首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Development最新文献

英文 中文
Promoting children’s mathematical and statistical understanding through parent-child math games 通过亲子数学游戏促进儿童对数学和统计的理解
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101480
Mary DePascale , Geetha B. Ramani

Basic statistical literacy is essential for interpreting external sources and developing critical thinking skills necessary for engagement in real-world contexts. However, many children and adults struggle with understanding and interpreting data. Therefore, it is critical to develop engaging, effective methods for teaching early data analysis, as they could enhance children’s statistical understanding, math, and higher-order thinking skills. We examined the effectiveness of a home-based, experimental game intervention for children’s (ages 5–6, 50 % female, 67 % white, 12 % Asian, 8 % biracial) statistical understanding and math skills. Families (majority high household income and parent education) were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: graphing board game, graphing card game, or literacy board game. Children in the graphing conditions improved on statistical understanding and arithmetic, and children in the literacy condition did not. These results support the development of play-based materials to promote early mathematical and statistical skills, with implications for children’s mathematical development.

基本的统计素养对于解释外部信息来源和培养参与现实世界所需的批判性思维能力至关重要。然而,许多儿童和成人在理解和解释数据方面都有困难。因此,开发吸引人的、有效的早期数据分析教学方法至关重要,因为这些方法可以提高儿童的统计理解、数学和高阶思维能力。我们研究了基于家庭的实验性游戏干预对儿童(5-6 岁,50% 女性,67% 白人,12% 亚洲人,8% 双种族)统计理解和数学技能的有效性。这些家庭(大多数家庭收入较高,父母受教育程度较高)被随机分配到三种条件中的一种:图形棋盘游戏、图形卡片游戏或识字棋盘游戏。图形游戏条件下的儿童在统计理解和算术方面有所提高,而识字游戏条件下的儿童则没有提高。这些结果支持开发以游戏为基础的材料,以促进早期数学和统计技能,并对儿童的数学发展产生影响。
{"title":"Promoting children’s mathematical and statistical understanding through parent-child math games","authors":"Mary DePascale ,&nbsp;Geetha B. Ramani","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101480","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101480","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Basic statistical literacy is essential for interpreting external sources and developing critical thinking skills necessary for engagement in real-world contexts. However, many children and adults struggle with understanding and interpreting data. Therefore, it is critical to develop engaging, effective methods for teaching early data analysis, as they could enhance children’s statistical understanding, math, and higher-order thinking skills. We examined the effectiveness of a home-based, experimental game intervention for children’s (ages 5–6, 50 % female, 67 % white, 12 % Asian, 8 % biracial) statistical understanding and math skills. Families (majority high household income and parent education) were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: graphing board game, graphing card game, or literacy board game. Children in the graphing conditions improved on statistical understanding and arithmetic, and children in the literacy condition did not. These results support the development of play-based materials to promote early mathematical and statistical skills, with implications for children’s mathematical development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"71 ","pages":"Article 101480"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141592904","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of foreigner talk on children’s evaluations of addressees 外国人的谈话对儿童评价对象的影响
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101486
Danielle Labotka, Susan A. Gelman

Children evaluate others based on how they speak, but do children evaluate others based on how they are spoken to? We examined how U.S. children and adults (N = 170 5- to 10-year-olds, 49 % female; 107 adults; in a city with a foreign population of 17.9 %) evaluated addressees of Foreigner Talk (i.e., slow, loud, simplified speech). In Study 1, children and adults evaluated Foreigner Talk addressees more negatively than Peer Talk or Teacher Talk addressees. In Study 2, adults and older children incorporated Foreigner Talk with additional contextual cues to inform their evaluations: a local peer receiving Foreigner Talk received lower evaluations than a foreign peer receiving Foreigner Talk. With medium to large effect sizes, these studies indicate the importance of speech register in children’s social inferences.

儿童是根据别人说话的方式来评价别人的,但儿童是否根据别人对他们说话的方式来评价别人呢?我们研究了美国儿童和成人(N = 170 名 5-10 岁儿童,49% 为女性;107 名成人;所在城市的外国人口比例为 17.9%)如何评价 "老外说话"(即语速慢、声音大、语言简化)的对象。在研究 1 中,儿童和成人对 "老外 "的评价比对 "同伴 "或 "老师 "的评价更负面。在研究 2 中,成人和年龄较大的儿童在对外国人说话进行评价时,会结合额外的语境线索:接受外国人说话的本地同伴得到的评价低于接受外国人说话的外国同伴得到的评价。这些研究以中等到较大的效应大小表明了语域在儿童社会推断中的重要性。
{"title":"The effect of foreigner talk on children’s evaluations of addressees","authors":"Danielle Labotka,&nbsp;Susan A. Gelman","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101486","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101486","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Children evaluate others based on how they speak, but do children evaluate others based on how they are spoken to? We examined how U.S. children and adults (<em>N</em> = 170 5- to 10-year-olds, 49 % female; 107 adults; in a city with a foreign population of 17.9 %) evaluated addressees of Foreigner Talk (i.e., slow, loud, simplified speech). In Study 1, children and adults evaluated Foreigner Talk addressees more negatively than Peer Talk or Teacher Talk addressees. In Study 2, adults and older children incorporated Foreigner Talk with additional contextual cues to inform their evaluations: a local peer receiving Foreigner Talk received lower evaluations than a foreign peer receiving Foreigner Talk. With medium to large effect sizes, these studies indicate the importance of speech register in children’s social inferences.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"71 ","pages":"Article 101486"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141728996","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Optimizing adolescent health: Investigating physical activity’s impact on fitness, working memory, and academic performance 优化青少年健康:调查体育活动对体能、工作记忆和学习成绩的影响
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101478
Pei-Ju Kang , Hsu-Chan Kuo

This quasi-experimental study investigates the intricate relationship among adolescents’ physical activity, working memory, and academic performance, recognizing their significance in adolescent development. Employing a quasi-experimental design, 85 13-year-olds from a Taiwanese junior high school were involved. Data scrutiny was facilitated through Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA), with the mediation effect explored via Smart Partial Least Squares structural equation modeling (Smart PLS-SEM). Findings indicate significant improvements in physical fitness among both aerobic and resistance exercise groups compared to controls. Additionally, both exercise cohorts demonstrated a stronger positive association with verbal working memory scores than the control group. Intriguingly, visuospatial working memory fully mediated the relationship between resistance exercise and science performance. These results highlight the potential advantages of incorporating brief daily exercise sessions to bolster adolescent physical fitness and emphasize the mediating function of working memory in connecting physical activity with academic accomplishment. This study furnishes valuable insights for educators and policymakers striving to enhance adolescent well-being and academic achievement through targeted interventions.

本准实验研究探讨了青少年体育锻炼、工作记忆和学习成绩之间错综复杂的关系,认识到它们在青少年成长过程中的重要意义。研究采用准实验设计,共有 85 名来自台湾一所初中的 13 岁学生参与。通过协方差分析(ANCOVA)对数据进行了仔细分析,并通过智能偏最小二乘法结构方程模型(Smart PLS-SEM)探讨了中介效应。研究结果表明,与对照组相比,有氧运动组和阻力运动组的体能均有明显改善。此外,与对照组相比,两组锻炼者的言语工作记忆得分都有更强的正相关性。耐人寻味的是,视觉空间工作记忆完全调节了阻力运动与科学成绩之间的关系。这些结果凸显了每天进行简短锻炼以增强青少年体质的潜在优势,并强调了工作记忆在体育锻炼与学习成绩之间的中介作用。这项研究为教育工作者和政策制定者提供了宝贵的见解,他们正努力通过有针对性的干预措施来提高青少年的健康水平和学业成绩。
{"title":"Optimizing adolescent health: Investigating physical activity’s impact on fitness, working memory, and academic performance","authors":"Pei-Ju Kang ,&nbsp;Hsu-Chan Kuo","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101478","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101478","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This quasi-experimental study investigates the intricate relationship among adolescents’ physical activity, working memory, and academic performance, recognizing their significance in adolescent development. Employing a quasi-experimental design, 85 13-year-olds from a Taiwanese junior high school were involved. Data scrutiny was facilitated through Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA), with the mediation effect explored via Smart Partial Least Squares structural equation modeling (Smart PLS-SEM). Findings indicate significant improvements in physical fitness among both aerobic and resistance exercise groups compared to controls. Additionally, both exercise cohorts demonstrated a stronger positive association with verbal working memory scores than the control group. Intriguingly, visuospatial working memory fully mediated the relationship between resistance exercise and science performance. These results highlight the potential advantages of incorporating brief daily exercise sessions to bolster adolescent physical fitness and emphasize the mediating function of working memory in connecting physical activity with academic accomplishment. This study furnishes valuable insights for educators and policymakers striving to enhance adolescent well-being and academic achievement through targeted interventions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"71 ","pages":"Article 101478"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141540745","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of early picture book interaction experience on Chinese character reading of children with Dyslexia: The perspective of visual temporal processing 早期绘本互动经验对阅读障碍儿童汉字阅读的影响:视觉时空处理的视角
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101477
Hung-Ju Tsai , Ji-Kang Chen , Li-Chih Wang

The present study investigated the moderation effect of visual temporal processing among the relationship between early picture book interaction experience and Chinese character reading of Chinese children with and without dyslexia. In total 118 Chinese children aged 8 to 10 years old were recruited from Taiwan. Half of them were typical learners, while the other half were identified as having dyslexia. We found children with typically developing showed significantly higher levels of early picture book interaction experience and visual temporal processing compared to those with dyslexia. Also, two groups of participants showed distinct profiles of visual temporal processing, early picture book interaction experience, and Chinese character reading. For typically developing students, visual temporal processing significantly contributed to Chinese character reading. In contrast, for students with dyslexia, the interaction between visual temporal processing and early picture book interaction experience was a significant predictor, and early picture book interaction experience’s significant contribution to Chinese character reading was only found for those who had better visual temporal processing.

本研究探讨了视觉时间加工对有阅读障碍和无阅读障碍的中国儿童早期绘本互动经验与汉字阅读之间关系的调节作用。本研究从台湾招募了 118 名 8 至 10 岁的中国儿童。其中一半为典型学习者,另一半被确认为有阅读障碍。我们发现,与有阅读障碍的儿童相比,典型发展期儿童在早期绘本互动经验和视觉时空处理方面的水平明显更高。此外,两组参与者在视觉时间处理、早期绘本互动经验和汉字阅读方面也表现出不同的特征。对于典型发展型学生来说,视觉时间处理对汉字阅读有明显的促进作用。相反,对于有阅读障碍的学生来说,视觉时间处理和早期绘本互动经验之间的交互作用是一个重要的预测因素,只有那些视觉时间处理较好的学生才会发现早期绘本互动经验对汉字阅读有显著的促进作用。
{"title":"The impact of early picture book interaction experience on Chinese character reading of children with Dyslexia: The perspective of visual temporal processing","authors":"Hung-Ju Tsai ,&nbsp;Ji-Kang Chen ,&nbsp;Li-Chih Wang","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101477","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101477","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The present study investigated the moderation effect of visual temporal processing among the relationship between early picture book interaction experience and Chinese character reading of Chinese children with and without dyslexia. In total 118 Chinese children aged 8 to 10 years old were recruited from Taiwan. Half of them were typical learners, while the other half were identified as having dyslexia. We found children with typically developing showed significantly higher levels of early picture book interaction experience and visual temporal processing compared to those with dyslexia. Also, two groups of participants showed distinct profiles of visual temporal processing, early picture book interaction experience, and Chinese character reading. For typically developing students, visual temporal processing significantly contributed to Chinese character reading. In contrast, for students with dyslexia, the interaction between visual temporal processing and early picture book interaction experience was a significant predictor, and early picture book interaction experience’s significant contribution to Chinese character reading was only found for those who had better visual temporal processing.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"71 ","pages":"Article 101477"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141422935","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children’s expectations of selective informing: The role of informational relevance on group membership based informing 儿童对选择性告知的期望:信息相关性对基于群体成员身份的信息提供的作用
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101472
Sunae Kim , Mariwan I. Arif

Surprisingly little is known about how informational relevance guides children’s informing decisions. Although prior studies have demonstrated that children selectively inform and teach others these studies do not directly address whether children consider informational relevance specific to an outgroup member. We also know that children by age 5 and 6 show robust preferences for their ingroup members in various decisions but does information relevance modulate their ingroup preferences? In three experiments (N = 180), we investigated whether Iraqi Kurdish 6-year-old children expect others to inform an ingroup member or an outgroup member, depending on the informational relevance. In Experiment 1 children expected others to inform an ingroup member rather than an outgroup member irrespective of information type – extending prior work on ingroup preferences. In experiments 2 and 3, in which the relevance of the information to an outgroup member was highlighted, children’s expectation about informing an ingroup member was modulated by information type. Together, the findings suggest that children consider informational relevance to guide their expectations about others’ selective informing in the context of group membership, which could further explain how cultural knowledge is maintained and reinforced among members of the same cultural group.

令人惊讶的是,人们对信息相关性如何引导儿童做出提供信息的决定知之甚少。尽管之前的研究已经证明,儿童会有选择性地向他人提供信息并教导他人,但这些研究并没有直接探讨儿童是否会考虑到信息相关性对外群成员的具体影响。我们还知道,5 到 6 岁的儿童在各种决策中都会表现出强烈的内群体成员偏好,但信息相关性是否会调节他们的内群体偏好呢?在三项实验(N = 180)中,我们调查了伊拉克库尔德 6 岁儿童是期望他人告知内群成员还是外群成员,这取决于信息相关性。在实验 1 中,无论信息类型如何,儿童都希望他人告知内群体成员而非外群体成员--这是对先前有关内群体偏好研究的延伸。在实验 2 和实验 3 中,当信息与外群体成员的相关性被强调时,儿童对通知内群体成员的期望会受到信息类型的影响。总之,这些研究结果表明,儿童会考虑信息的相关性,以指导他们对他人在群体成员身份背景下有选择性地提供信息的预期,这可以进一步解释文化知识是如何在同一文化群体成员之间得到维持和强化的。
{"title":"Children’s expectations of selective informing: The role of informational relevance on group membership based informing","authors":"Sunae Kim ,&nbsp;Mariwan I. Arif","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101472","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101472","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Surprisingly little is known about how informational relevance guides children’s informing decisions. Although prior studies have demonstrated that children selectively inform and teach others these studies do not directly address whether children consider informational relevance specific to an outgroup member. We also know that children by age 5 and 6 show robust preferences for their ingroup members in various decisions but does information relevance modulate their ingroup preferences? In three experiments (<em>N</em> = 180), we investigated whether Iraqi Kurdish 6-year-old children expect others to inform an ingroup member or an outgroup member, depending on the informational relevance. In Experiment 1 children expected others to inform an ingroup member rather than an outgroup member irrespective of information type – extending prior work on ingroup preferences. In experiments 2 and 3, in which the relevance of the information to an outgroup member was highlighted, children’s expectation about informing an ingroup member was modulated by information type. Together, the findings suggest that children consider informational relevance to guide their expectations about others’ selective informing in the context of group membership, which could further explain how cultural knowledge is maintained and reinforced among members of the same cultural group.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"71 ","pages":"Article 101472"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000571/pdfft?md5=88729a5a48d6b4836ed47f698e53a3be&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000571-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141313391","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can Serena Williams’s tennis racquet make me a better tennis player? Beliefs about Ability Contagion in Children and Adults 塞雷娜-威廉姆斯的网球拍能让我成为更好的网球运动员吗?儿童和成人对能力传染的信念
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-12 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101461
Kristan A. Marchak , Marianne Turgeon , Merranda McLaughlin , Susan A. Gelman

We examined U.S. and Canadian children’s (n = 214) and adults’ (n = 72) belief in ability contagion – an expected improvement in performance when using a celebrity’s object (e.g., superior tennis skills when using Serena Williams’s racquet). Four- to 7-year-olds preferred using a celebrity to a non-celebrity object, but their performance on a related task did not differ depending on which object they used (Study 1). Adults and 5- to 8-year-olds expected that a celebrity object would lead to superior performance in a forced-choice paradigm (Study 2), but not when given the option to state that the objects were the same (Study 3), even though adults and older children (> 7.01 years) judged the celebrity object to have an enhanced worth. We find that participants do not believe in ability contagion using either implicit or explicit measures. We discuss implications of our results for versions of contagion accounts of celebrity objects.

我们研究了美国和加拿大儿童(n = 214)和成人(n = 72)对能力传染的信念--即使用名人的物品(例如,使用塞雷娜-威廉姆斯的球拍就能提高网球技术)时,预期成绩会有所提高。4 至 7 岁的儿童更喜欢使用名人的物品,而不是非名人的物品,但他们在相关任务中的表现并不因使用哪种物品而有所不同(研究 1)。成人和 5 至 8 岁的儿童都认为名人物品会使他们在强迫选择范式中取得更好的成绩(研究 2),但如果让他们选择说这两个物品是一样的(研究 3),他们就不这么认为了,尽管成人和年龄较大的儿童(> 7.01 岁)都认为名人物品具有更高的价值。我们发现,无论是采用内隐测量还是外显测量,参与者都不相信能力传染。我们将讨论我们的结果对名人对象传染说法版本的影响。
{"title":"Can Serena Williams’s tennis racquet make me a better tennis player? Beliefs about Ability Contagion in Children and Adults","authors":"Kristan A. Marchak ,&nbsp;Marianne Turgeon ,&nbsp;Merranda McLaughlin ,&nbsp;Susan A. Gelman","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101461","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101461","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We examined U.S. and Canadian children’s (<em>n</em> = 214) and adults’ (<em>n</em> = 72) belief in <em>ability contagion</em> – an expected improvement in performance when using a celebrity’s object (e.g., superior tennis skills when using Serena Williams’s racquet). Four- to 7-year-olds preferred using a celebrity to a non-celebrity object, but their performance on a related task did not differ depending on which object they used (Study 1). Adults and 5- to 8-year-olds expected that a celebrity object would lead to superior performance in a forced-choice paradigm (Study 2), but not when given the option to state that the objects were the same (Study 3), even though adults and older children (&gt; 7.01 years) judged the celebrity object to have an enhanced worth. We find that participants do not believe in ability contagion using either implicit or explicit measures. We discuss implications of our results for versions of contagion accounts of celebrity objects.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"71 ","pages":"Article 101461"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000467/pdfft?md5=060dfaee6cb2043a079792c65a7975af&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000467-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141314673","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Young children’s copying of block constructions: Significant constraints in a highly complex task 幼儿对积木结构的模仿:高度复杂任务中的重要制约因素
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101463
Barbara Landau, E. Emory Davis, Cathryn S. Cortesa, Zihan Wang, Jonathan D. Jones, Amy L. Shelton

Block construction is ubiquitous in early development, yet is surprisingly complex, involving step-by-step sequenced actions to create specific structures. Here, we use novel analytic methods to characterize these action sequences in detail, including which individual parts of the structure (‘states’) are built and how these structures are combined, creating a fully specified build path towards the final structure. We find that, like adults tested in a previous study, 4- to 8-year-olds build by creating a small subset of possible individual states and full build paths, and that they prioritize building layer-by-layer. The individual states and build paths that children produce are strikingly similar to those of adults, resulting in structures that are more stable than other possible (but not attested) states and paths. Our approach serves as a lens into the cognitive processes underlying block building and suggests that children’s building is guided by significant cognitive constraints consistent with “computational thinking”.

区块构建在早期发育过程中无处不在,但其复杂程度却令人吃惊,它涉及一步步按顺序创建特定结构的操作。在这里,我们使用新颖的分析方法详细描述了这些动作序列的特征,包括结构的各个部分("状态")是如何构建的,以及这些结构是如何组合在一起的,从而创造出一条通向最终结构的完全指定的构建路径。我们发现,与之前研究中测试的成人一样,4 到 8 岁的儿童也会通过创建一小部分可能的单个状态和完整的构建路径来进行构建,而且他们会优先考虑逐层构建。儿童创造的单个状态和构建路径与成人惊人地相似,其结构比其他可能的(但未经证实的)状态和路径更加稳定。我们的研究方法是了解积木搭建认知过程的一个视角,它表明儿童的搭建过程受到与 "计算思维 "一致的重要认知限制的指导。
{"title":"Young children’s copying of block constructions: Significant constraints in a highly complex task","authors":"Barbara Landau,&nbsp;E. Emory Davis,&nbsp;Cathryn S. Cortesa,&nbsp;Zihan Wang,&nbsp;Jonathan D. Jones,&nbsp;Amy L. Shelton","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101463","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101463","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Block construction is ubiquitous in early development, yet is surprisingly complex, involving step-by-step sequenced actions to create specific structures. Here, we use novel analytic methods to characterize these action sequences in detail, including which individual parts of the structure (‘states’) are built and how these structures are combined, creating a fully specified build path towards the final structure. We find that, like adults tested in a previous study, 4- to 8-year-olds build by creating a small subset of <em>possible</em> individual states and full build paths, and that they prioritize building layer-by-layer. The individual states and build paths that children produce are strikingly similar to those of adults, resulting in structures that are more stable than other possible (but not attested) states and paths. Our approach serves as a lens into the cognitive processes underlying block building and suggests that children’s building is guided by significant cognitive constraints consistent with “computational thinking”.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"71 ","pages":"Article 101463"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141294764","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The influence of story character realism and theme on protagonists’ internal states and dialogue in children’s retells 故事人物的现实性和主题对儿童复述作品中主人公内心状态和对话的影响
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101458
Samantha J. Russell , J. Jessica Wang , Kate Cain

Anthropomorphised animals are a prevalent character type in children’s books. However, stories with fantastical protagonists are associated with poorer social learning than those with human protagonists. We explored whether children’s representations of characters’ internal states and dialogue in story retells were related to story character realism (anthropomorphised animal, human), story theme (sharing, busyness), age, and vocabulary. Three- to seven-year-olds (N = 171) listened to one of four versions of an illustrated storybook that manipulated character realism and theme. Developmental trends were evident: Older children included more internal state references and dialogue in their retells than younger children. Of note, children retelling a prosocial story with human protagonists included more socio-relational language than those retelling a busy-themed story featuring humans; the same advantage was not evident for stories with animal protagonists. These findings imply that realistic protagonists may elicit more robust representations of social ideas in the minds of young children.

拟人化的动物是儿童读物中常见的角色类型。然而,与以人类为主角的故事相比,以幻想为主角的故事与较差的社会学习有关。我们探讨了儿童在复述故事时对人物内心状态和对话的表述是否与故事人物的真实性(拟人化动物、人类)、故事主题(分享、忙碌)、年龄和词汇量有关。三至七岁的儿童(人数=171)聆听了四种版本之一的插图故事书,这些版本对人物的逼真性和主题进行了处理。发展的趋势非常明显:与年龄较小的儿童相比,年龄较大的儿童在复述故事时会引用更多的内部状态和对话。值得注意的是,复述以人类为主角的亲社会故事的儿童比复述以人类为主角的繁忙主题故事的儿童使用了更多的社会关系语言;而复述以动物为主角的故事的儿童则没有明显的这种优势。这些研究结果表明,逼真的主人公可能会在幼儿头脑中引发更强烈的社会观念表征。
{"title":"The influence of story character realism and theme on protagonists’ internal states and dialogue in children’s retells","authors":"Samantha J. Russell ,&nbsp;J. Jessica Wang ,&nbsp;Kate Cain","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101458","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101458","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Anthropomorphised animals are a prevalent character type in children’s books. However, stories with fantastical protagonists are associated with poorer social learning than those with human protagonists. We explored whether children’s representations of characters’ internal states and dialogue in story retells were related to story character realism (anthropomorphised animal, human), story theme (sharing, busyness), age, and vocabulary. Three- to seven-year-olds (<em>N</em> = 171) listened to one of four versions of an illustrated storybook that manipulated character realism and theme. Developmental trends were evident: Older children included more internal state references and dialogue in their retells than younger children. Of note, children retelling a prosocial story with human protagonists included more socio-relational language than those retelling a busy-themed story featuring humans; the same advantage was not evident for stories with animal protagonists. These findings imply that realistic protagonists may elicit more robust representations of social ideas in the minds of young children.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"71 ","pages":"Article 101458"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000431/pdfft?md5=fe55ef38d900d2f5ddd8b4365484df24&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000431-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141291530","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Factors affecting children’s direct learning and productive memory processes in the context of virtual museums 影响儿童在虚拟博物馆中直接学习和生产性记忆过程的因素
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-07 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101454
Lucy M. Cronin-Golomb, Jelena Pejic, Hilary E. Miller-Goldwater, Patricia J. Bauer

Informal educational opportunities such as visits to museums, aquariums, and zoos support children’s semantic knowledge gain. Most research focuses on outcomes of direct learning, such as factual recall. The extent to which children engage in productive memory processes such as inferential reasoning and self-derivation through memory integration is not yet well understood. We assessed 8- to 9-year-old children’s performance on tests of direct (e.g., fact recall) and productive (e.g., inference, integration) learning from virtual museum exhibits. We also examined the influence of children’s involvement on learning outcomes, through measuring within-exhibit dyadic conversation and post-exhibit reflection. Children performed successfully on all three tests of learning; fact recall was the most accessible and self-derivation was the least. Both within and post-exhibit involvement predicted overall learning outcomes; within-exhibit conversational phrases predicted self-derivation performance in particular. The current work provides novel insights into mechanisms that support children’s informal learning.

参观博物馆、水族馆和动物园等非正规教育机会有助于儿童获得语义知识。大多数研究侧重于直接学习的结果,如事实回忆。至于儿童在多大程度上参与了生产性记忆过程,如通过记忆整合进行推理和自我激励,目前尚不十分清楚。我们评估了 8 至 9 岁儿童在虚拟博物馆展品的直接学习(如事实回忆)和生产性学习(如推理、整合)测试中的表现。我们还通过测量展品内部的对话和展品后的反思,研究了儿童参与对学习成果的影响。儿童在所有三项学习测试中都取得了成功;事实回忆最容易获得,自我激励最少。表演中和表演后的参与都能预测总体学习结果;表演中的会话短语尤其能预测自我激励的表现。目前的研究为支持儿童非正式学习的机制提供了新的见解。
{"title":"Factors affecting children’s direct learning and productive memory processes in the context of virtual museums","authors":"Lucy M. Cronin-Golomb,&nbsp;Jelena Pejic,&nbsp;Hilary E. Miller-Goldwater,&nbsp;Patricia J. Bauer","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101454","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101454","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Informal educational opportunities such as visits to museums, aquariums, and zoos support children’s semantic knowledge gain. Most research focuses on outcomes of direct learning, such as factual recall. The extent to which children engage in productive memory processes such as inferential reasoning and self-derivation through memory integration is not yet well understood. We assessed 8- to 9-year-old children’s performance on tests of direct (e.g., fact recall) and productive (e.g., inference, integration) learning from virtual museum exhibits. We also examined the influence of children’s involvement on learning outcomes, through measuring within-exhibit dyadic conversation and post-exhibit reflection. Children performed successfully on all three tests of learning; fact recall was the most accessible and self-derivation was the least. Both within and post-exhibit involvement predicted overall learning outcomes; within-exhibit conversational phrases predicted self-derivation performance in particular. The current work provides novel insights into mechanisms that support children’s informal learning.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"71 ","pages":"Article 101454"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141291529","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A cross-sequential study of theory of mind, IQ, and fair sharing framed socially and non-socially in young children 对幼儿思维理论、智商以及社会和非社会公平分享框架的跨序列研究
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-03 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101460
Jennifer Vonk , Theodore S. Tomeny , Stephanie E. Jett , Sterett H. Mercer , Julie Cwikla

Both ability and motivation underlie uniquely human prosociality but the study of resource allocation in young children has focused on prosocial motivations rather than mathematical ability to fairly allocate rewards. We examined the development of fair sharing by testing 3–6YO children at two time points (N at Time 1 = 158, N at Time 2 =111) with ToM, IQ, fractional quantities (proper or improper fraction problems), and framing of the problem (social or non-social) as predictors of children’s performance. As expected, children performed better with time and age, and on proper versus improper fractions. However, in contrast to our predictions, performance was not consistently related to general IQ, ToM, or framing of the problem although ToM interacted with age and framing to predict performance. Our results suggest that it is important to consider limitations in numerical ability rather than assuming selfish motivations when young children fail to share fairly.

能力和动机都是人类独特的亲社会性的基础,但对幼儿资源分配的研究主要集中在亲社会动机上,而不是公平分配奖励的数学能力上。我们在两个时间点(时间 1 = 158 人,时间 2 = 111 人)对 3-6 岁儿童进行了测试,以 ToM、智商、分数数量(正分数或不正分数问题)和问题框架(社会或非社会)作为预测儿童表现的指标,从而研究了公平分享的发展情况。不出所料,随着时间和年龄的增长,儿童在正分数和不正分数问题上的表现都更好。然而,与我们的预测不同的是,尽管 ToM 与年龄和问题框架相互作用,可以预测成绩,但成绩与一般智商、ToM 或问题框架并不一致。我们的研究结果表明,当幼儿不能公平分享时,重要的是要考虑他们计算能力的局限性,而不是假设他们有自私的动机。
{"title":"A cross-sequential study of theory of mind, IQ, and fair sharing framed socially and non-socially in young children","authors":"Jennifer Vonk ,&nbsp;Theodore S. Tomeny ,&nbsp;Stephanie E. Jett ,&nbsp;Sterett H. Mercer ,&nbsp;Julie Cwikla","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101460","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101460","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Both ability and motivation underlie uniquely human prosociality but the study of resource allocation in young children has focused on prosocial motivations rather than mathematical ability to fairly allocate rewards. We examined the development of fair sharing by testing 3–6YO children at two time points (N at Time 1 = 158, N at Time 2 =111) with ToM, IQ, fractional quantities (proper or improper fraction problems), and framing of the problem (social or non-social) as predictors of children’s performance. As expected, children performed better with time and age, and on proper versus improper fractions. However, in contrast to our predictions, performance was not consistently related to general IQ, ToM, or framing of the problem although ToM interacted with age and framing to predict performance. Our results suggest that it is important to consider limitations in numerical ability rather than assuming selfish motivations when young children fail to share fairly.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"71 ","pages":"Article 101460"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141239065","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1