首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Development最新文献

英文 中文
Sociolinguistic development in a diverse, multilinguistic environment: Evidence from multilingual children in Gujarat, India 多元化、多语言环境中的社会语言发展:来自印度古吉拉特邦多语种儿童的证据
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101504
Ruthe Foushee , Sophie Regan , Roya Baharloo , Mahesh Srinivasan
In today’s pluralistic societies, children regularly acquire multiple languages and are exposed to an even larger set of languages spoken by others in their environment. Yet despite the prevalence of multilingualism globally, most research on sociolinguistic development has focused on monolingual children in environments with relatively little linguistic diversity, and as such has left questions of what children take different languages to socially signify largely unaddressed. The present study aimed to fill this gap by tracing the development of social inferences about different languages among 129 multilingual 7- to 13-year-olds in Gujarat, India. Contrary to the prediction that children in multilingual contexts should be unlikely to make stereotyping inferences about a person speaking a language (e.g., because they might expect the person to know additional languages), children in our sample selectively linked the different languages and language varieties that we probed (Gujarati, Marathi, Hindi, Urdu, Tamil, American English, Indian English, and Mandarin Chinese) with different social dimensions—including facial appearance, geographic origin, religion, and wealth. Children’s responses generally reflected associations grounded in real-world regularities, but also reflected some associations that do not have a real-world basis (e.g., judging that Indian English speakers tend to be white, Christian, and originate from outside of India). Older children were also more likely to predict different languages to be differentially learnable by individuals of specific ethnicities, exhibiting a kind of essentialist belief. We discuss our findings in light of the sociolinguistic study of personae.
在当今多元化社会中,儿童经常习得多种语言,并接触到环境中其他人所说的更多语言。然而,尽管全球范围内多语言现象十分普遍,但大多数有关社会语言发展的研究都集中在语言多样性相对较少的环境中使用单语的儿童身上,因此,关于儿童认为不同语言在社会上意味着什么的问题在很大程度上没有得到解决。本研究旨在通过追踪印度古吉拉特邦 129 名 7 至 13 岁多语种儿童对不同语言的社会推断能力的发展,填补这一空白。根据预测,在多语言环境中,儿童不太可能对讲一种语言的人进行刻板印象推断(例如,因为他们可能会认为这个人还会其他语言),与此相反,在我们的样本中,儿童选择性地将我们探究的不同语言和语言变体(古吉拉特语、马拉地语、印地语、乌尔都语、泰米尔语、美式英语、印度英语和汉语普通话)与不同的社会维度联系起来,包括面部外貌、地理出身、宗教信仰和财富。儿童的回答一般反映了基于现实世界规律的联想,但也反映了一些没有现实世界基础的联想(例如,判断讲印度英语的人往往是白人、基督徒和来自印度以外的地方)。年龄较大的儿童也更倾向于预测特定种族的人学习不同语言的能力不同,表现出一种本质主义信念。我们将根据社会语言学中的 "角色 "研究来讨论我们的发现。
{"title":"Sociolinguistic development in a diverse, multilinguistic environment: Evidence from multilingual children in Gujarat, India","authors":"Ruthe Foushee ,&nbsp;Sophie Regan ,&nbsp;Roya Baharloo ,&nbsp;Mahesh Srinivasan","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101504","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101504","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>In today’s pluralistic societies, children regularly acquire multiple languages and are exposed to an even larger set of languages spoken by others in their environment. Yet despite the prevalence of multilingualism globally, most research on sociolinguistic development has focused on monolingual children in environments with relatively little linguistic diversity, and as such has left questions of what children take different languages to socially signify largely unaddressed. The present study aimed to fill this gap by tracing the development of social inferences about different languages among 129 multilingual 7- to 13-year-olds in Gujarat, India. Contrary to the prediction that children in multilingual contexts should be unlikely to make stereotyping inferences about a person speaking a language (e.g., because they might expect the person to know additional languages), children in our sample selectively linked the different languages and language varieties that we probed (Gujarati, Marathi, Hindi, Urdu, Tamil, American English, Indian English, and Mandarin Chinese) with different social dimensions—including facial appearance, geographic origin, religion, and wealth. Children’s responses generally reflected associations grounded in real-world regularities, but also reflected some associations that do not have a real-world basis (e.g., judging that Indian English speakers tend to be white, Christian, and originate from outside of India). Older children were also more likely to predict different languages to be differentially learnable by individuals of specific ethnicities, exhibiting a kind of essentialist belief. We discuss our findings in light of the sociolinguistic study of <em>personae</em>.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"72 ","pages":"Article 101504"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142425036","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Preschoolers prioritize humans over robots less than adults do: An eye-tracking study 学龄前儿童比成年人更不优先考虑人类而不是机器人:一项眼动追踪研究
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101505
Ke Zhou , Min Chen , Hui Xu , Yi Cao , Zhiqiang Yan
This study investigates how people make moral judgments about humans and robots, aiming to understand the reasons behind these moral choices. Using eye tracking technology, we examined 36 preschoolers and 36 adults as they faced road-accident dilemmas involving humans and robots. Our findings reveal notable differences between preschoolers and adults. Preschoolers were more likely to make utilitarian decisions, considering sacrificing humans to save robots morally acceptable compared to adults. Eye tracking data showed that preschoolers focused longer on human-robot and robot-human interactions than adults did. Our results highlight the role of empathy in shaping moral judgments. When controlling for empathy, early eye tracking indicators did not significantly predict moral judgments. Overall, our findings indicate that preschoolers prioritize humans over robots less than adults do. Additionally, individuals' moral preferences are reflected in their attentional processes, particularly during the early stages of moral judgment formation, and empathy plays a crucial role in how people morally judge humans and robots.
本研究调查了人们如何对人类和机器人做出道德判断,旨在了解这些道德选择背后的原因。我们利用眼动跟踪技术,对 36 名学龄前儿童和 36 名成年人在面对涉及人类和机器人的交通事故困境时的表现进行了研究。我们的研究结果显示了学龄前儿童和成年人之间的显著差异。与成人相比,学龄前儿童更倾向于做出功利性决策,认为牺牲人类来拯救机器人在道德上是可以接受的。眼动跟踪数据显示,学龄前儿童比成年人更关注人与机器人以及机器人与人之间的互动。我们的研究结果凸显了移情在道德判断中的作用。在控制移情的情况下,早期眼动追踪指标并不能显著预测道德判断。总体而言,我们的研究结果表明,学龄前儿童比成年人更看重人类而非机器人。此外,个体的道德偏好反映在他们的注意过程中,特别是在道德判断形成的早期阶段,移情在人们如何对人类和机器人进行道德判断方面起着至关重要的作用。
{"title":"Preschoolers prioritize humans over robots less than adults do: An eye-tracking study","authors":"Ke Zhou ,&nbsp;Min Chen ,&nbsp;Hui Xu ,&nbsp;Yi Cao ,&nbsp;Zhiqiang Yan","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101505","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101505","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>This study investigates how people make moral judgments about humans and robots, aiming to understand the reasons behind these moral choices. Using eye tracking technology, we examined 36 preschoolers and 36 adults as they faced road-accident dilemmas involving humans and robots. Our findings reveal notable differences between preschoolers and adults. Preschoolers were more likely to make utilitarian decisions, considering sacrificing humans to save robots morally acceptable compared to adults. Eye tracking data showed that preschoolers focused longer on human-robot and robot-human interactions than adults did. Our results highlight the role of empathy in shaping moral judgments. When controlling for empathy, early eye tracking indicators did not significantly predict moral judgments. Overall, our findings indicate that preschoolers prioritize humans over robots less than adults do. Additionally, individuals' moral preferences are reflected in their attentional processes, particularly during the early stages of moral judgment formation, and empathy plays a crucial role in how people morally judge humans and robots.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"72 ","pages":"Article 101505"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142357169","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attending to talker characteristics: Word learning and recognition in monolingually- and multilingually-raised infants 关注说话者的特征:单语和多语婴儿的词汇学习与识别
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101508
Federica Bulgarelli , Sophie Barry , Elika Bergelson
Before age one, infants often fail to recognize words produced by new talkers or in new accents. We ask whether infant’s varying experiences, namely exposure to multiple languages or accented speech, might influence this ability. Monolingually and multilingually-raised North-American 8-month-olds were habituated to a novel word-object link, and tested to see whether they would increase their looking time (i.e. dishabituate) when 1) the word-object link was broken (i.e. hearing a new word with the old object or vice versa), and 2) when the word was produced by a new talker (Exp 1) or in a new accent (Exp 2) (i.e. changes that maintain the word-object link). Monolingually- and multilingually-raised infants dishabituated to all changes, suggesting that their varying accent and language experiences do not shape word learning and recognition as tested here. This work provides further evidence that 8-month-olds’ word-object links are non-adult-like from a more diverse group of participants.
一岁前,婴儿经常无法识别新的说话者或新的口音所产生的词语。我们的问题是,婴儿的不同经历(即接触多种语言或重音说话)是否会影响他们的这种能力。我们对 8 个月大的北美单语和多语种婴儿进行了习惯新词-对象联系的测试,以观察他们在以下情况下是否会增加寻找时间(即 "不习惯"):1)词-对象联系中断(即听到新词与旧对象或相反);2)新的说话者(实验 1)或新的口音(实验 2)(即保持词-对象联系的变化)。单语和多语养育的婴儿对所有变化都不适应,这表明他们不同的口音和语言经验并不会影响单词的学习和识别。这项研究进一步证明,8 个月大的婴儿与词-对象的联系是非成人型的。
{"title":"Attending to talker characteristics: Word learning and recognition in monolingually- and multilingually-raised infants","authors":"Federica Bulgarelli ,&nbsp;Sophie Barry ,&nbsp;Elika Bergelson","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101508","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101508","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Before age one, infants often fail to recognize words produced by new talkers or in new accents. We ask whether infant’s varying experiences, namely exposure to multiple languages or accented speech, might influence this ability. Monolingually and multilingually-raised North-American 8-month-olds were habituated to a novel word-object link, and tested to see whether they would increase their looking time (i.e. dishabituate) when 1) the word-object link was broken (i.e. hearing a new word with the old object or vice versa), and 2) when the word was produced by a new talker (Exp 1) or in a new accent (Exp 2) (i.e. changes that maintain the word-object link). Monolingually- and multilingually-raised infants dishabituated to all changes, suggesting that their varying accent and language experiences do not shape word learning and recognition as tested here. This work provides further evidence that 8-month-olds’ word-object links are non-adult-like from a more diverse group of participants.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"72 ","pages":"Article 101508"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142425038","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children's needs-oriented decision-making: A developmental perspective 儿童以需求为导向的决策:发展的视角
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101506
Qingfeng Peng, Mei Li, Hong Li
Fairness is a fundamental aspect of moral development, with need-based allocation representing a key form of equitable resource distribution, where resources are allocated to those with greater needs. Despite its importance, the motivational underpinnings of children's need-based allocation remain inadequately understood. In this review, we propose an explanatory model to elucidate the motivation behind children's need-based allocation. We suggest that this motivation evolves through three developmental stages: the balancing tendency stage (ages 4–5), the balancing-altruistic dual-motivation stage (ages 6–7), and the altruistic tendency stage (ages 8 and above). Finally, we discuss future research directions to further explore and understand the developmental trajectory of children's fairness-related decision-making.
公平是道德发展的一个基本方面,而按需分配则是公平分配资源的一种重要形式,即把资源分配给更需要的人。尽管其重要性不言而喻,但人们对儿童按需分配的动机基础仍然了解不足。在这篇综述中,我们提出了一个解释模型,以阐明儿童按需分配背后的动机。我们认为,这种动机经历了三个发展阶段:平衡倾向阶段(4-5 岁)、平衡-利他双重动机阶段(6-7 岁)和利他倾向阶段(8 岁以上)。最后,我们讨论了未来的研究方向,以进一步探索和理解儿童公平相关决策的发展轨迹。
{"title":"Children's needs-oriented decision-making: A developmental perspective","authors":"Qingfeng Peng,&nbsp;Mei Li,&nbsp;Hong Li","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101506","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101506","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Fairness is a fundamental aspect of moral development, with need-based allocation representing a key form of equitable resource distribution, where resources are allocated to those with greater needs. Despite its importance, the motivational underpinnings of children's need-based allocation remain inadequately understood. In this review, we propose an explanatory model to elucidate the motivation behind children's need-based allocation. We suggest that this motivation evolves through three developmental stages: the balancing tendency stage (ages 4–5), the balancing-altruistic dual-motivation stage (ages 6–7), and the altruistic tendency stage (ages 8 and above). Finally, we discuss future research directions to further explore and understand the developmental trajectory of children's fairness-related decision-making.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"72 ","pages":"Article 101506"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142326425","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Developmental changes in the time window for the explicit sense of agency experienced across the lifespan 人的一生中,明确的代入感时间窗口的发展变化
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101503
Satoshi Nobusako , Yusaku Takamura , Kyohei Koge , Michihiro Osumi , Takaki Maeda , Shu Morioka
Sense of agency (SoA) is the subjective experience of individuals that they are initiators and controllers of their own actions, but it is not known how the time window for SoA changes developmentally over the lifetime. The present study examined developmental changes in the time window for SoA in school-age children (6–12 years), adolescents (16–18 years), young adults (20–25 years), adults (28–64 years), and older adults (65–83 years). The current results show that the time window for SoA is longer in young adults than in school-age children, adolescents, adults, and older adults, suggesting that young adulthood may be an important period of developmental change in the time window for SoA across the lifespan.
代入感(SoA)是个人的主观体验,即他们是自己行动的发起者和控制者,但目前尚不清楚代入感的时间窗口在人的一生中会发生怎样的发展变化。本研究考察了学龄儿童(6-12 岁)、青少年(16-18 岁)、青年(20-25 岁)、成年人(28-64 岁)和老年人(65-83 岁)的 SoA 时间窗的发展变化。目前的研究结果表明,与学龄儿童、青少年、成年人和老年人相比,青壮年的SoA时间窗更长,这表明青壮年时期可能是整个生命周期中SoA时间窗发展变化的重要时期。
{"title":"Developmental changes in the time window for the explicit sense of agency experienced across the lifespan","authors":"Satoshi Nobusako ,&nbsp;Yusaku Takamura ,&nbsp;Kyohei Koge ,&nbsp;Michihiro Osumi ,&nbsp;Takaki Maeda ,&nbsp;Shu Morioka","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101503","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101503","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Sense of agency (SoA) is the subjective experience of individuals that they are initiators and controllers of their own actions, but it is not known how the time window for SoA changes developmentally over the lifetime. The present study examined developmental changes in the time window for SoA in school-age children (6–12 years), adolescents (16–18 years), young adults (20–25 years), adults (28–64 years), and older adults (65–83 years). The current results show that the time window for SoA is longer in young adults than in school-age children, adolescents, adults, and older adults, suggesting that young adulthood may be an important period of developmental change in the time window for SoA across the lifespan.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"72 ","pages":"Article 101503"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142320379","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Development of L1-L2 naming skills in a monolingual context: Evidence from children and adolescents 单语环境下 L1-L2 命名能力的发展:来自儿童和青少年的证据
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101492
Antonio Iniesta , Marta Rivera , Daniela Paolieri , Teresa Bajo

Adolescence is marked by significant developmental changes that can influence language processing and control. This study aimed to uncover developmental differences in language co-activation and control in unbalanced Spanish (L1)-English (L2) bilinguals. Children and adolescents attending bilingual schools within a L1 monolingual context completed a picture-naming task including cognates and non-cognates nouns, with collection of behavioral and ERP data. The study consistently found a cognate facilitation effect (CFE) in L2, evident in enhanced accuracy, faster reaction times, and reduced N400 negativity for cognates in comparison with no-cognate nouns. However, in L1, CFE was only observed in the N400 component, indicating weaker transfer from L2 to L1. Additionally, children exhibited greater N200 negativity when naming cognates in L1, while adolescents showed no N200 modulations, suggesting differences in frontal control region involvement and potential differences in control strategies. Language co-activation appears independent of maturation, while language control depends on development.

青春期的显著发育变化会影响语言处理和控制。本研究旨在揭示不平衡的西班牙语(第一语言)-英语(第二语言)双语者在语言共同激活和控制方面的发展差异。在母语为单语的环境下,在双语学校就读的儿童和青少年完成了一项图片命名任务,包括认知和非认知名词,并收集了行为和ERP数据。研究一致发现,在 L2 中存在认知促进效应(CFE),表现为认知名词与非认知名词相比,认知名词的准确性提高、反应时间加快、N400 负性降低。然而,在 L1 中,只在 N400 成分中观察到 CFE,这表明从 L2 到 L1 的迁移较弱。此外,儿童在用 L1 命名同义词时表现出更大的 N200 负性,而青少年则没有表现出 N200 调节,这表明额叶控制区参与的差异和控制策略的潜在差异。语言共同激活似乎与成熟无关,而语言控制则取决于发育情况。
{"title":"Development of L1-L2 naming skills in a monolingual context: Evidence from children and adolescents","authors":"Antonio Iniesta ,&nbsp;Marta Rivera ,&nbsp;Daniela Paolieri ,&nbsp;Teresa Bajo","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101492","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101492","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Adolescence is marked by significant developmental changes that can influence language processing and control. This study aimed to uncover developmental differences in language co-activation and control in unbalanced Spanish (L1)-English (L2) bilinguals. Children and adolescents attending bilingual schools within a L1 monolingual context completed a picture-naming task including cognates and non-cognates nouns, with collection of behavioral and ERP data. The study consistently found a cognate facilitation effect (CFE) in L2, evident in enhanced accuracy, faster reaction times, and reduced N400 negativity for cognates in comparison with no-cognate nouns. However, in L1, CFE was only observed in the N400 component, indicating weaker transfer from L2 to L1. Additionally, children exhibited greater N200 negativity when naming cognates in L1, while adolescents showed no N200 modulations, suggesting differences in frontal control region involvement and potential differences in control strategies. Language co-activation appears independent of maturation, while language control depends on development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"72 ","pages":"Article 101492"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000777/pdfft?md5=878b3e6bde9c9e49c6dc1ad47de64ec2&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000777-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142241551","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The shape bias in Mandarin-exposed young autistic children: The role of abstract shape representation 接触普通话的自闭症幼儿的形状偏差:抽象形状表征的作用
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101491
Wenwen Hou , Yi (Esther) Su , Letitia R. Naigles , Li Li , Muyu Zhou

The shape bias is an important word learning strategy in children’s language development. Although some studies have observed an absent or atypical shape bias in autistic children, there is no converging evidence regarding its underlying bases. Moreover, previous research has been exclusively conducted in learners of Indo-European languages, yet it is unclear whether the shape bias is a universal word learning constraint across languages. This study aims to investigate the shape bias and its association with shape representation ability in 40 1–3-year-old non-autistic children, and 41 2–6-year-old autistic children, exposed to Mandarin Chinese. The results suggested that Mandarin-exposed non-autistic children exhibited a shape bias, while autistic children did not. Further, a positive correlation was found between the shape representation accuracy and shape bias performance in the autistic group. These findings provide cross-linguistic evidence for the shape bias as a word learning constraint in non-autistic toddlers but challenges in utilizing this constraint in word learning by young autistic children. Importantly, these results shed new light on the critical role of abstract representations of object shape in facilitating shape bias knowledge in autistic children.

形状偏误是儿童语言发展过程中一种重要的单词学习策略。虽然有些研究观察到自闭症儿童不存在形状偏误或形状偏误不典型,但关于其根本原因却没有一致的证据。此外,以往的研究都是针对印欧语言学习者进行的,但形状偏误是否是一种跨语言的普遍词汇学习限制尚不清楚。本研究旨在调查 40 名 1-3 岁非自闭症儿童和 41 名 2-6 岁自闭症儿童在接触普通话后出现的形状偏差及其与形状表征能力的关联。结果表明,接触普通话的非自闭症儿童表现出形状偏差,而自闭症儿童则没有。此外,还发现自闭症儿童组的形状表征准确性与形状偏差表现之间存在正相关。这些发现提供了跨语言证据,证明形状偏差是非自闭症幼儿学习单词的一种制约因素,但自闭症幼儿在利用这种制约因素学习单词时却面临挑战。重要的是,这些结果揭示了物体形状的抽象表征在促进自闭症儿童形状偏差知识方面的关键作用。
{"title":"The shape bias in Mandarin-exposed young autistic children: The role of abstract shape representation","authors":"Wenwen Hou ,&nbsp;Yi (Esther) Su ,&nbsp;Letitia R. Naigles ,&nbsp;Li Li ,&nbsp;Muyu Zhou","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101491","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101491","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The shape bias is an important word learning strategy in children’s language development. Although some studies have observed an absent or atypical shape bias in autistic children, there is no converging evidence regarding its underlying bases. Moreover, previous research has been exclusively conducted in learners of Indo-European languages, yet it is unclear whether the shape bias is a universal word learning constraint across languages. This study aims to investigate the shape bias and its association with shape representation ability in 40 1–3-year-old non-autistic children, and 41 2–6-year-old autistic children, exposed to Mandarin Chinese. The results suggested that Mandarin-exposed non-autistic children exhibited a shape bias, while autistic children did not. Further, a positive correlation was found between the shape representation accuracy and shape bias performance in the autistic group. These findings provide cross-linguistic evidence for the shape bias as a word learning constraint in non-autistic toddlers but challenges in utilizing this constraint in word learning by young autistic children. Importantly, these results shed new light on the critical role of abstract representations of object shape in facilitating shape bias knowledge in autistic children.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"72 ","pages":"Article 101491"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142241550","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The “How many?” task inadequately assesses the understanding of the cardinality principle 有多少?"任务没有充分评估对万有引力原则的理解
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101500
Catherine Thevenot, Marie Krenger

Once children have acquired the cardinality principle, they understand that the last number-word used in counting represents the total number of objects in a set. This principle is often assessed using the “How many?” task, which consists in asking “How many?” objects there are in a set after children have counted them. However, we show in this study that out of 188 kindergarteners (mean age: 4 ½ years), 42 (22,3 %) succeeded in repeating the last count word in the “How many?” task but failed to correctly apply the one-to-one correspondence principle during counting. Even when only the easiest countable sets were considered (i.e., linear and homogeneous collections or sets with a very limited number of objects), still more than 10 % of children from our sample repeated the last count word but failed to apply the one-to-one correspondence principle. Such developmental profile, in which children understand that the last word used in counting represents the total number of objects in a set but fail to grasp that each individual object must be associated with a single number word to determine this total, is not psychologically plausible. We conclude that the “How many?” task leads to an inaccurate assessment of the cardinality principle, in both its basic and more meaningful conception, in a non-negligible number of young children.

一旦孩子们掌握了 "万有引力 "原则,他们就会明白,数数时使用的最后一个数 字代表了一组物体的总数。对这一原则的评估通常采用 "有多少?"任务,即在儿童数完一组物体后,询问 "有多少?"。然而,我们在本研究中发现,在 188 名幼儿园儿童(平均年龄:4 岁半)中,有 42 人(22.3%)在 "有多少?"任务中成功地重复了最后一个数词,但在数数时却未能正确应用一一对应原则。即使只考虑最简单的可数集(即线性和同质集合或物体数量非常有限的集合),我们的样本中仍有超过 10% 的儿童重复了最后一个数词,但未能应用一一对应原则。在这种发展过程中,幼儿懂得计数时使用的最后一个数词代表集合中物体的总数,但却不懂得每个物体都必须与一个数词相关联才能确定总数,这在心理学上是说不通的。我们的结论是,"有多少?"任务会导致相当数量的幼儿对万有引力原理的基本概念和更有意义的概念作出不准确的评估。
{"title":"The “How many?” task inadequately assesses the understanding of the cardinality principle","authors":"Catherine Thevenot,&nbsp;Marie Krenger","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101500","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101500","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Once children have acquired the cardinality principle, they understand that the last number-word used in counting represents the total number of objects in a set. This principle is often assessed using the <em>“How many?”</em> task, which consists in asking “How many?” objects there are in a set after children have counted them. However, we show in this study that out of 188 kindergarteners (mean age: 4 ½ years), 42 (22,3 %) succeeded in repeating the last count word in the <em>“How many?”</em> task but failed to correctly apply the one-to-one correspondence principle during counting. Even when only the easiest countable sets were considered (i.e., linear and homogeneous collections or sets with a very limited number of objects), still more than 10 % of children from our sample repeated the last count word but failed to apply the one-to-one correspondence principle. Such developmental profile, in which children understand that the last word used in counting represents the total number of objects in a set but fail to grasp that each individual object must be associated with a single number word to determine this total, is not psychologically plausible. We conclude that the <em>“How many?”</em> task leads to an inaccurate assessment of the cardinality principle, in both its basic and more meaningful conception, in a non-negligible number of young children.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"72 ","pages":"Article 101500"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000856/pdfft?md5=07734e18e2d6021099b4a13ce4141d72&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000856-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142241549","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children’s perceptions of intergroup similarity and dissimilarity and their association with attitudes towards a conflict out-group 儿童对群体间相似性和不相似性的看法及其与对冲突外群体态度的关联
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101499
Meytal Nasie , Shiri Stanescu

Children’s perceptions of similarity and dissimilarity between in- and out-groups and their associations with intergroup attitudes were examined. Using mixed qualitative and quantitative methods, 5- and 8-year-old Jewish-Israeli children (N = 100, 48 % girls) were investigated. They were asked about Arabs (which are considered a conflict out-group). Specifically, they were asked whether a Jewish girl/boy and an Arab girl/boy are similar or dissimilar and in what way. They were also asked about their attitudes towards Arabs. In general, children reported more intergroup dissimilarities than similarities between Jews and Arabs. Children’s perceptions revealed that they considered three types of properties as fundamental for intergroup (dis)similarity: appearance, psychological characteristics, and social identity. The results indicate that children form intergroup dissimilarity perceptions at an early age, but only at about 8 years of age do these perceptions become associated with negative attitudes towards the out-group. These findings highlight the importance of promoting early awareness of similarities between groups to mitigate the development of negative attitudes as children grow older.

研究考察了儿童对本群体和外群体之间相似性和不相似性的看法及其与群体间态度的关联。研究采用定性和定量相结合的方法,对 5 岁和 8 岁的以色列犹太儿童(人数 = 100,48% 为女童)进行了调查。他们被问及有关阿拉伯人(被视为冲突外群体)的问题。具体地说,他们被问及犹太女孩/男孩和阿拉伯女孩/男孩是相似还是不相似,在哪些方面相似。他们还被问及对阿拉伯人的态度。总的来说,孩子们报告的群体间差异多于犹太人和阿拉伯人之间的相似性。儿童的看法表明,他们认为三类属性是群体间(不)相似性的基本要素:外表、心理特征和社会身份。研究结果表明,儿童在很小的时候就会形成群体间不相似的观念,但只有到了 8 岁左右,这些观念才会与对外群体的消极态度联系起来。这些研究结果凸显了促进儿童尽早认识群体间相似性的重要性,以减轻儿童在长大后形成的消极态度。
{"title":"Children’s perceptions of intergroup similarity and dissimilarity and their association with attitudes towards a conflict out-group","authors":"Meytal Nasie ,&nbsp;Shiri Stanescu","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101499","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101499","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Children’s perceptions of similarity and dissimilarity between in- and out-groups and their associations with intergroup attitudes were examined. Using mixed qualitative and quantitative methods, 5- and 8-year-old Jewish-Israeli children (<em>N</em> = 100, 48 % girls) were investigated. They were asked about Arabs (which are considered a conflict out-group). Specifically, they were asked whether a Jewish girl/boy and an Arab girl/boy are similar or dissimilar and in what way. They were also asked about their attitudes towards Arabs. In general, children reported more intergroup dissimilarities than similarities between Jews and Arabs. Children’s perceptions revealed that they considered three types of properties as fundamental for intergroup (dis)similarity: appearance, psychological characteristics, and social identity. The results indicate that children form intergroup dissimilarity perceptions at an early age, but only at about 8 years of age do these perceptions become associated with negative attitudes towards the out-group. These findings highlight the importance of promoting early awareness of similarities between groups to mitigate the development of negative attitudes as children grow older.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"72 ","pages":"Article 101499"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142241548","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dating early memories: When did events really happen? 约会早期记忆:事件到底发生在什么时候?
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101489
Carole Peterson , Qi Wang , Darcy Hallett , Sophie Bartlett , Amanda Y.F. Ma , Melissa M. MacKay , Emma J. Pretty , Luciana Viscarra , Elaine Y. Wang

Can children accurately date their early memories? This question has important real-life consequences such as when jurors evaluate the credibility of child eyewitness testimony in court. Answering this question is difficult given that adults present at remembered events may be inaccurate themselves in retroactively dating the memories recalled by their children, and often cannot provide reliable validation. In this study, prior to child interviews the parents of 6- to 13-year-olds provided eight memories of events with known dates, two each from when children were age 2, 3, 4, and 5 years. A total of 104 6- to 13-year-olds participated (47 % female, 70 % White and 26 % Asian or multi-ethnic), recruited from Canada (36 %) and USA (64 %). Children typically made systematic dating errors. Memories of events that had occurred when children had been age 2 were misdated by 1½ years on average, and as children’s age at the time of remembered events increased, misdating errors decreased. Errors usually involved children thinking they had been older at the time of remembered events than they actually were – a phenomenon termed ‘forward telescoping’ (versus ‘backward telescoping, when individuals think that they had been older at the time of remembered events than they actually had been). For example, many of the events from when children were age 2 were recalled by the children, but they misdated them to older ages. Although ‘age of memory’ (age of the child at the time of the remembered event) was significantly related to errors in dating, with more errors for memories from younger periods of their lives, ‘age of child’ at the time they did the memory task did not differ depending upon how old the children were. Findings have theoretical and forensic implications.

儿童能否准确地确定其早期记忆的日期?这个问题在现实生活中具有重要影响,例如陪审员在法庭上评估儿童目击证人证词的可信度时。要回答这个问题是很困难的,因为当时在场的成年人在追溯孩子的记忆时可能并不准确,而且往往无法提供可靠的验证。在本研究中,6 到 13 岁儿童的父母在对儿童进行访谈之前,提供了 8 个已知日期的事件记忆,其中 2 个分别来自儿童 2 岁、3 岁、4 岁和 5 岁时。共有 104 名 6 至 13 岁的儿童(47% 为女性,70% 为白人,26% 为亚裔或多种族)参加了此次访谈,他们分别来自加拿大(36%)和美国(64%)。儿童通常会犯系统性的约会错误。儿童对 2 岁时发生的事件的记忆平均错记了 1 年半,随着儿童记忆事件发生时年龄的增加,错记日期的错误也随之减少。错误通常涉及儿童认为他们在记忆事件发生时的年龄比实际年龄大--这种现象被称为 "向前伸缩"(相对于 "向后伸缩",即个人认为他们在记忆事件发生时的年龄比实际年龄大)。例如,孩子们回忆起了许多他们 2 岁时发生的事情,但他们却错误地把这些事情的时间推算到了更大的年龄。虽然 "记忆年龄"(记忆事件发生时儿童的年龄)与年代错误有很大关系,儿童对较年轻时期的记忆错误较多,但儿童做记忆任务时的 "儿童年龄 "并不因儿童的年龄而异。研究结果具有理论和法医学意义。
{"title":"Dating early memories: When did events really happen?","authors":"Carole Peterson ,&nbsp;Qi Wang ,&nbsp;Darcy Hallett ,&nbsp;Sophie Bartlett ,&nbsp;Amanda Y.F. Ma ,&nbsp;Melissa M. MacKay ,&nbsp;Emma J. Pretty ,&nbsp;Luciana Viscarra ,&nbsp;Elaine Y. Wang","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101489","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101489","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Can children accurately date their early memories? This question has important real-life consequences such as when jurors evaluate the credibility of child eyewitness testimony in court. Answering this question is difficult given that adults present at remembered events may be inaccurate themselves in retroactively dating the memories recalled by their children, and often cannot provide reliable validation. In this study, prior to child interviews the parents of 6- to 13-year-olds provided eight memories of events with known dates, two each from when children were age 2, 3, 4, and 5 years. A total of 104 6- to 13-year-olds participated (47 % female, 70 % White and 26 % Asian or multi-ethnic), recruited from Canada (36 %) and USA (64 %). Children typically made systematic dating errors. Memories of events that had occurred when children had been age 2 were misdated by 1½ years on average, and as children’s age at the time of remembered events increased, misdating errors decreased. Errors usually involved children thinking they had been older at the time of remembered events than they actually were – a phenomenon termed ‘forward telescoping’ (versus ‘backward telescoping, when individuals think that they had been older at the time of remembered events than they actually had been). For example, many of the events from when children were age 2 were recalled by the children, but they misdated them to older ages. Although ‘age of memory’ (age of the child at the time of the remembered event) was significantly related to errors in dating, with more errors for memories from younger periods of their lives, ‘age of child’ at the time they did the memory task did not differ depending upon how old the children were. Findings have theoretical and forensic implications.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"71 ","pages":"Article 101489"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000741/pdfft?md5=937432cd47a9c31986716e10d60efb5f&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000741-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142039608","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1