-8, 1-9, 1-10) as tools to measure attractiveness, calculating a range of commonly used statistics for each. While certain properties suggested researchers might choose to favour the 1-5, 1-7 and 1-8 scales, we generally found little evidence of an advantage for one scale over any other. Taken together, our investigation provides consideration of currently used techniques for measuring facial attractiveness and makes recommendations for researchers in this field.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11348630/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141080830","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No evidence for an other-race effect in dominance and trustworthy judgements from faces. 没有证据表明人脸的支配力和可信度判断存在其他种族效应。
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-17 DOI: 10.1177/03010066241258204
Ao Wang, Bartholomew P A Quinn, Hannah Gofton, Timothy J Andrews

A variety of evidence shows that social categorization of people based on their race can lead to stereotypical judgements and prejudicial behaviour. Here, we explore the extent to which trait judgements of faces are influenced by race. To address this issue, we measured the reliability of first impressions for own-race and other-race faces in Asian and White participants. Participants viewed pairs of faces and were asked to indicate which of the two faces was more dominant or which of the two faces was more trustworthy. We measured the consistency (or reliability) of these judgements across participants for own-race and other-races faces. We found that judgements of dominance or trustworthiness showed similar levels of reliability for own-race and other-race faces. Moreover, an item analysis showed that the judgements on individual trials were very similar across participants from different races. Next, participants made overall ratings of dominance and trustworthiness from own-race and other-race faces. Again, we found that there was no evidence for an ORE. Together, these results provide a new approach to measuring trait judgements of faces and show that in these conditions there is no ORE for the perception of dominance and trustworthiness.

各种证据表明,基于种族对人的社会分类会导致刻板的判断和偏见行为。在此,我们探讨了人脸特征判断受种族影响的程度。为了解决这个问题,我们测量了亚裔和白人参与者对自己种族和其他种族面孔的第一印象的可靠性。参与者观看一对面孔,并被要求指出这两张面孔中哪张更有优势或哪张更值得信赖。我们测量了不同参与者对自己种族和其他种族面孔的这些判断的一致性(或可靠性)。我们发现,对于自己种族和其他种族的面孔,优势或可信度的判断显示出相似的可靠性水平。此外,项目分析显示,不同种族的参与者在单个试验中的判断非常相似。接下来,参与者对自己种族和其他种族面孔的支配力和可信度进行了总体评分。我们再次发现,没有证据表明存在 ORE。总之,这些结果提供了一种测量人脸特质判断的新方法,并表明在这些条件下,对支配力和可信度的感知不存在 ORE。
{"title":"No evidence for an other-race effect in dominance and trustworthy judgements from faces.","authors":"Ao Wang, Bartholomew P A Quinn, Hannah Gofton, Timothy J Andrews","doi":"10.1177/03010066241258204","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066241258204","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A variety of evidence shows that social categorization of people based on their race can lead to stereotypical judgements and prejudicial behaviour. Here, we explore the extent to which trait judgements of faces are influenced by race. To address this issue, we measured the reliability of first impressions for own-race and other-race faces in Asian and White participants. Participants viewed pairs of faces and were asked to indicate which of the two faces was more dominant or which of the two faces was more trustworthy. We measured the consistency (or reliability) of these judgements across participants for own-race and other-races faces. We found that judgements of dominance or trustworthiness showed similar levels of reliability for own-race and other-race faces. Moreover, an item analysis showed that the judgements on individual trials were very similar across participants from different races. Next, participants made overall ratings of dominance and trustworthiness from own-race and other-race faces. Again, we found that there was no evidence for an ORE. Together, these results provide a new approach to measuring trait judgements of faces and show that in these conditions there is no ORE for the perception of dominance and trustworthiness.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11348627/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141332360","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Vibrotactile spatial acuity on the back. 背面的振动触觉空间敏锐度。
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-11 DOI: 10.1177/03010066241258969
Myrthe A Plaisier, Cahelle S J M Vleeshouwers, Nynke Boonstra, Yueying Shi, Sam J I van der Velden, Wouter K Vos, Astrid M L Kappers

Vibrotactile feedback can be built into clothing such as vests. This means that often vibrotactile information is presented to the back. It is known that the back has a relatively low spatial acuity. Spatial acuity varies across different limbs and sometimes with different locations on a limb. These known anisotropies suggest that there might be systematic variations in vibrotactile spatial acuity for different areas of the back and also for different orientations (i.e. horizontal vs. vertical). Here we systematically measured spatial acuity in four areas of the back for both horizontal and vertical orientations. The results show no significant differences in spatial acuity for the back areas that were tested. Spatial acuity was, however, higher in the horizontal direction than in the vertical direction by roughly a factor of two. This means that when designing vibrotactile displays for the back the tactor density can be lower in the vertical direction than in the horizontal direction and density should be constant for different areas of the back.

振动反馈可内置在背心等衣物中。这意味着振动触觉信息通常会呈现在背部。众所周知,背部的空间敏锐度相对较低。不同肢体的空间敏锐度各不相同,有时还会随肢体的不同位置而变化。这些已知的各向异性表明,背部不同区域以及不同方向(即水平与垂直)的振动触觉空间敏锐度可能存在系统性差异。在这里,我们系统地测量了背部四个区域在水平和垂直方向上的空间敏锐度。结果显示,接受测试的背部区域在空间敏锐度方面没有明显差异。不过,水平方向的空间敏锐度要比垂直方向的高出大约两倍。这意味着,在为背部设计振动触觉显示器时,垂直方向的触点密度可以低于水平方向,而且背部不同区域的触点密度应保持一致。
{"title":"Vibrotactile spatial acuity on the back.","authors":"Myrthe A Plaisier, Cahelle S J M Vleeshouwers, Nynke Boonstra, Yueying Shi, Sam J I van der Velden, Wouter K Vos, Astrid M L Kappers","doi":"10.1177/03010066241258969","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066241258969","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Vibrotactile feedback can be built into clothing such as vests. This means that often vibrotactile information is presented to the back. It is known that the back has a relatively low spatial acuity. Spatial acuity varies across different limbs and sometimes with different locations on a limb. These known anisotropies suggest that there might be systematic variations in vibrotactile spatial acuity for different areas of the back and also for different orientations (i.e. horizontal vs. vertical). Here we systematically measured spatial acuity in four areas of the back for both horizontal and vertical orientations. The results show no significant differences in spatial acuity for the back areas that were tested. Spatial acuity was, however, higher in the horizontal direction than in the vertical direction by roughly a factor of two. This means that when designing vibrotactile displays for the back the tactor density can be lower in the vertical direction than in the horizontal direction and density should be constant for different areas of the back.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11348621/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141307203","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Temporal resolution relates to sensory hyperreactivity independently of stimulus detection sensitivity in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. 时间分辨率与自闭症谱系障碍患者的感觉过度反应有关,而与刺激检测灵敏度无关。
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-12 DOI: 10.1177/03010066241259729
Ayako Kaneko, Takeshi Atsumi, Masakazu Ide

Researchers have been focusing on perceptual characteristics of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in terms of sensory hyperreactivity. Previously, we demonstrated that temporal resolution, which is the accuracy to differentiate the order of two successive vibrotactile stimuli, is associated with the severity of sensory hyperreactivity. We currently examined whether an increase in the perceptual intensity of a tactile stimulus, despite its short duration, is derived from high temporal resolution and high frequency of sensory temporal summation. Twenty ASD and 22 typically developing (TD) participants conducted two psychophysical experimental tasks to evaluate detectable duration of vibrotactile stimulus with same amplitude and to evaluate temporal resolution. The sensory hyperreactivity was estimated using self-reported questionnaire. There was no relationship between the temporal resolution and the duration of detectable stimuli in both groups. However, the ASD group showed severe sensory hyperreactivity in daily life than TD group, and the ASD participants with severe sensory hyperreactivity tended to have high temporal resolution, not high sensitivity of detectable duration. Contrary to the hypothesis, there might be different processing between temporal resolution and sensitivity for stimulus detection. We suggested that the atypical temporal processing would affect to sensory reactivity in ASD.

研究人员一直在关注自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)在感觉过度反应方面的知觉特征。在此之前,我们已经证明,时间分辨率(即区分两个连续振动触觉刺激顺序的准确性)与感觉过度反应的严重程度有关。我们现在研究的是,尽管触觉刺激持续时间很短,但触觉强度的增加是否源于高时间分辨率和高频率的感觉时间相加。20 名 ASD 和 22 名典型发育(TD)参与者进行了两项心理物理实验任务,以评估具有相同振幅的振动触觉刺激的可察觉持续时间和时间分辨率。通过自我报告问卷对感觉过度反应进行了估计。两组受试者的时间分辨率与可探测到的刺激持续时间之间没有关系。然而,与TD组相比,ASD组在日常生活中表现出严重的感觉过度反应,而且感觉过度反应严重的ASD参与者往往具有高时间分辨率,而不是对可检测到的持续时间具有高灵敏度。与假设相反,时间分辨率和刺激检测灵敏度之间可能存在不同的处理过程。我们认为,非典型的时间处理会影响 ASD 患者的感觉反应性。
{"title":"Temporal resolution relates to sensory hyperreactivity independently of stimulus detection sensitivity in individuals with autism spectrum disorder.","authors":"Ayako Kaneko, Takeshi Atsumi, Masakazu Ide","doi":"10.1177/03010066241259729","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066241259729","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Researchers have been focusing on perceptual characteristics of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in terms of sensory hyperreactivity. Previously, we demonstrated that temporal resolution, which is the accuracy to differentiate the order of two successive vibrotactile stimuli, is associated with the severity of sensory hyperreactivity. We currently examined whether an increase in the perceptual intensity of a tactile stimulus, despite its short duration, is derived from high temporal resolution and high frequency of sensory temporal summation. Twenty ASD and 22 typically developing (TD) participants conducted two psychophysical experimental tasks to evaluate <i>detectable duration</i> of vibrotactile stimulus with same amplitude and to evaluate temporal resolution. The sensory hyperreactivity was estimated using self-reported questionnaire. There was no relationship between the temporal resolution and the duration of detectable stimuli in both groups. However, the ASD group showed severe sensory hyperreactivity in daily life than TD group, and the ASD participants with severe sensory hyperreactivity tended to have high temporal resolution, not high sensitivity of detectable duration. Contrary to the hypothesis, there might be different processing between temporal resolution and sensitivity for stimulus detection. We suggested that the atypical temporal processing would affect to sensory reactivity in ASD.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141307202","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The importance of multisensory-motor learning on subsequent visual recognition. 多感官运动学习对后续视觉识别的重要性
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-20 DOI: 10.1177/03010066241258967
Hellen Kyler, Karin James

Speed of visual object recognition is facilitated after active manual exploration of objects relative to passive visual processing alone. Manual exploration allows viewers to select important information about object structure that may facilitate recognition. Viewpoints where the objects' axis of elongation is perpendicular or parallel to the line of sight are selected more during exploration, recognized faster than other viewpoints, and afford the most information about structure when object movement is controlled by the viewer. Prior work used virtual object exploration in active and passive viewing conditions, limiting multisensory structural object information. Adding multisensory information to encoding may change accuracy of overall recognition, viewpoint selection, and viewpoint recognition. We tested whether the known active advantage for object recognition would change when real objects were studied, affording visual and haptic information. Participants interacted with 3D novel objects during manual exploration or passive viewing of another's object interactions. Object recognition was tested using several viewpoints of rendered objects. We found that manually explored objects were recognized more accurately than objects studied through passive exploration and that recognition of viewpoints differed from previous work.

相对于单纯的被动视觉处理,主动手动探索物体可提高视觉物体识别的速度。手动探索可以让观众选择有关物体结构的重要信息,从而促进识别。在探索过程中,物体的伸长轴与视线垂直或平行的视点会被更多地选择,识别速度也比其他视点快,而且当物体的移动由观众控制时,这些视点能提供最多的结构信息。之前的研究使用主动和被动观看条件下的虚拟物体探索,限制了多感官结构物体信息。在编码中加入多感官信息可能会改变整体识别、视点选择和视点识别的准确性。我们测试了在研究真实物体时,提供视觉和触觉信息的已知主动物体识别优势是否会发生变化。参与者在手动探索或被动观看他人的物体交互过程中与三维新物体进行交互。使用渲染物体的多个视角对物体识别进行了测试。我们发现,手动探索的物体比被动探索的物体识别更准确,而且视角识别与之前的研究有所不同。
{"title":"The importance of multisensory-motor learning on subsequent visual recognition.","authors":"Hellen Kyler, Karin James","doi":"10.1177/03010066241258967","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066241258967","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Speed of visual object recognition is facilitated after active manual exploration of objects relative to passive visual processing alone. Manual exploration allows viewers to select important information about object structure that may facilitate recognition. Viewpoints where the objects' axis of elongation is perpendicular or parallel to the line of sight are selected more during exploration, recognized faster than other viewpoints, and afford the most information about structure when object movement is controlled by the viewer. Prior work used virtual object exploration in active and passive viewing conditions, limiting multisensory structural object information. Adding multisensory information to encoding may change accuracy of overall recognition, viewpoint selection, and viewpoint recognition. We tested whether the known active advantage for object recognition would change when real objects were studied, affording visual and haptic information. Participants interacted with 3D novel objects during manual exploration or passive viewing of another's object interactions. Object recognition was tested using several viewpoints of rendered objects. We found that manually explored objects were recognized more accurately than objects studied through passive exploration and that recognition of viewpoints differed from previous work.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141428063","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Ames room and the misunderstood versions and depictions. 埃姆斯厅以及被误解的版本和描述。
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-28 DOI: 10.1177/03010066241272260
Ekaterina Koshmanova, Maria Dvoeglazova, Vladislav Myrov, Elena Gorina, Kristina Vodorezova, Elena S Gorbunova, Tadamasa Sawada

The Ames room illusion is one of the best-known geometrical illusions but its geometrical properties are often misunderstood. This study discusses the differences in the geometrical properties between the original Ames room and what have been often referred to as "Ames rooms" in recent studies.

艾姆斯房间错觉是最著名的几何错觉之一,但其几何特性却常常被误解。本研究讨论了原始艾姆斯房间与近期研究中经常被称为 "艾姆斯房间 "的房间在几何特性上的差异。
{"title":"The Ames room and the misunderstood versions and depictions.","authors":"Ekaterina Koshmanova, Maria Dvoeglazova, Vladislav Myrov, Elena Gorina, Kristina Vodorezova, Elena S Gorbunova, Tadamasa Sawada","doi":"10.1177/03010066241272260","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/03010066241272260","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Ames room illusion is one of the best-known geometrical illusions but its geometrical properties are often misunderstood. This study discusses the differences in the geometrical properties between the original Ames room and what have been often referred to as \"Ames rooms\" in recent studies.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2024-08-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142086393","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Obituary: Wim van de Grind (23 April 1936-14 July 2024). 讣告维姆-范-德-格林德(1936 年 4 月 23 日-2024 年 7 月 14 日)。
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-14 DOI: 10.1177/03010066241274439
Frans Verstraten
{"title":"Obituary: Wim van de Grind (23 April 1936-14 July 2024).","authors":"Frans Verstraten","doi":"10.1177/03010066241274439","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/03010066241274439","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2024-08-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141977041","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Crossmodal to unimodal transfer of temporal perceptual learning. 时间感知学习的跨模态向单模态迁移
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-12 DOI: 10.1177/03010066241270271
Xing-Nan Zhao, Shu-Chen Guan, Ying-Zi Xiong, Cong Yu

Subsecond temporal processing is crucial for activities requiring precise timing. Here, we investigated perceptual learning of crossmodal (auditory-visual or visual-auditory) temporal interval discrimination (TID) and its impacts on unimodal (visual or auditory) TID performance. The research purpose was to test whether learning is based on a more abstract and conceptual representation of subsecond time, which would predict crossmodal to unimodal learning transfer. The experiments revealed that learning to discriminate a 200-ms crossmodal temporal interval, defined by a pair of visual and auditory stimuli, significantly reduced crossmodal TID thresholds. Moreover, the crossmodal TID training also minimized unimodal TID thresholds with a pair of visual or auditory stimuli at the same interval, even if crossmodal TID thresholds are multiple times higher than unimodal TID thresholds. Subsequent training on unimodal TID failed to reduce unimodal TID thresholds further. These results indicate that learning of high-threshold crossmodal TID tasks can benefit low-threshold unimodal temporal processing, which may be achieved through training-induced improvement of a conceptual representation of subsecond time in the brain.

亚秒级的时间处理对于需要精确计时的活动至关重要。在此,我们研究了跨模态(听觉-视觉或视觉-听觉)时间间隔辨别(TID)的知觉学习及其对单模态(视觉或听觉)TID表现的影响。研究目的是检验学习是否基于对亚秒级时间更抽象和概念化的表征,从而预测从跨模态到单模态的学习迁移。实验表明,学习辨别由一对视觉和听觉刺激所定义的200毫秒跨模态时间间隔,能显著降低跨模态TID阈值。此外,即使跨模态TID阈值比单模态TID阈值高出数倍,跨模态TID训练也能最大限度地降低在相同时间间隔内使用一对视觉或听觉刺激时的单模态TID阈值。随后的单模态 TID 训练未能进一步降低单模态 TID 阈值。这些结果表明,学习高阈值的跨模态 TID 任务可有利于低阈值的单模态时间处理,这可能是通过训练引起的大脑中亚秒级时间概念表征的改善实现的。
{"title":"Crossmodal to unimodal transfer of temporal perceptual learning.","authors":"Xing-Nan Zhao, Shu-Chen Guan, Ying-Zi Xiong, Cong Yu","doi":"10.1177/03010066241270271","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/03010066241270271","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Subsecond temporal processing is crucial for activities requiring precise timing. Here, we investigated perceptual learning of crossmodal (auditory-visual or visual-auditory) temporal interval discrimination (TID) and its impacts on unimodal (visual or auditory) TID performance. The research purpose was to test whether learning is based on a more abstract and conceptual representation of subsecond time, which would predict crossmodal to unimodal learning transfer. The experiments revealed that learning to discriminate a 200-ms crossmodal temporal interval, defined by a pair of visual and auditory stimuli, significantly reduced crossmodal TID thresholds. Moreover, the crossmodal TID training also minimized unimodal TID thresholds with a pair of visual or auditory stimuli at the same interval, even if crossmodal TID thresholds are multiple times higher than unimodal TID thresholds. Subsequent training on unimodal TID failed to reduce unimodal TID thresholds further. These results indicate that learning of high-threshold crossmodal TID tasks can benefit low-threshold unimodal temporal processing, which may be achieved through training-induced improvement of a conceptual representation of subsecond time in the brain.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2024-08-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141917906","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Consistent social information perceived in animated backgrounds improves ensemble perception of facial expressions. 在动画背景中感知到的一致的社会信息可改善面部表情的集合感知。
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-09 DOI: 10.1177/03010066241253073
Mengfei Zhao, Jun Wang

Observers can rapidly extract the mean emotion from a set of faces with remarkable precision, known as ensemble coding. Previous studies have demonstrated that matched physical backgrounds improve the precision of ongoing ensemble tasks. However, it remains unknown whether this facilitation effect still occurs when matched social information is perceived from the backgrounds. In two experiments, participants decided whether the test face in the retrieving phase appeared more disgusted or neutral than the mean emotion of the face set in the encoding phase. Both phases were paired with task-irrelevant animated backgrounds, which included either the forward movement trajectory carrying the "cooperatively chasing" information, or the backward movement trajectory conveying no such chasing information. The backgrounds in the encoding and retrieving phases were either mismatched (i.e., forward and backward replays of the same trajectory), or matched (i.e., two identical forward movement trajectories in Experiment 1, or two different forward movement trajectories in Experiment 2). Participants in both experiments showed higher ensemble precisions and better discrimination sensitivities when backgrounds matched. The findings suggest that consistent social information perceived from memory-related context exerts a context-matching facilitation effect on ensemble coding and, more importantly, this effect is independent of consistent physical information.

观察者可以从一组面孔中快速提取出平均情绪,而且精确度极高,这就是所谓的集合编码。以往的研究表明,匹配的物理背景能提高正在进行的集合任务的精确度。然而,当从背景中感知到匹配的社会信息时,这种促进作用是否还会发生,目前仍是未知数。在两项实验中,参与者决定检索阶段的测试人脸比编码阶段的人脸集的平均情绪更厌恶还是更中性。这两个阶段都搭配了与任务无关的动画背景,其中包括带有 "合作追逐 "信息的前向运动轨迹,或没有传递此类追逐信息的后向运动轨迹。编码和检索阶段的背景要么是不匹配的(即同一轨迹的正向和反向重放),要么是匹配的(即实验 1 中两个相同的前向运动轨迹,或实验 2 中两个不同的前向运动轨迹)。当背景匹配时,两个实验中的参与者都表现出更高的集合精确度和更好的辨别灵敏度。研究结果表明,从与记忆相关的背景中感知到的一致的社会信息对集合编码产生了背景匹配促进效应,更重要的是,这种效应与一致的物理信息无关。
{"title":"Consistent social information perceived in animated backgrounds improves ensemble perception of facial expressions.","authors":"Mengfei Zhao, Jun Wang","doi":"10.1177/03010066241253073","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066241253073","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Observers can rapidly extract the mean emotion from a set of faces with remarkable precision, known as ensemble coding. Previous studies have demonstrated that matched physical backgrounds improve the precision of ongoing ensemble tasks. However, it remains unknown whether this facilitation effect still occurs when matched social information is perceived from the backgrounds. In two experiments, participants decided whether the test face in the retrieving phase appeared more disgusted or neutral than the mean emotion of the face set in the encoding phase. Both phases were paired with task-irrelevant animated backgrounds, which included either the forward movement trajectory carrying the \"cooperatively chasing\" information, or the backward movement trajectory conveying no such chasing information. The backgrounds in the encoding and retrieving phases were either mismatched (i.e., forward and backward replays of the same trajectory), or matched (i.e., two identical forward movement trajectories in Experiment 1, or two different forward movement trajectories in Experiment 2). Participants in both experiments showed higher ensemble precisions and better discrimination sensitivities when backgrounds matched. The findings suggest that consistent social information perceived from memory-related context exerts a context-matching facilitation effect on ensemble coding and, more importantly, this effect is independent of consistent physical information.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140899070","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Perception
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1