首页 > 最新文献

Animal Behaviour最新文献

英文 中文
Activity of Asiatic lions in relation to activity of prey and kleptoparasitism 亚洲狮的活动与猎物活动和偷食寄生有关
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.002
Kausik Banerjee , Chittaranjan V. Dave , Kartikeya Singh Chauhan , Shomen Mukherjee , Yadvendradev V. Jhala
Carnivores alter their activity to maximize individual fitness by increasing encounters with prey, reducing overlap with humans and minimizing opportunities for loss of kills to kleptoparasitism. We observed 16 radiocollared lions, Panthera leo leo, within the Gir landscape by continuous focal animal sampling for a period of 2–10 days in each session and studied their hunting and feeding patterns with respect to the activity patterns of their prey, chital and domestic livestock. Lions were mostly active during the night while most of the day was spent resting under cover, when temperatures and human activity were high. Activity of lions differed between the sexes but coincided with the activity of their prey. Feeding intervals and feeding bouts for lions were longer inside the protected area compared to multiuse landscapes outside it. Lions tended to feed longer on predated than scavenged carcasses. Livestock comprised the majority of the lions’ diet in the Gir landscape. We observed a low level of kleptoparasitism in the Asiatic lions compared with their counterparts in the Serengeti in Africa. Hunting by lionesses declined during the corresponding male activity period, possibly to minimize kleptoparasitism by male lions. We conclude that the hunting and feeding times of lions are adapted to maximize payoffs.
食肉动物会通过增加与猎物的接触、减少与人类的重叠以及最大限度地减少因偷猎而损失猎物的机会来改变自己的活动,从而最大限度地提高个体的生存能力。我们通过每次为期 2-10 天的连续重点动物取样,在吉尔地貌中观察了 16 头携带放射线耳环的狮子(Panthera leo leo),并研究了它们的狩猎和进食模式与猎物(chital 和家畜)的活动模式之间的关系。狮子大多在夜间活动,而白天的大部分时间则是在气温和人类活动频繁时在遮蔽物下休息。狮子的活动在性别上存在差异,但与猎物的活动相吻合。与保护区外的多用途景观相比,保护区内狮子的进食间隔和进食次数更长。狮子捕食被捕食者尸体的时间往往长于捕食被拾取者尸体的时间。在 Gir 地貌景观中,牲畜是狮子的主要食物。与非洲塞伦盖蒂的狮子相比,我们观察到亚洲狮的偷窃寄生程度较低。在相应的雄狮活动期间,雌狮的狩猎活动有所减少,这可能是为了尽量减少雄狮的偷食行为。我们的结论是,狮子的狩猎和觅食时间是为了获得最大回报而调整的。
{"title":"Activity of Asiatic lions in relation to activity of prey and kleptoparasitism","authors":"Kausik Banerjee ,&nbsp;Chittaranjan V. Dave ,&nbsp;Kartikeya Singh Chauhan ,&nbsp;Shomen Mukherjee ,&nbsp;Yadvendradev V. Jhala","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.002","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.002","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Carnivores alter their activity to maximize individual fitness by increasing encounters with prey, reducing overlap with humans and minimizing opportunities for loss of kills to kleptoparasitism. We observed 16 radiocollared lions, <em>Panthera leo leo</em>, within the Gir landscape by continuous focal animal sampling for a period of 2–10 days in each session and studied their hunting and feeding patterns with respect to the activity patterns of their prey, chital and domestic livestock. Lions were mostly active during the night while most of the day was spent resting under cover, when temperatures and human activity were high. Activity of lions differed between the sexes but coincided with the activity of their prey. Feeding intervals and feeding bouts for lions were longer inside the protected area compared to multiuse landscapes outside it. Lions tended to feed longer on predated than scavenged carcasses. Livestock comprised the majority of the lions’ diet in the Gir landscape. We observed a low level of kleptoparasitism in the Asiatic lions compared with their counterparts in the Serengeti in Africa. Hunting by lionesses declined during the corresponding male activity period, possibly to minimize kleptoparasitism by male lions. We conclude that the hunting and feeding times of lions are adapted to maximize payoffs.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"218 ","pages":"Pages 195-206"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142586365","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hard exoskeleton of beetles as armour against avian predators 甲虫的坚硬外骨骼是抵御鸟类捕食者的盔甲
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.007
Wataru Kojima , Kosuke Hidaka , Sakito Koizumi , Yuta Murayama , Toshiyuki Nakata
The robust exoskeleton of beetles, Coleoptera, is believed to have protective advantages, contributing to their evolutionary success. However, empirical evidence of the defensive capabilities of this exoskeleton remains surprisingly scarce. Here, we demonstrate the critical role of the robust beetle exoskeleton in protecting against avian predation. We found that flower chafers (Scarabaeidae, Cetoniinae) have more robust bodies than other scarab species. Laboratory experiments with naïve Japanese quail, Coturnix japonica, revealed that some individuals of intact Cetoniinae survived attacks without serious injury, whereas all individuals of soft scarab species or elytra-removed Cetoniinae were consumed. The survival rate of intact Cetoniinae increased in complex environments because the combination of their stiffness and elliptical shape made it difficult for quail to handle the prey. Field experiments with wild white-cheeked starlings, Sturnus cineraceus, and Eurasian tree sparrows, Passer montanus, demonstrated that most individuals of Cetoniinae species were ignored, whereas soft species were readily preyed upon. Further, when we presented the starlings with the Cetoniinae species Protaetia orientalis that had artificially softened bodies and altered appearance, the starlings readily preyed upon them. This observation suggests that P. orientalis is not chemically defended. Moreover, wild birds can visually discriminate hard species because of prior experience with the unprofitable prey. These results collectively provide evidence that the robust exoskeleton of beetles protects them from predatory attack.
甲虫(鞘翅目)坚固的外骨骼被认为具有保护优势,有助于它们在进化过程中取得成功。然而,有关这种外骨骼防御能力的实证证据仍然少得令人吃惊。在这里,我们证明了坚固的甲虫外骨骼在抵御鸟类捕食方面的关键作用。我们发现花糠金龟(Scarabaeidae,Cetoniinae)的身体比其他金龟子物种更加坚固。用天真无邪的日本鹌鹑(Coturnix japonica)进行的实验室实验表明,一些完整的花蟾科个体在受到攻击后存活下来,没有受到严重伤害,而所有软体猩红蝶物种或被切除外骨骼的花蟾科个体都被吃掉了。在复杂的环境中,完好无损的Cetoniinae的存活率会增加,这是因为它们的硬度和椭圆形的结合使得鹌鹑很难处理这些猎物。用野生白颊椋鸟(Sturnus cineraceus)和欧亚树麻雀(Passer montanus)进行的野外实验表明,大多数Cetoniinae种类的个体都会被忽略,而软体种类则很容易被捕食。此外,当我们向椋鸟展示人工软化身体和改变外观的Cetoniinae物种Protaetia orientalis时,椋鸟很容易捕食它们。这一观察结果表明,东方椋鸟没有化学防御能力。此外,野生鸟类可以通过视觉来辨别坚硬的物种,因为它们以前曾捕食过无利可图的猎物。这些结果共同证明,甲虫坚固的外骨骼可以保护它们免受捕食性攻击。
{"title":"Hard exoskeleton of beetles as armour against avian predators","authors":"Wataru Kojima ,&nbsp;Kosuke Hidaka ,&nbsp;Sakito Koizumi ,&nbsp;Yuta Murayama ,&nbsp;Toshiyuki Nakata","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.007","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.007","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>The robust exoskeleton of beetles, Coleoptera, is believed to have protective advantages, contributing to their evolutionary success. However, empirical evidence of the defensive capabilities of this exoskeleton remains surprisingly scarce. Here, we demonstrate the critical role of the robust beetle exoskeleton in protecting against avian predation. We found that flower chafers (Scarabaeidae, Cetoniinae) have more robust bodies than other scarab species. Laboratory experiments with naïve Japanese quail, <em>Coturnix japonica</em>, revealed that some individuals of intact Cetoniinae survived attacks without serious injury, whereas all individuals of soft scarab species or elytra-removed Cetoniinae were consumed. The survival rate of intact Cetoniinae increased in complex environments because the combination of their stiffness and elliptical shape made it difficult for quail to handle the prey. Field experiments with wild white-cheeked starlings, <em>Sturnus cineraceus</em>, and Eurasian tree sparrows, <em>Passer montanus</em>, demonstrated that most individuals of Cetoniinae species were ignored, whereas soft species were readily preyed upon. Further, when we presented the starlings with the Cetoniinae species <em>Protaetia orientalis</em> that had artificially softened bodies and altered appearance, the starlings readily preyed upon them. This observation suggests that <em>P. orientalis</em> is not chemically defended. Moreover, wild birds can visually discriminate hard species because of prior experience with the unprofitable prey. These results collectively provide evidence that the robust exoskeleton of beetles protects them from predatory attack.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"218 ","pages":"Pages 163-172"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142571460","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differential impact of artificial light at night on cognitive flexibility in visual and spatial reversal learning tasks 夜间人造光对视觉和空间反转学习任务中认知灵活性的不同影响
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.008
Gaia De Russi , Giulia Montalbano , Elia Gatto , Elena Maggi , Stefano Cannicci , Cristiano Bertolucci , Tyrone Lucon-Xiccato
Artificial light at night (ALAN) is a pervasive environmental pollutant with multiple adverse effects on animal biology. As the vertebrate nervous system is particularly sensitive to light effects, this study explores the potential negative impact of ALAN on cognition in adult zebrafish, Danio rerio. Fish from mesocosm populations exposed to either ALAN or control conditions underwent visual and spatial learning tasks, along with an evaluation of cognitive flexibility with visual and spatial reversal learning tasks. Contrary to our initial prediction of a general negative impact, learning performance was not affected by the ALAN treatment. The analysis of reversal learning revealed task-dependent effects on cognitive flexibility: fish exposed to ALAN performed worse than control fish in the visual reversal learning task, but displayed better performance in the spatial reversal learning task. We propose that the influence of ALAN on cognition might differ between different cognitive functions or involve noncognitive factors that play different roles in the testing paradigms. The study underscores the importance of task specificity in nonadaptive cognitive plasticity and calls for ecological assessments to quantify fitness consequences of ALAN in natural settings.
夜间人造光(ALAN)是一种普遍存在的环境污染物,对动物生物学有多种不利影响。由于脊椎动物的神经系统对光的影响特别敏感,本研究探讨了 ALAN 对成年斑马鱼(Danio rerio)认知能力的潜在负面影响。来自暴露于 ALAN 或对照条件下的中观宇宙种群的鱼类接受了视觉和空间学习任务,并通过视觉和空间反转学习任务评估了认知灵活性。与我们最初预测的普遍负面影响相反,学习成绩并未受到 ALAN 处理的影响。对逆转学习的分析表明,认知灵活性受到任务的影响:暴露于ALAN的鱼在视觉逆转学习任务中的表现比对照组鱼差,但在空间逆转学习任务中的表现更好。我们认为,ALAN 对认知的影响可能因不同的认知功能而异,或者涉及在测试范式中发挥不同作用的非认知因素。这项研究强调了任务特异性在非适应性认知可塑性中的重要性,并呼吁进行生态评估,以量化ALAN在自然环境中对体能的影响。
{"title":"Differential impact of artificial light at night on cognitive flexibility in visual and spatial reversal learning tasks","authors":"Gaia De Russi ,&nbsp;Giulia Montalbano ,&nbsp;Elia Gatto ,&nbsp;Elena Maggi ,&nbsp;Stefano Cannicci ,&nbsp;Cristiano Bertolucci ,&nbsp;Tyrone Lucon-Xiccato","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.008","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.008","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Artificial light at night (ALAN) is a pervasive environmental pollutant with multiple adverse effects on animal biology. As the vertebrate nervous system is particularly sensitive to light effects, this study explores the potential negative impact of ALAN on cognition in adult zebrafish, <em>Danio rerio</em>. Fish from mesocosm populations exposed to either ALAN or control conditions underwent visual and spatial learning tasks, along with an evaluation of cognitive flexibility with visual and spatial reversal learning tasks. Contrary to our initial prediction of a general negative impact, learning performance was not affected by the ALAN treatment. The analysis of reversal learning revealed task-dependent effects on cognitive flexibility: fish exposed to ALAN performed worse than control fish in the visual reversal learning task, but displayed better performance in the spatial reversal learning task. We propose that the influence of ALAN on cognition might differ between different cognitive functions or involve noncognitive factors that play different roles in the testing paradigms. The study underscores the importance of task specificity in nonadaptive cognitive plasticity and calls for ecological assessments to quantify fitness consequences of ALAN in natural settings.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"218 ","pages":"Pages 173-183"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142571461","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Without optimum support: effect of maternal early deprivation on play in semiwild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes 没有最佳支持:母性早期剥夺对半野生黑猩猩游戏的影响
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.016
Martina Francesconi , Antonio Lorenzo Loprete , Veronica Maglieri , Marina Davila-Ross , Elisabetta Palagi
In mammals, robust connection between mothers and offspring plays pivotal roles in physiological and socioemotional development. Our study investigated the consequences of early maternal deprivation on social play behaviour in immature chimpanzees, examining expected and unexpected findings. In line with the predicted hypothesis, orphaned individuals exhibited lower frequencies of social play than mother-reared chimpanzees, supporting traditional notions of the indispensable role of maternal influence in the correct expression of social behaviours. However, the absence of discernible differences in the variability and roughness of playful patterns, tested for the first time in orphan and mother-reared chimpanzees, suggests that the presence of adult models may not be fundamental for expanding and regulating immature playful behavioural repertoire. Escalation into overt aggression did not differ between orphan and mother-reared chimpanzees, challenging the idea of the crucial role of maternal investment in developing self-restrain abilities during play. Orphans engaged in shorter sessions compared with mother-reared subjects; this strategy is naturally adopted by wild and captive chimpanzees when they need to cope with the risk of escalation during play fighting. Although maternal deprivation has been linked to anxiety-related and depressive behaviours in primates, our results reveal a degree of behavioural resilience in orphans that are still able to adhere to social rules during play, particularly when having contact with their peers. Although play behaviour development can arise from the multifaceted interconnection between early experiences, social dynamics and individual propensity to interact socially, our findings support the view, emerging from a plethora of studies on many social mammals, that play has an irreplaceable role in an individual's life history.
在哺乳动物中,母亲与后代之间的紧密联系在生理和社会情感发育中起着关键作用。我们的研究调查了早期母性缺失对未成年黑猩猩社交游戏行为的影响,研究了预期和意外的发现。与预想的假设一致,孤儿黑猩猩的社会游戏频率低于由母亲抚养的黑猩猩,这支持了母性影响在正确表达社会行为中不可或缺的传统观点。然而,首次在孤儿黑猩猩和母亲饲养的黑猩猩身上测试的游戏模式的可变性和粗糙度没有明显的差异,这表明成人模型的存在可能不是扩大和调节未成熟游戏行为的根本。孤儿黑猩猩和由母亲饲养的黑猩猩在升级为明显的攻击行为方面没有差异,这对母性投资在游戏过程中培养自我约束能力的关键作用这一观点提出了挑战。与母亲饲养的黑猩猩相比,孤儿的游戏时间更短;当野生和圈养的黑猩猩需要应对游戏打斗升级的风险时,它们自然会采取这种策略。虽然母性剥夺与灵长类动物的焦虑和抑郁行为有关,但我们的研究结果表明,孤儿在一定程度上具有行为恢复能力,它们在游戏中仍然能够遵守社会规则,尤其是在与同伴接触时。尽管游戏行为的发展可能源于早期经历、社会动力和个体社会交往倾向之间的多方面相互联系,但我们的研究结果支持了对许多社会哺乳动物的大量研究得出的观点,即游戏在个体的生活史中具有不可替代的作用。
{"title":"Without optimum support: effect of maternal early deprivation on play in semiwild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes","authors":"Martina Francesconi ,&nbsp;Antonio Lorenzo Loprete ,&nbsp;Veronica Maglieri ,&nbsp;Marina Davila-Ross ,&nbsp;Elisabetta Palagi","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.016","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.016","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>In mammals, robust connection between mothers and offspring plays pivotal roles in physiological and socioemotional development. Our study investigated the consequences of early maternal deprivation on social play behaviour in immature chimpanzees, examining expected and unexpected findings. In line with the predicted hypothesis, orphaned individuals exhibited lower frequencies of social play than mother-reared chimpanzees, supporting traditional notions of the indispensable role of maternal influence in the correct expression of social behaviours. However, the absence of discernible differences in the variability and roughness of playful patterns, tested for the first time in orphan and mother-reared chimpanzees, suggests that the presence of adult models may not be fundamental for expanding and regulating immature playful behavioural repertoire. Escalation into overt aggression did not differ between orphan and mother-reared chimpanzees, challenging the idea of the crucial role of maternal investment in developing self-restrain abilities during play. Orphans engaged in shorter sessions compared with mother-reared subjects; this strategy is naturally adopted by wild and captive chimpanzees when they need to cope with the risk of escalation during play fighting. Although maternal deprivation has been linked to anxiety-related and depressive behaviours in primates, our results reveal a degree of behavioural resilience in orphans that are still able to adhere to social rules during play, particularly when having contact with their peers. Although play behaviour development can arise from the multifaceted interconnection between early experiences, social dynamics and individual propensity to interact socially, our findings support the view, emerging from a plethora of studies on many social mammals, that play has an irreplaceable role in an individual's life history.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"218 ","pages":"Pages 185-193"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142571462","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Potential signature whistle production by Indo-Pacific humpback dolphins, Sousa chinensis, in the northern South China sea 南中国海北部印度洋中华白海豚发出哨声的潜在特征
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.001
Agathe Serres , Jean-Hugh Thomas , Lijun Dong , Shenglan Chen , Binshuai Liu , Songhai Li
Dolphin communication involves acoustic signals, including whistles, and the well-studied bottlenose dolphins produce individually distinctive whistles called signature whistles (SWs). The production of a potential SW by an injured Indo-Pacific humpback dolphin has been reported, but no study has attempted to validate this finding in this species. Using data collected during encounters with free-ranging Indo-Pacific humpback dolphins at two locations in the northern South China Sea, we investigated the production of SWs by these dolphins. Of the 3846 analysed whistles, 37% were identified as potential SWs (PSWs) using the SIGnature whistle IDentification method and categorized into 82 PSW types. Overall, PSWs were identified during 54% of encounters. Given the high production rate of stereotyped whistles (62% of all whistles in 90% of encounters) compared with the identified PSWs, we suggest that the SIGnature whistle IDentification method criteria cannot be fully adapted for the detection of SWs in Indo-Pacific humpback dolphins, and more research should be conducted to adapt the criteria to the species. In addition, the characteristics of PSWs differed slightly between locations, potentially because of the geographical separation of populations and habitat differences (e.g. noise levels). The present results confirm the production of stereotyped whistles, including PSWs, by Indo-Pacific humpback dolphins. Further research should be conducted to confirm whether these whistles are similar to bottlenose dolphins’ SWs.
海豚的交流涉及包括哨声在内的声学信号,其中研究较多的瓶鼻海豚会发出被称为 "签名哨声"(SW)的独特哨声。据报道,一只受伤的印度洋-太平洋座头海豚可能会发出 SW,但还没有研究试图在该物种中验证这一发现。利用在南中国海北部两个地点与自由活动的印度洋-太平洋中华白海豚相遇时收集的数据,我们对这些海豚发出的 SW 进行了调查。在分析的 3846 个哨声中,37% 通过 SIGnature 哨声识别方法被识别为潜在的 SW(PSW),并分为 82 种 PSW 类型。总体而言,在 54% 的相遇中,PSW 被识别出来。鉴于定型哨音的产生率较高(在90%的相遇中占所有哨音的62%),与已识别的PSW相比,我们认为SIGnature哨音识别方法的标准不能完全适用于检测印度洋-太平洋中华白海豚的SW,因此应开展更多的研究,使该标准适用于该物种。此外,不同地点的 PSWs 特征略有不同,这可能是由于种群的地理分隔和栖息地的差异(如噪音水平)造成的。本研究结果证实了印度洋-太平洋中华白海豚能发出包括PSW在内的定型哨音。应开展进一步研究,以确认这些哨声是否与瓶鼻海豚的SW相似。
{"title":"Potential signature whistle production by Indo-Pacific humpback dolphins, Sousa chinensis, in the northern South China sea","authors":"Agathe Serres ,&nbsp;Jean-Hugh Thomas ,&nbsp;Lijun Dong ,&nbsp;Shenglan Chen ,&nbsp;Binshuai Liu ,&nbsp;Songhai Li","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.001","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.001","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Dolphin communication involves acoustic signals, including whistles, and the well-studied bottlenose dolphins produce individually distinctive whistles called signature whistles (SWs). The production of a potential SW by an injured Indo-Pacific humpback dolphin has been reported, but no study has attempted to validate this finding in this species. Using data collected during encounters with free-ranging Indo-Pacific humpback dolphins at two locations in the northern South China Sea, we investigated the production of SWs by these dolphins. Of the 3846 analysed whistles, 37% were identified as potential SWs (PSWs) using the SIGnature whistle IDentification method and categorized into 82 PSW types. Overall, PSWs were identified during 54% of encounters. Given the high production rate of stereotyped whistles (62% of all whistles in 90% of encounters) compared with the identified PSWs, we suggest that the SIGnature whistle IDentification method criteria cannot be fully adapted for the detection of SWs in Indo-Pacific humpback dolphins, and more research should be conducted to adapt the criteria to the species. In addition, the characteristics of PSWs differed slightly between locations, potentially because of the geographical separation of populations and habitat differences (e.g. noise levels). The present results confirm the production of stereotyped whistles, including PSWs, by Indo-Pacific humpback dolphins. Further research should be conducted to confirm whether these whistles are similar to bottlenose dolphins’ SWs.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"218 ","pages":"Pages 149-161"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142571463","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Navigating the night: effects of artificial light on the behaviour of Atlantic puffin fledglings 夜间导航:人造光对大西洋海雀雏鸟行为的影响
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-31 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.09.008
Taylor M. Brown , Sabina I. Wilhelm , Aaron D. Slepkov , Kaitlyn Baker , Gabriela F. Mastromonaco , Gary Burness
Every year in Newfoundland, young Atlantic puffins, Fratercula arctica, departing their nests at night for the first time become stranded in towns near their breeding colonies, a phenomenon thought to be caused by attraction towards artificial light. To test this hypothesis, we conducted three behavioural experiments. First, we illuminated beaches near a breeding colony to determine whether more fledglings would become stranded in illuminated versus dark conditions. Next, we conducted a Y-maze experiment to test stranded fledglings for phototactic behaviour in general and for preferences among high-pressure sodium (HPS), Warm white light-emitting diode (LED), Cool white LED, Blue LED and Orange LED light. Lastly, we quantified activity levels of stranded fledglings in an open field test during exposure to several different light types. We found significantly more fledglings stranded when beaches were illuminated, and fledglings significantly preferred light over darkness in the Y-maze, supporting our hypothesis that Atlantic puffin fledglings become stranded due to light attraction. Fledglings displayed no preferences for certain light types over others in the Y-maze, potentially suggesting that strandings in this species may not be mitigable by changing the streetlight type in stranding-prone towns. Interestingly, fledglings exhibited higher activity levels in darkness and HPS light than in LED light, potentially holding implications for rescue, rehabilitation and husbandry programmes. Overall, our findings demonstrate that the only evidence-based strategy for the reduction of Atlantic puffin strandings is the reduction of coastal artificial lighting; however, further research is needed to determine whether aspects of artificial light besides bulb type may be altered to effectively reduce light attraction in this species.
每年在纽芬兰,第一次在夜间离巢的大西洋海雀(Fratercula arctica)幼鸟都会在繁殖地附近的城镇搁浅,这种现象被认为是受到人造光的吸引所致。为了验证这一假设,我们进行了三个行为实验。首先,我们照亮了繁殖地附近的海滩,以确定在光照条件下与黑暗条件下是否会有更多的雏鸟搁浅。接着,我们进行了一个Y型迷宫实验,测试搁浅雏鸟的一般趋光行为,以及它们对高压钠灯(HPS)、暖白光发光二极管(LED)、冷白光发光二极管、蓝光发光二极管和橙光发光二极管的偏好。最后,我们对搁浅的雏鸟在暴露于几种不同光照类型时的活动水平进行了量化。我们发现,当海滩上有灯光时,搁浅的雏鸟数量明显增加,而且在Y型迷宫中,雏鸟明显偏好光而不是黑暗,这支持了我们的假设,即大西洋海雀雏鸟是由于光的吸引而搁浅的。在Y型迷宫中,雏海雀没有表现出对某些光线类型的偏好,这可能表明,在容易发生搁浅的城镇,改变路灯类型可能无法缓解该物种的搁浅。有趣的是,雏鸟在黑暗和 HPS 灯光下比在 LED 灯光下表现出更高的活动水平,这可能会对拯救、康复和饲养计划产生影响。总之,我们的研究结果表明,减少大西洋海雀搁浅的唯一有据可依的策略是减少沿海人工照明;然而,还需要进一步研究,以确定除了灯泡类型外,是否还可以改变人工照明的其他方面,以有效减少该物种的光吸引力。
{"title":"Navigating the night: effects of artificial light on the behaviour of Atlantic puffin fledglings","authors":"Taylor M. Brown ,&nbsp;Sabina I. Wilhelm ,&nbsp;Aaron D. Slepkov ,&nbsp;Kaitlyn Baker ,&nbsp;Gabriela F. Mastromonaco ,&nbsp;Gary Burness","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.09.008","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.09.008","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Every year in Newfoundland, young Atlantic puffins, <em>Fratercula arctica</em>, departing their nests at night for the first time become stranded in towns near their breeding colonies, a phenomenon thought to be caused by attraction towards artificial light. To test this hypothesis, we conducted three behavioural experiments. First, we illuminated beaches near a breeding colony to determine whether more fledglings would become stranded in illuminated versus dark conditions. Next, we conducted a Y-maze experiment to test stranded fledglings for phototactic behaviour in general and for preferences among high-pressure sodium (HPS), Warm white light-emitting diode (LED), Cool white LED, Blue LED and Orange LED light. Lastly, we quantified activity levels of stranded fledglings in an open field test during exposure to several different light types. We found significantly more fledglings stranded when beaches were illuminated, and fledglings significantly preferred light over darkness in the Y-maze, supporting our hypothesis that Atlantic puffin fledglings become stranded due to light attraction. Fledglings displayed no preferences for certain light types over others in the Y-maze, potentially suggesting that strandings in this species may not be mitigable by changing the streetlight type in stranding-prone towns. Interestingly, fledglings exhibited higher activity levels in darkness and HPS light than in LED light, potentially holding implications for rescue, rehabilitation and husbandry programmes. Overall, our findings demonstrate that the only evidence-based strategy for the reduction of Atlantic puffin strandings is the reduction of coastal artificial lighting; however, further research is needed to determine whether aspects of artificial light besides bulb type may be altered to effectively reduce light attraction in this species.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"218 ","pages":"Pages 135-148"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142553683","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evaluating indicators of wellbeing in captive grey mouse lemurs, Microcebus murinus, in the context of enrichment 评估圈养灰鼠狐猴(Microcebus murinus)在强化环境中的幸福指数
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-30 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.09.013
Juan Pablo Perea-Rodriguez , Julie Ferreira , Aude Anzeraey , Fabienne Aujard , Caroline Gilbert , Fabien Pifferi , Emmanuelle Pouydebat , Jérémy Terrien , Audrey Maille
Developing and implementing effective approaches to improve the welfare of animals participating in research requires reliable monitoring of their wellbeing. Since wellbeing is a multidimensional state that relates to an individual's physiological, behavioural and psychological/mental health, assessing it requires the use of measures that are both ecologically relevant and species-specific. Grey mouse lemurs are small, nocturnal, arboreal and omnivorous primates and a powerful model for fundamental and translational research. As such, it is important to study the factors that may help predict their wellbeing in captivity. Therefore, we monitored 12 adult mouse lemurs (six females and six males) housed with a same-sex weight-matched cage mate for 5 weeks under standard (Control) levels of environmental enrichment and 12 adults (six females and six males) under relatively High enrichment. We hypothesized that higher structural enrichment would stimulate metabolic energy expenditure, influencing how individuals would respond to a battery of behavioural and physiological measures, especially since their feeding rations remained unchanged throughout the study. We characterized circadian activity, judgement bias, predatory motivation, thermal reactivity to stress and changes in body composition. Generally, mouse lemurs under High enrichment showed more activity and increased predatory motivation and optimism than those in the Control group during daylight. Lemurs under High enrichment also showed increased responses during a stress reactivity test and higher losses in body and fat mass. Principal component analysis identified seven (out of nine) components that explained 96% of the variability in the data. Using a Naïve Bayes classifier algorithm trained with these new principal components, we classified the animals as either from High or Control enrichment levels with 100% accuracy. In summary, by measuring several relevant behavioural and physiological responses to environmental enrichment, we identified a multidimensional effect related to differences in welfare.
要制定和实施有效的方法来改善参与研究的动物的福利,就必须对其福利进行可靠的监测。由于福利是一种多维状态,与个体的生理、行为和心理/精神健康有关,因此评估福利需要使用与生态相关且针对特定物种的测量方法。灰鼠狐猴是一种小型、夜行、树栖和杂食灵长类动物,是基础研究和转化研究的有力模型。因此,研究有助于预测灰鼠狐猴在圈养条件下健康状况的因素非常重要。因此,我们对 12 只成年鼠狐猴(6 只雌性和 6 只雄性)进行了为期 5 周的监测,它们与同性体重匹配的笼伴一起生活在标准(控制)富集环境下,以及 12 只成年鼠狐猴(6 只雌性和 6 只雄性)生活在相对较高的富集环境下。我们假设,较高的结构富集度会刺激新陈代谢能量消耗,影响个体对一系列行为和生理指标的反应,尤其是在整个研究过程中它们的喂食量保持不变的情况下。我们对昼夜节律活动、判断偏差、捕食动机、对压力的热反应以及身体成分的变化进行了描述。一般来说,与对照组相比,高度富集下的狐猴在白天表现出更多的活动、更强的捕食动机和更乐观的情绪。高度富集条件下的狐猴在应激反应测试中也表现出更高的反应,身体和脂肪质量的损失也更大。主成分分析确定了七个(共九个)成分,解释了数据中 96% 的变异性。通过使用用这些新主成分训练的奈夫贝叶斯分类器算法,我们将动物分为 "高 "或 "控制 "富集水平,准确率达到 100%。总之,通过测量动物对环境富集的几种相关行为和生理反应,我们确定了与福利差异相关的多维效应。
{"title":"Evaluating indicators of wellbeing in captive grey mouse lemurs, Microcebus murinus, in the context of enrichment","authors":"Juan Pablo Perea-Rodriguez ,&nbsp;Julie Ferreira ,&nbsp;Aude Anzeraey ,&nbsp;Fabienne Aujard ,&nbsp;Caroline Gilbert ,&nbsp;Fabien Pifferi ,&nbsp;Emmanuelle Pouydebat ,&nbsp;Jérémy Terrien ,&nbsp;Audrey Maille","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.09.013","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.09.013","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Developing and implementing effective approaches to improve the welfare of animals participating in research requires reliable monitoring of their wellbeing. Since wellbeing is a multidimensional state that relates to an individual's physiological, behavioural and psychological/mental health, assessing it requires the use of measures that are both ecologically relevant and species-specific. Grey mouse lemurs are small, nocturnal, arboreal and omnivorous primates and a powerful model for fundamental and translational research. As such, it is important to study the factors that may help predict their wellbeing in captivity. Therefore, we monitored 12 adult mouse lemurs (six females and six males) housed with a same-sex weight-matched cage mate for 5 weeks under standard (Control) levels of environmental enrichment and 12 adults (six females and six males) under relatively High enrichment. We hypothesized that higher structural enrichment would stimulate metabolic energy expenditure, influencing how individuals would respond to a battery of behavioural and physiological measures, especially since their feeding rations remained unchanged throughout the study. We characterized circadian activity, judgement bias, predatory motivation, thermal reactivity to stress and changes in body composition. Generally, mouse lemurs under High enrichment showed more activity and increased predatory motivation and optimism than those in the Control group during daylight. Lemurs under High enrichment also showed increased responses during a stress reactivity test and higher losses in body and fat mass. Principal component analysis identified seven (out of nine) components that explained 96% of the variability in the data. Using a Naïve Bayes classifier algorithm trained with these new principal components, we classified the animals as either from High or Control enrichment levels with 100% accuracy. In summary, by measuring several relevant behavioural and physiological responses to environmental enrichment, we identified a multidimensional effect related to differences in welfare.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"218 ","pages":"Pages 121-134"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142553682","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social tension in the aftermath of public conflicts: an ethological analysis in humans 公共冲突后的社会紧张局势:对人类的伦理学分析
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-30 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.003
Virginia Pallante , Ivan Norscia , Marie Rosenkrantz Lindegaard
In social mammals, conflicts are stressful events for the individuals involved. In the postconflict context, it is possible to detect the emotional state of the former opponents through the expression of displacement activities and aggressive behaviours, which indicate an increase in social tension. In humans, stressful events also induce a physiological response that leads to increased social tension behaviours. However, the variation of such behaviours in the postconflict context has never been investigated. Therefore, by conducting a video analysis of street fights recorded by closed-circuit television cameras, we explored the variation in behaviours associated with anxiety, aggression-related anger and other behaviours possibly related to both anxiety and anger (body postures and talking with gestures) in human opponents. We compared the expression of social tension behaviours before and after the eruption of the conflict and found that displacement activities (related to anxiety), aggressive behaviours (related to anger) and talking with gestures (possibly related to anxiety/anger) increased in the postconflict context, but body postures remained unchanged. Moreover, displacement activities and aggressive patterns showed a temporal variation, decreasing within 10 min following the conflict. Finally, the occurrence of anxiety-related behaviours was more sensitive to aggression intensity than anger-related behaviours, indicating that different social tension behaviours rely on different responses that might be separable. Our study highlights the importance of the ethological approach for evaluating postconflict social tension in humans, which shows a variation in its expression as observed in nonhuman primates. Following a similar comparative approach, we encourage further studies to explore the role of social tension in altering postconflict social dynamics.
在社会性哺乳动物中,冲突对参与冲突的个体来说是一种压力事件。在冲突后的环境中,可以通过流离失所活动和攻击行为的表现来检测前对手的情绪状态,这表明社会紧张度增加了。在人类中,压力事件也会引起生理反应,导致社会紧张行为的增加。然而,这种行为在冲突后环境中的变化却从未被研究过。因此,我们通过对闭路电视摄像机录制的街头斗殴视频进行分析,探讨了人类对手中与焦虑、攻击相关的愤怒以及其他可能与焦虑和愤怒相关的行为(身体姿势和说话手势)有关的行为变化。我们对冲突爆发前后的社会紧张行为表现进行了比较,发现在冲突后的环境中,位移活动(与焦虑有关)、攻击行为(与愤怒有关)和用手势说话(可能与焦虑/愤怒有关)有所增加,但身体姿势保持不变。此外,位移活动和攻击性行为模式呈现出时间上的变化,在冲突后 10 分钟内有所减少。最后,焦虑相关行为比愤怒相关行为对攻击强度更敏感,这表明不同的社会紧张行为依赖于不同的反应,而这些反应可能是可分离的。我们的研究凸显了用伦理学方法评估人类冲突后社会紧张关系的重要性,这种方法与在非人灵长类动物身上观察到的社会紧张关系表现形式不同。我们鼓励采用类似的比较方法,进一步研究社会紧张在改变冲突后社会动态中的作用。
{"title":"Social tension in the aftermath of public conflicts: an ethological analysis in humans","authors":"Virginia Pallante ,&nbsp;Ivan Norscia ,&nbsp;Marie Rosenkrantz Lindegaard","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.003","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.10.003","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>In social mammals, conflicts are stressful events for the individuals involved. In the postconflict context, it is possible to detect the emotional state of the former opponents through the expression of displacement activities and aggressive behaviours, which indicate an increase in social tension. In humans, stressful events also induce a physiological response that leads to increased social tension behaviours. However, the variation of such behaviours in the postconflict context has never been investigated. Therefore, by conducting a video analysis of street fights recorded by closed-circuit television cameras, we explored the variation in behaviours associated with anxiety, aggression-related anger and other behaviours possibly related to both anxiety and anger (body postures and talking with gestures) in human opponents. We compared the expression of social tension behaviours before and after the eruption of the conflict and found that displacement activities (related to anxiety), aggressive behaviours (related to anger) and talking with gestures (possibly related to anxiety/anger) increased in the postconflict context, but body postures remained unchanged. Moreover, displacement activities and aggressive patterns showed a temporal variation, decreasing within 10 min following the conflict. Finally, the occurrence of anxiety-related behaviours was more sensitive to aggression intensity than anger-related behaviours, indicating that different social tension behaviours rely on different responses that might be separable. Our study highlights the importance of the ethological approach for evaluating postconflict social tension in humans, which shows a variation in its expression as observed in nonhuman primates. Following a similar comparative approach, we encourage further studies to explore the role of social tension in altering postconflict social dynamics.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"218 ","pages":"Pages 109-120"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142554372","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Within-cavity nest preferences: honey bees favour the entrance rather than the ceiling when initiating their nest 洞内筑巢偏好:蜜蜂开始筑巢时偏爱入口而非天花板
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-29 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.09.005
Peter R. Marting , Claire S. Bailey , Michael L. Smith
Cavity-nesting animals are limited by the options available in the environment and thus may have to adapt their nest structure to optimize its function. After choosing a cavity, honey bee colonies must decide where to initiate their comb nest within the empty cavity. Within-cavity location preferences remain untested, but previous studies suggest proximity to the ceiling, entrance and east side may be important. To distinguish potential preferences for nest initiation sites, we designed ‘infinity nest’ hive boxes that were extremely elongated (2.4 m), with target features at opposite ends, thus forcing colonies to choose between the entrance, the east side and the ceiling. Not only did colonies show a clear preference for nesting near the entrance (89%; N = 9), but when the entrance was far from the ceiling, colonies modified their nest by building combs from vertical walls. Combs that were attached to walls were more vertically elongated than combs attached to horizontal ceilings, which likely reduces the straining forces from gravity and shows that bees can alter their entire nest structure to accommodate different attachment angles and initiation site preferences. Therefore, honey bees are not constrained to attaching their comb to the cavity's ceiling and instead pursue other important initiation site features, like proximity to the cavity entrance, which may reduce commute time. Given that colonies reuse their nest for multiple worker generations, the decision of where to initiate the nest within a cavity likely has long-term impacts for colony function. This work demonstrates the importance of plasticity in built structures and the extended phenotype.
巢穴动物受到环境选择的限制,因此可能不得不调整巢穴结构以优化其功能。蜜蜂群在选择了一个巢穴后,必须决定在空巢穴内的哪个位置开始筑巢。蜂群对巢腔内位置的偏好尚未得到验证,但先前的研究表明,靠近天花板、入口和东侧的位置可能很重要。为了区分蜂群对筑巢地点的潜在偏好,我们设计了 "无穷大巢 "蜂箱,蜂箱极度拉长(2.4 米),两端各有一个目标特征,从而迫使蜂群在入口、东侧和天花板之间做出选择。不仅蜂群明显偏好在入口附近筑巢(89%;N = 9),而且当入口远离天花板时,蜂群会通过在垂直墙壁上建造蜂巢来改变其巢穴。与附着在水平天花板上的蜂巢相比,附着在墙壁上的蜂巢更加垂直细长,这很可能减少了重力带来的拉力,也表明蜜蜂可以改变整个巢穴结构,以适应不同的附着角度和启动地点偏好。因此,蜜蜂并不局限于将梳子固定在巢穴的天花板上,而是追求其他重要的起始点特征,如靠近巢穴入口,这可能会减少往返时间。鉴于蜂群会重复使用其巢穴供多代工蜂使用,决定在巢穴内的哪个位置开始筑巢很可能会对蜂群功能产生长期影响。这项工作证明了建筑结构的可塑性和扩展表型的重要性。
{"title":"Within-cavity nest preferences: honey bees favour the entrance rather than the ceiling when initiating their nest","authors":"Peter R. Marting ,&nbsp;Claire S. Bailey ,&nbsp;Michael L. Smith","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.09.005","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.09.005","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Cavity-nesting animals are limited by the options available in the environment and thus may have to adapt their nest structure to optimize its function. After choosing a cavity, honey bee colonies must decide where to initiate their comb nest within the empty cavity. Within-cavity location preferences remain untested, but previous studies suggest proximity to the ceiling, entrance and east side may be important. To distinguish potential preferences for nest initiation sites, we designed ‘infinity nest’ hive boxes that were extremely elongated (2.4<!--> <!-->m), with target features at opposite ends, thus forcing colonies to choose between the entrance, the east side and the ceiling. Not only did colonies show a clear preference for nesting near the entrance (89%; <em>N</em> <!-->=<!--> <!-->9), but when the entrance was far from the ceiling, colonies modified their nest by building combs from vertical walls. Combs that were attached to walls were more vertically elongated than combs attached to horizontal ceilings, which likely reduces the straining forces from gravity and shows that bees can alter their entire nest structure to accommodate different attachment angles and initiation site preferences. Therefore, honey bees are not constrained to attaching their comb to the cavity's ceiling and instead pursue other important initiation site features, like proximity to the cavity entrance, which may reduce commute time. Given that colonies reuse their nest for multiple worker generations, the decision of where to initiate the nest within a cavity likely has long-term impacts for colony function. This work demonstrates the importance of plasticity in built structures and the extended phenotype.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"218 ","pages":"Pages 255-261"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142700114","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social information use for spatial decision in the common lizard, Zootoca vivipara 普通蜥蜴 Zootoca vivipara 利用社会信息进行空间决策的情况
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.09.014
Mathieu Brevet , Staffan Jacob , Alexis Rutschmann , Murielle Richard , Julien Cote , Jean Clobert
Individuals' movements are conditioned by the acquisition of information from personal interactions with the environment or from social sources. Despite the importance of social information in movement decision making, little is known about how individuals proceed when social information comes from multiple sources. Here, we specifically tackled this issue by experimentally testing (1) how social information from multiple sources is used to make relocation decisions and (2) whether a contrast in this information enables individuals to orient themselves in space. Using the common lizard as a model species, we conducted repeated experiments in which a focal neonate received information from two other neonates coming from distinct peripheral environments before being given the opportunity to relocate to either peripheral environment. Specifically, we focused on information on resource availability and intraspecific competition by considering informants' body mass (BM) at birth and their subsequent food intake. Our analyses revealed that the amount of resources in the informants' environments affected relocation decisions, depending on the focal individual's phenotype. We found the probability of relocation increased when both the informants' food intake and the focal individual's BM increased or decreased. We also found the relocation increased when both the informants' and focal individual's BM increased or decreased. Our findings suggest that focal individuals adjust their relocation response to social information according to their needs and/or physical abilities. Conversely, we found no significant effect of differences in BM between informants or spatial variability in resource availability on spatial orientation. This study highlights that multiple sources of social information that reflect the quality of the surrounding environment (competition or resource availability) can be used for movement decision making.
个体的运动受制于从个人与环境的互动或社会来源中获取的信息。尽管社会信息在迁移决策中非常重要,但人们对个体如何利用来自多个来源的社会信息却知之甚少。在这里,我们通过实验测试了(1)如何利用来自多个来源的社会信息来做出迁移决策,以及(2)这些信息的对比是否能使个体在空间中定位。我们以普通蜥蜴为模型物种,进行了重复实验,在实验中,一个焦点新生个体在有机会搬迁到任何一个外围环境之前,会接收来自其他两个不同外围环境的新生个体的信息。具体来说,我们通过考虑信息提供者出生时的体重(BM)及其随后的食物摄入量,重点研究了有关资源可用性和种内竞争的信息。我们的分析表明,信息提供者所处环境中的资源量会影响迁移决定,这取决于焦点个体的表型。我们发现,当信息提供者的食物摄入量和焦点个体的体重都增加或减少时,迁移的概率就会增加。我们还发现,当信息提供者和病灶个体的生物量都增加或减少时,迁移的概率也会增加。我们的研究结果表明,焦点个体会根据自己的需要和/或体能调整对社会信息的迁移反应。相反,我们发现信息提供者之间的 BM 差异或资源可用性的空间变化对空间定向没有明显影响。这项研究强调,反映周围环境质量(竞争或资源可用性)的多种社会信息来源可用于迁移决策。
{"title":"Social information use for spatial decision in the common lizard, Zootoca vivipara","authors":"Mathieu Brevet ,&nbsp;Staffan Jacob ,&nbsp;Alexis Rutschmann ,&nbsp;Murielle Richard ,&nbsp;Julien Cote ,&nbsp;Jean Clobert","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.09.014","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.09.014","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Individuals' movements are conditioned by the acquisition of information from personal interactions with the environment or from social sources. Despite the importance of social information in movement decision making, little is known about how individuals proceed when social information comes from multiple sources. Here, we specifically tackled this issue by experimentally testing (1) how social information from multiple sources is used to make relocation decisions and (2) whether a contrast in this information enables individuals to orient themselves in space. Using the common lizard as a model species, we conducted repeated experiments in which a focal neonate received information from two other neonates coming from distinct peripheral environments before being given the opportunity to relocate to either peripheral environment. Specifically, we focused on information on resource availability and intraspecific competition by considering informants' body mass (BM) at birth and their subsequent food intake. Our analyses revealed that the amount of resources in the informants' environments affected relocation decisions, depending on the focal individual's phenotype. We found the probability of relocation increased when both the informants' food intake and the focal individual's BM increased or decreased. We also found the relocation increased when both the informants' and focal individual's BM increased or decreased. Our findings suggest that focal individuals adjust their relocation response to social information according to their needs and/or physical abilities. Conversely, we found no significant effect of differences in BM between informants or spatial variability in resource availability on spatial orientation. This study highlights that multiple sources of social information that reflect the quality of the surrounding environment (competition or resource availability) can be used for movement decision making.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"218 ","pages":"Pages 95-108"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142534674","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Animal Behaviour
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1