首页 > 最新文献

Perception最新文献

英文 中文
Do masks cover more than just a face? A study on how facemasks affect the perception of emotional expressions according to their degree of intensity. 面具的作用不仅仅是遮住一张脸吗?一项关于面具如何根据强烈程度影响情绪表达感知的研究。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-14 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231201230
Pauline J N Thomas, Stéphanie Caharel

Emotional facial expressions convey crucial information in nonverbal communication and serve as a mediator in face-to-face relationships. Their recognition would rely on specific facial traits depending on the perceived emotion. During the COVID-19 pandemic, wearing a facemask has thus disrupted the human ability to read emotions from faces. Yet, these effects are usually assessed across studies from faces expressing stereotypical and exaggerated emotions, which is far removed from real-life conditions. The objective of the present study was to evaluate the impact of facemasks through an emotion categorization task using morphs ranging from a neutral face and an expressive face (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, and sadness) (from 0% neutral to 100% expressive in 20% steps). Our results revealed a strong impact of facemasks on the recognition of expressions of disgust, happiness, and sadness, resulting in a decrease in performance and an increase in misinterpretations, both for low and high levels of intensity. In contrast, the recognition of anger and fear, as well as neutral expression, was found to be less impacted by mask-wearing. Future studies should address this issue from a more ecological point of view with the aim of taking concrete adaptive measures in the context of daily interactions.

情绪化的面部表情在非语言交流中传递着重要信息,在面对面的人际关系中起着中介作用。识别面部表情需要根据所感知到的情绪来判断面部特征。因此,在 COVID-19 大流行期间,佩戴面罩破坏了人类从面部读取情绪的能力。然而,这些影响通常是通过研究表达刻板和夸张情绪的人脸来评估的,这与现实生活中的情况相去甚远。本研究的目的是通过一项情绪分类任务来评估面罩的影响,该任务使用的形态包括中性脸和表情脸(愤怒、厌恶、恐惧、快乐和悲伤)(从 0% 中性脸到 100% 的表情脸,每 20% 为一个步骤)。我们的结果表明,面具对厌恶、快乐和悲伤表情的识别有很大影响,导致低强度和高强度的识别成绩下降和误解增加。相比之下,戴面具对愤怒和恐惧以及中性表情的识别影响较小。未来的研究应该从更生态的角度来解决这个问题,以便在日常互动中采取具体的适应措施。
{"title":"Do masks cover more than just a face? A study on how facemasks affect the perception of emotional expressions according to their degree of intensity.","authors":"Pauline J N Thomas, Stéphanie Caharel","doi":"10.1177/03010066231201230","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066231201230","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emotional facial expressions convey crucial information in nonverbal communication and serve as a mediator in face-to-face relationships. Their recognition would rely on specific facial traits depending on the perceived emotion. During the COVID-19 pandemic, wearing a facemask has thus disrupted the human ability to read emotions from faces. Yet, these effects are usually assessed across studies from faces expressing stereotypical and exaggerated emotions, which is far removed from real-life conditions. The objective of the present study was to evaluate the impact of facemasks through an emotion categorization task using morphs ranging from a neutral face and an expressive face (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, and sadness) (from 0% neutral to 100% expressive in 20% steps). Our results revealed a strong impact of facemasks on the recognition of expressions of disgust, happiness, and sadness, resulting in a decrease in performance and an increase in misinterpretations, both for low and high levels of intensity. In contrast, the recognition of anger and fear, as well as neutral expression, was found to be less impacted by mask-wearing. Future studies should address this issue from a more ecological point of view with the aim of taking concrete adaptive measures in the context of daily interactions.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10244559","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Humans can sense large numbers of objects in a box by touch alone. 人类仅凭触摸就可以感知盒子里的大量物体。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-19 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231207324
Ilja Frissen, Alexander N Chen

Everyday experiences suggest that a container, such as a box of cereal, can convey pertinent information about the nature and quantity of its content. This study investigated how well people can judge large quantities of objects in a container through haptic perception. Stimuli consisted of plastic drinking straws cut to "small" (1.5 cm) or "big" (4.5 cm) pieces contained in plastic food containers. Participants performed both a magnitude estimation of the number of objects and a direct estimation of the proportion of the container perceived to be filled with objects. Overall, participants demonstrated considerable accuracy for both tasks and irrespective of the size of the content. Post-experiment interviews revealed three potential strategies. Participants either focused on the container's contents, the excess space in the container, or the perceived weight of the container (content).

日常经验表明,一个容器,比如一盒麦片,可以传达关于其内容物的性质和数量的相关信息。这项研究调查了人们通过触觉感知判断容器中大量物体的能力。刺激措施包括切成“小”(1.5 cm)或“大”(4.5 cm)装在塑料食品容器中的碎片。参与者对物体的数量进行了幅度估计,并直接估计了被感知为充满物体的容器的比例。总体而言,无论内容大小,参与者在这两项任务中都表现出了相当大的准确性。实验后的访谈揭示了三种潜在的策略。参与者要么关注容器的内容物、容器中多余的空间,要么关注容器(内容物)的感知重量。
{"title":"Humans can sense large numbers of objects in a box by touch alone.","authors":"Ilja Frissen, Alexander N Chen","doi":"10.1177/03010066231207324","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066231207324","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Everyday experiences suggest that a container, such as a box of cereal, can convey pertinent information about the nature and quantity of its content. This study investigated how well people can judge large quantities of objects in a container through haptic perception. Stimuli consisted of plastic drinking straws cut to \"small\" (1.5 cm) or \"big\" (4.5 cm) pieces contained in plastic food containers. Participants performed both a magnitude estimation of the number of objects and a direct estimation of the proportion of the container perceived to be filled with objects. Overall, participants demonstrated considerable accuracy for both tasks and irrespective of the size of the content. Post-experiment interviews revealed three potential strategies. Participants either focused on the container's contents, the excess space in the container, or the perceived weight of the container (content).</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49684228","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Stroop effect and mental imagery. 斯特劳普效应和心理意象。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-09 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231212152
Bence Nanay

The classic Stroop task is very simple: you have to name the color of words printed on a page. If these words are color words (like "red" or "blue"), where the color named and the color it is printed in are different (say, "red" printed in blue), the reaction time increases significantly. My aim is to argue that the existing psychological explanations of the Stroop effect need to be supplemented. The Stroop effect is not exclusively about access to motor control. It is also, to a large extent, about interferences in perceptual processing. To put it briefly, reading the color word triggers-laterally and automatically-visual imagery of the color and this interferes with the processing of the perceived color of the word. In other words, the Stroop effect is to a large extent a sensory phenomenon, and it has less to do with attention, conflict monitoring, or other higher-level phenomena.

经典的Stroop任务非常简单:你必须命名打印在页面上的单词的颜色。如果这些单词是彩色单词(如“红色”或“蓝色”),其中命名的颜色和打印的颜色不同(例如,用蓝色打印的“红色”),反应时间会显著增加。我的目的是认为现有的斯特罗普效应的心理学解释需要补充。Stroop效应不仅仅是关于对电机控制的访问。在很大程度上,这也是关于感知处理中的干扰。简单地说,阅读颜色单词会横向自动地触发颜色的视觉图像,这会干扰单词感知颜色的处理。换句话说,斯特劳普效应在很大程度上是一种感官现象,与注意力、冲突监测或其他更高层次的现象关系不大。
{"title":"The Stroop effect and mental imagery.","authors":"Bence Nanay","doi":"10.1177/03010066231212152","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066231212152","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The classic Stroop task is very simple: you have to name the color of words printed on a page. If these words are color words (like \"red\" or \"blue\"), where the color named and the color it is printed in are different (say, \"red\" printed in blue), the reaction time increases significantly. My aim is to argue that the existing psychological explanations of the Stroop effect need to be supplemented. The Stroop effect is not exclusively about access to motor control. It is also, to a large extent, about interferences in perceptual processing. To put it briefly, reading the color word triggers-laterally and automatically-visual imagery of the color and this interferes with the processing of the perceived color of the word. In other words, the Stroop effect is to a large extent a sensory phenomenon, and it has less to do with attention, conflict monitoring, or other higher-level phenomena.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10798018/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72015876","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The neural oscillations in delta- and theta-bands contribute to divided attention in audiovisual integration. 三角带和θ带的神经振荡导致视听整合中的注意力分散。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-30 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231208539
Xi Yang, Chen Ying, Lan Zhu, Wang Wenjing

One of key mechanisms implicated in multisensory processing is neural oscillations in distinct frequency band. Many studies explored the modulation of attention by recording the electroencephalography signals when subjects attended one modality, and ignored the other modality input. However, when attention is directed toward one modality, it may be not always possible to shut out completely inputs from a different modality. Since many situations require division of attention between audition and vision, it is imperative to investigate the neural mechanisms underlying processing of concurrent auditory and visual sensory streams. In the present study, we designed a task of audiovisual semantic discrimination, in which the subjects were asked to share attention to both auditory and visual stimuli. We explored the contribution of neural oscillations in lower-frequency to the modulation of divided attention on audiovisual integration. Our results implied that theta-band activity contributes to the early modulation of divided attention, and delta-band activity contributes to the late modulation of divided attention to audiovisual integration. Moreover, the fronto-central delta- and theta-bands activity is likely a marker of divided attention in audiovisual integration, and the neural oscillation on delta- and theta-bands is conducive to allocating attention resources to dual-tasking involving task-coordinating abilities.

涉及多感官处理的关键机制之一是不同频带的神经振荡。许多研究探索了当受试者注意一种模态而忽略另一种模态输入时,通过记录脑电图信号来调节注意力。然而,当注意力集中在一种模态上时,可能并不总是能够完全排除来自不同模态的输入。由于许多情况需要在听觉和视觉之间划分注意力,因此必须研究同时处理听觉和视觉感觉流的神经机制。在本研究中,我们设计了一项视听语义辨别任务,要求受试者共同关注听觉和视觉刺激。我们探讨了低频率的神经振荡对视听整合中分散注意力的调节作用。我们的研究结果表明,θ带活动有助于分化注意力的早期调节,而δ带活动有利于分化注意力对视听整合的后期调节。此外,在视听整合中,前部-中央的δ-和θ带活动可能是注意力分散的标志,δ-和theta带上的神经振荡有助于将注意力资源分配给涉及任务协调能力的双重任务。
{"title":"The neural oscillations in delta- and theta-bands contribute to divided attention in audiovisual integration.","authors":"Xi Yang, Chen Ying, Lan Zhu, Wang Wenjing","doi":"10.1177/03010066231208539","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066231208539","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>One of key mechanisms implicated in multisensory processing is neural oscillations in distinct frequency band. Many studies explored the modulation of attention by recording the electroencephalography signals when subjects attended one modality, and ignored the other modality input. However, when attention is directed toward one modality, it may be not always possible to shut out completely inputs from a different modality. Since many situations require division of attention between audition and vision, it is imperative to investigate the neural mechanisms underlying processing of concurrent auditory and visual sensory streams. In the present study, we designed a task of audiovisual semantic discrimination, in which the subjects were asked to share attention to both auditory and visual stimuli. We explored the contribution of neural oscillations in lower-frequency to the modulation of divided attention on audiovisual integration. Our results implied that theta-band activity contributes to the early modulation of divided attention, and delta-band activity contributes to the late modulation of divided attention to audiovisual integration. Moreover, the fronto-central delta- and theta-bands activity is likely a marker of divided attention in audiovisual integration, and the neural oscillation on delta- and theta-bands is conducive to allocating attention resources to dual-tasking involving task-coordinating abilities.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71415022","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Book review: Attending to Moving Objects by Holcombe, Alex 书评:关注移动物体》,作者:霍尔科姆,亚历克斯
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-12-06 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231218910
Emily M. Crowe
{"title":"Book review: Attending to Moving Objects by Holcombe, Alex","authors":"Emily M. Crowe","doi":"10.1177/03010066231218910","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/03010066231218910","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-12-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138597197","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Humans can sense small numbers of objects in a box by touch alone. 人类仅凭触摸就能感知盒子里的少量物体。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-20 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231201960
Ilja Frissen, Zhanat Kappassov, Kai-Yi Huang, Mounia Ziat

Everyday experiences suggest that a container, such as a box of chocolate sprinkles, can convey pertinent information about the nature of its content. Despite the familiarity of the experience, we do not know whether people can perceive the number of objects in the container from touch alone and how accurately they can do so. In three experiments, participants handled containers holding between one and five objects and verbally estimated their number. Containers were small cardboard jewelry boxes, and objects were round beads of varying diameter and weight. Any useful visual and auditory cues were precluded. Experiment 1 demonstrated very accurate performance, provided the objects were of sufficient weight. Experiment 2 demonstrated that withholding information about the possible number of objects inside the container does not affect accuracy at a group level but does produce occasional overestimations at an individual level. Experiment 3 demonstrated that removing the weight cue leads to systematic underestimations but does not eliminate people's ability to distinguish between different numbers of objects in the container. This study contributes to a growing picture that container haptics is surprisingly capable.

日常经验表明,一个容器,比如一盒巧克力,可以传达有关其内容性质的相关信息。尽管人们对这种体验很熟悉,但我们不知道人们是否能仅凭触摸就能感知容器中物体的数量,以及他们能多准确地感知。在三个实验中,参与者处理了装有一到五个物体的容器,并口头估计了它们的数量。容器是小纸板首饰盒,物体是直径和重量不等的圆形珠子。任何有用的视觉和听觉线索都被排除在外。实验1证明了非常精确的性能,前提是物体有足够的重量。实验2表明,隐瞒容器内可能物体数量的信息不会影响群体水平的准确性,但会偶尔在个体水平上产生过高估计。实验3表明,去除重量提示会导致系统性的低估,但不会消除人们区分容器中不同数量物体的能力。这项研究为容器触觉的惊人能力提供了一个不断增长的画面。
{"title":"Humans can sense small numbers of objects in a box by touch alone.","authors":"Ilja Frissen, Zhanat Kappassov, Kai-Yi Huang, Mounia Ziat","doi":"10.1177/03010066231201960","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066231201960","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Everyday experiences suggest that a container, such as a box of chocolate sprinkles, can convey pertinent information about the nature of its content. Despite the familiarity of the experience, we do not know whether people can perceive the number of objects in the container from touch alone and how accurately they can do so. In three experiments, participants handled containers holding between one and five objects and verbally estimated their number. Containers were small cardboard jewelry boxes, and objects were round beads of varying diameter and weight. Any useful visual and auditory cues were precluded. Experiment 1 demonstrated very accurate performance, provided the objects were of sufficient weight. Experiment 2 demonstrated that withholding information about the possible number of objects inside the container does not affect accuracy at a group level but does produce occasional overestimations at an individual level. Experiment 3 demonstrated that removing the weight cue leads to systematic underestimations but does not eliminate people's ability to distinguish between different numbers of objects in the container. This study contributes to a growing picture that container haptics is surprisingly capable.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10634214/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41140636","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Face inversion effect on perceived cuteness of infant faces. 面孔倒置对婴儿面孔可爱性感知的影响。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-04 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231198417
Kana Kuraguchi, Hiroshi Nittono

Research has demonstrated that attractiveness evaluations of adult faces were less accurate when faces were inverted than upright. It remains unknown, however, whether a similar effect applies to perceived cuteness of infants, which is assumed to be based on elemental facial features called the "baby schema." In this research, we studied the face inversion effect on perceived cuteness of infant faces in a rating task and a two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) task. We also examined beauty as a control dimension. Although the rating task revealed no inversion effect, the 2AFC task showed poorer discrimination performance with inverted faces than with upright faces in both evaluations. These results indicate that infant cuteness and beauty dimensions are correlated well with each other, and their perception not only relies on elemental features that are not strongly affected by inversion but is also affected by holistic facial configurations when a detailed comparison is required.

研究表明,当面部倒置时,对成人面部吸引力的评估不如直立时准确。然而,目前尚不清楚类似的影响是否适用于婴儿的感知可爱度,这被认为是基于被称为“婴儿模式”的基本面部特征。在这项研究中,我们研究了在评级任务和两种可选强迫选择(2AFC)任务中,面部倒置对婴儿面部感知可爱度的影响。我们还研究了美作为一种控制维度。尽管评级任务没有显示出倒置效应,但在两次评估中,2AFC任务在倒置人脸的情况下表现出比直立人脸更差的辨别能力。这些结果表明,婴儿的可爱和美丽维度之间有很好的相关性,当需要进行详细的比较时,他们的感知不仅依赖于不受倒置强烈影响的基本特征,而且还受到整体面部配置的影响。
{"title":"Face inversion effect on perceived cuteness of infant faces.","authors":"Kana Kuraguchi, Hiroshi Nittono","doi":"10.1177/03010066231198417","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066231198417","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research has demonstrated that attractiveness evaluations of adult faces were less accurate when faces were inverted than upright. It remains unknown, however, whether a similar effect applies to perceived cuteness of infants, which is assumed to be based on elemental facial features called the \"baby schema.\" In this research, we studied the face inversion effect on perceived cuteness of infant faces in a rating task and a two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) task. We also examined beauty as a control dimension. Although the rating task revealed no inversion effect, the 2AFC task showed poorer discrimination performance with inverted faces than with upright faces in both evaluations. These results indicate that infant cuteness and beauty dimensions are correlated well with each other, and their perception not only relies on elemental features that are not strongly affected by inversion but is also affected by holistic facial configurations when a detailed comparison is required.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10137313","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Stimulus intensity modulates perceived tactile distance. 刺激强度调节感知的触觉距离。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-11 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231200434
Matthew R Longo, Sonia Medina

Several features of tactile stimuli modulate the perceived distance between touches. In particular, distances are perceived as farther apart when the time interval between them is longer, than when it is shorter. Such effects have been interpreted as a form of 'psychological relativity', analogous to Einstein's conception of a four-dimensional space-time. We investigated whether similar effects occur for stimulus features other than time, specifically stimulus intensity. We hypothesised that perceived distance would be increased when the two stimuli differed in intensity, since they would then be farther apart in a multi-dimensional feature space. Participants made verbal estimates of the perceived distance between two touches on their left hand. Intensity was manipulated such that both stimuli could be intense, both could be light, or one could be intense and the other light. We found no evidence for change in perceived tactile distance when stimuli intensity mis-matched. In contrast, there were clear effects of average stimulus intensity on perceived distance. Intense stimuli were judged as farther apart than light stimuli, and mixed stimuli were intermediate. These results are consistent with theories of general magnitude representation, which argue that multiple dimensions of magnitude are dependent on a shared underlying representation of domain-general magnitude.

触觉刺激的几个特征调节触摸之间的感知距离。特别是,当距离之间的时间间隔较长时,距离被认为比较短时相距更远。这种效应被解释为“心理相对论”的一种形式,类似于爱因斯坦的四维时空概念。我们调查了除了时间之外的刺激特征,特别是刺激强度,是否会产生类似的影响。我们假设,当两种刺激的强度不同时,感知距离会增加,因为它们在多维特征空间中会相距更远。参与者对左手两次触摸之间的感知距离进行了口头估计。强度被操纵,使得两种刺激都可以是强烈的,两种都可以是光的,或者一种可以是强烈而另一种是光的。当刺激强度不匹配时,我们没有发现感知触觉距离变化的证据。相反,平均刺激强度对感知距离有明显的影响。强烈刺激被判断为比轻度刺激相距更远,混合刺激则是中等的。这些结果与一般震级表示理论一致,后者认为震级的多个维度取决于域一般震级的共享底层表示。
{"title":"Stimulus intensity modulates perceived tactile distance.","authors":"Matthew R Longo, Sonia Medina","doi":"10.1177/03010066231200434","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066231200434","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Several features of tactile stimuli modulate the perceived distance between touches. In particular, distances are perceived as farther apart when the time interval between them is longer, than when it is shorter. Such effects have been interpreted as a form of 'psychological relativity', analogous to Einstein's conception of a four-dimensional space-time. We investigated whether similar effects occur for stimulus features other than time, specifically stimulus intensity. We hypothesised that perceived distance would be increased when the two stimuli differed in intensity, since they would then be farther apart in a multi-dimensional feature space. Participants made verbal estimates of the perceived distance between two touches on their left hand. Intensity was manipulated such that both stimuli could be intense, both could be light, or one could be intense and the other light. We found no evidence for change in perceived tactile distance when stimuli intensity mis-matched. In contrast, there were clear effects of average stimulus intensity on perceived distance. Intense stimuli were judged as farther apart than light stimuli, and mixed stimuli were intermediate. These results are consistent with theories of general magnitude representation, which argue that multiple dimensions of magnitude are dependent on a shared underlying representation of domain-general magnitude.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10570415","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Use of minimal working memory in visual comparison: An eye-tracking study. 视觉比较中最小工作记忆的使用:一项眼球追踪研究。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-15 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231194488
Yijing Li, Xiangling Zhuang, Guojie Ma

In this study, we used a novel application of the previous paradigm provided by Pomplun to examine the eye movement strategies of using minimal working memory in visual comparison. This paradigm includes two tasks: one is a free comparison and the other is a single sequential comparison. In the free comparison, participants can freely view two horizontally presented stimuli until they judge whether the two stimuli are the same or not. In the single sequential comparison, participants can only view the left-side stimuli one time, and when their eyes cross the invisible boundary at the center of the screen, the left-side stimuli disappear and the right-side stimuli appear. Participants need to judge whether the right-side stimuli are the same as the disappeared left-side stimuli. Eye movement data showed significant differences between the single sequential comparison and free comparison tasks that suggests the use of minimal working memory in free comparison. Moreover, when the number of items was more than three, an average of 2.87 items would be processed in each view sequence. Participants also used the alternating left-right reference strategy that made the shortest scan path with the use of minimal working memory. The typical eye movement strategy in visual comparison and its theoretical significance were discussed.

在这项研究中,我们使用了Pomplun提供的先前范式的新应用来检验在视觉比较中使用最小工作记忆的眼动策略。这种范式包括两个任务:一个是自由比较,另一个是单一的顺序比较。在自由比较中,参与者可以自由地观察两个水平呈现的刺激,直到他们判断这两个刺激是否相同。在单序列比较中,参与者只能看到左侧刺激一次,当他们的眼睛越过屏幕中心的不可见边界时,左侧刺激消失,右侧刺激出现。参与者需要判断右侧刺激是否与消失的左侧刺激相同。眼动数据显示,单序列比较和自由比较任务之间存在显著差异,这表明在自由比较中使用了最小工作记忆。此外,当项目数量超过三个时,每个视图序列中平均将处理2.87个项目。参与者还使用了交替的左右参考策略,利用最小的工作记忆做出最短的扫描路径。讨论了视觉比较中典型的眼动策略及其理论意义。
{"title":"Use of minimal working memory in visual comparison: An eye-tracking study.","authors":"Yijing Li, Xiangling Zhuang, Guojie Ma","doi":"10.1177/03010066231194488","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066231194488","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this study, we used a novel application of the previous paradigm provided by Pomplun to examine the eye movement strategies of using minimal working memory in visual comparison. This paradigm includes two tasks: one is a free comparison and the other is a single sequential comparison. In the free comparison, participants can freely view two horizontally presented stimuli until they judge whether the two stimuli are the same or not. In the single sequential comparison, participants can only view the left-side stimuli one time, and when their eyes cross the invisible boundary at the center of the screen, the left-side stimuli disappear and the right-side stimuli appear. Participants need to judge whether the right-side stimuli are the same as the disappeared left-side stimuli. Eye movement data showed significant differences between the single sequential comparison and free comparison tasks that suggests the use of minimal working memory in free comparison. Moreover, when the number of items was more than three, an average of 2.87 items would be processed in each view sequence. Participants also used the alternating left-right reference strategy that made the shortest scan path with the use of minimal working memory. The typical eye movement strategy in visual comparison and its theoretical significance were discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9997756","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of wearing an opaque or transparent face mask on the perception of facial expressions: A comparative study between Japanese school-aged children and adults. 戴不透明或透明口罩对面部表情感知的影响:日本学龄儿童和成年人的比较研究。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-20 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231200693
Yuki Miyazaki, Miki Kamatani, Shuma Tsurumi, Tomokazu Suda, Kei Wakasugi, Kaori Matsunaga, Jun I Kawahara

The negative side effects of mask-wearing on reading facial emotional cues have been investigated in several studies with adults post-2020. However, little is known about children. This study aimed to determine the negative influence of mask-wearing on reading emotions of adult faces by Japanese school-aged children, compared to Japanese adults. We also examined whether this negative influence could be alleviated by using a transparent face mask instead of an opaque one (surgical mask). The performance on reading emotions was measured using emotion categorization and emotion intensity rating tasks for adult faces. As per the findings, the accuracy of emotion recognition in children was impaired for various facial expressions (disgust, fear, happy, neutral, sad, and surprise faces), except for angry faces. Conversely, in adults, it was impaired for a few facial expressions. The perceived intensity for happy faces with a surgical mask was weaker in both children and adults than in those without the mask. A negative influence of wearing surgical masks was generally not observed for faces wearing a transparent mask in both children and adults. Thus, negative side effects of mask-wearing on reading emotions are observed for more facial expressions in children than in adults; transparent masks can help remedy these.

几项针对2020年后成年人的研究调查了戴口罩对阅读面部情绪线索的负面副作用。然而,人们对儿童知之甚少。本研究旨在确定与日本成年人相比,日本学龄儿童戴口罩对成人面部阅读情绪的负面影响。我们还研究了使用透明口罩而不是不透明口罩(外科口罩)是否可以减轻这种负面影响。使用成人面部的情绪分类和情绪强度评级任务来测量阅读情绪的表现。根据研究结果,除了愤怒的脸,儿童的各种面部表情(厌恶、恐惧、快乐、中性、悲伤和惊讶的脸)的情绪识别准确性都受到了损害。相反,在成年人中,它会因一些面部表情而受损。戴口罩的儿童和成年人对快乐面孔的感知强度都比不戴口罩的人弱。在儿童和成人中,佩戴透明口罩的人脸通常不会受到佩戴外科口罩的负面影响。因此,与成年人相比,儿童的面部表情更多,戴口罩对阅读情绪的负面副作用;透明口罩可以帮助解决这些问题。
{"title":"Effects of wearing an opaque or transparent face mask on the perception of facial expressions: A comparative study between Japanese school-aged children and adults.","authors":"Yuki Miyazaki, Miki Kamatani, Shuma Tsurumi, Tomokazu Suda, Kei Wakasugi, Kaori Matsunaga, Jun I Kawahara","doi":"10.1177/03010066231200693","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066231200693","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The negative side effects of mask-wearing on reading facial emotional cues have been investigated in several studies with adults post-2020. However, little is known about children. This study aimed to determine the negative influence of mask-wearing on reading emotions of adult faces by Japanese school-aged children, compared to Japanese adults. We also examined whether this negative influence could be alleviated by using a transparent face mask instead of an opaque one (surgical mask). The performance on reading emotions was measured using emotion categorization and emotion intensity rating tasks for adult faces. As per the findings, the accuracy of emotion recognition in children was impaired for various facial expressions (disgust, fear, happy, neutral, sad, and surprise faces), except for angry faces. Conversely, in adults, it was impaired for a few facial expressions. The perceived intensity for happy faces with a surgical mask was weaker in both children and adults than in those without the mask. A negative influence of wearing surgical masks was generally not observed for faces wearing a transparent mask in both children and adults. Thus, negative side effects of mask-wearing on reading emotions are observed for more facial expressions in children than in adults; transparent masks can help remedy these.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41166567","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Perception
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1