首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Development最新文献

英文 中文
When should children copy their mothers’ food choices? Social learning strategies about foods 儿童何时应该模仿母亲的食物选择?关于食物的社会学习策略
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101446
Naoko Nakamichi

Social learning strategies guide individuals in deciding from whom to learn and when. Recent research on food learning has revealed that children adopt “who” strategies, but whether they used “when” strategies remained unclear. This study investigated 4- to 6-year-old children’s beliefs about “when” another child should copy their mother’s food choice. In Experiment 1, children (N = 24, 58% female) determined whether another child should adopt social or asocial learning when selecting food from among familiar or novel foods. In Experiment 2, children (N = 26, 57% female) determined whether another child should adopt social or asocial learning when selecting food in an environment with a low or high percentage of bad-tasting foods. Children responded that social learning should be adopted when selecting from among novel foods and in an environment with a high percentage of bad-tasting foods. These findings revealed that children employ two “when” strategies when making food choices.

社会学习策略指导个人决定向谁学习以及何时学习。最近有关食物学习的研究表明,儿童会采用 "向谁学 "的策略,但他们是否会采用 "何时学 "的策略仍不清楚。本研究调查了 4 到 6 岁儿童对 "何时 "另一个孩子应该模仿他们母亲的食物选择的看法。在实验 1 中,儿童(24 人,58% 为女性)判断另一个儿童在从熟悉或新奇的食物中选择食物时,应该采用社会学习还是非社会学习。在实验 2 中,儿童(人数=26,女性占 57%)判断另一个儿童在坏味道食物比例较低或较高的环境中选择食物时,应该采用社会学习还是非社会学习。儿童的回答是,在从新奇食物中挑选食物时,以及在坏味道食物比例较高的环境中,应采用社会学习。这些发现表明,儿童在选择食物时采用了两种 "何时 "策略。
{"title":"When should children copy their mothers’ food choices? Social learning strategies about foods","authors":"Naoko Nakamichi","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101446","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101446","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Social learning strategies guide individuals in deciding from whom to learn and when. Recent research on food learning has revealed that children adopt “who” strategies, but whether they used “when” strategies remained unclear. This study investigated 4- to 6-year-old children’s beliefs about “when” another child should copy their mother’s food choice. In Experiment 1, children (<em>N</em> = 24, 58% female) determined whether another child should adopt social or asocial learning when selecting food from among familiar or novel foods. In Experiment 2, children (<em>N</em> = 26, 57% female) determined whether another child should adopt social or asocial learning when selecting food in an environment with a low or high percentage of bad-tasting foods. Children responded that social learning should be adopted when selecting from among novel foods and in an environment with a high percentage of bad-tasting foods. These findings revealed that children employ two “when” strategies when making food choices.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000315/pdfft?md5=f0e41db93eb47cd04135c9a23bf0c946&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000315-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140878549","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Memory for space and time in 2-year-olds 两岁儿童对空间和时间的记忆
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101443
Lindsey Mooney , Jasmeen Dadra , Kelsey Davinson , Naoya Tani , Simona Ghetti

Although the capacity to remember spatial and temporal information may develop at different rates throughout childhood, its early development has rarely been examined within the same participants, using the same task, and across different time delays elucidating retention of different aspects of early episodic memories. We used a novel tablet game to investigate memory for objects’ spatial locations and temporal order in a sample of toddlers ranging in age from 2 years to 2;8 years (M = 2;4 years, SD = 2 months; N = 73). We examined performance both immediately after an initial and an additional demonstration, following a 20-minute delay, and 1 week after learning; performance was also assessed following a new demonstration after the 1-week delay test. Using a linear mixed model, we found that toddlers remembered spatial locations better than temporal order, and temporal memory decayed more quickly and did not benefit from reminders compared to spatial memory, underscoring that early memory fragility may depend on the type of information being retained.

虽然记忆空间和时间信息的能力可能在整个童年期以不同的速度发展,但很少有人在同一参与者中、使用相同的任务、跨越不同的时间延迟来研究其早期发展,以阐明早期外显记忆不同方面的保留情况。我们使用一种新颖的平板电脑游戏来研究2岁至2;8岁幼儿对物体空间位置和时间顺序的记忆(中=2;4岁,标差=2个月;N=73)。我们考察了幼儿在初次演示和额外演示之后、延迟 20 分钟之后以及学习 1 周之后的表现;在延迟 1 周测试之后,我们还对幼儿在新演示之后的表现进行了评估。通过线性混合模型,我们发现幼儿对空间位置的记忆好于对时间顺序的记忆,与空间记忆相比,时间记忆衰减得更快,而且不能从提醒中获益。
{"title":"Memory for space and time in 2-year-olds","authors":"Lindsey Mooney ,&nbsp;Jasmeen Dadra ,&nbsp;Kelsey Davinson ,&nbsp;Naoya Tani ,&nbsp;Simona Ghetti","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101443","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101443","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Although the capacity to remember spatial and temporal information may develop at different rates throughout childhood, its early development has rarely been examined within the same participants, using the same task, and across different time delays elucidating retention of different aspects of early episodic memories. We used a novel tablet game to investigate memory for objects’ spatial locations and temporal order in a sample of toddlers ranging in age from 2 years to 2;8 years (<em>M</em> = 2;4 years, <em>SD</em> = 2 months; N = 73). We examined performance both immediately after an initial and an additional demonstration, following a 20-minute delay, and 1 week after learning; performance was also assessed following a new demonstration after the 1-week delay test. Using a linear mixed model, we found that toddlers remembered spatial locations better than temporal order, and temporal memory decayed more quickly and did not benefit from reminders compared to spatial memory, underscoring that early memory fragility may depend on the type of information being retained.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000285/pdfft?md5=4ffda44e68c99d046a592ea33dd674df&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000285-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140813553","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of cognitive and environmental factors on children’s early saving 认知和环境因素对儿童早期储蓄的影响
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101447
Katherine Dueck , Alex W. Castro , Deepthi Kamawar , Marina Milyavskaya , Elena Gallitto , Cristina M. Atance

Saving plays an important role in children’s everyday lives. We examine the impact of a verbal prompt on children’s saving in a laboratory task and whether cognitive and environmental factors predict children’s performance on this task, as well as on parent-reported child saving behaviors. One hundred and eighty-seven 3- to 7-year-olds completed the saving task, in which they received tokens that they could spend immediately on a less preferred reward or save for a more preferred reward later. Half the children received a verbal prompt to save whereas the other half did not. Significantly more children in the prompt condition saved at least one token for their preferred reward, suggesting that highlighting saving as an option increases children’s saving. Parent-reported child saving was also significantly predicted by parent-reported inhibitory control and the saving practices parents reported with their child. We discuss implications of our findings for fostering adaptive future-oriented reasoning in childhood.

储蓄在儿童的日常生活中扮演着重要角色。我们在一项实验室任务中研究了语言提示对儿童储蓄的影响,以及认知和环境因素是否能预测儿童在这项任务中的表现,以及家长报告的儿童储蓄行为。有 187 名 3 至 7 岁的儿童完成了储蓄任务,他们在任务中获得了代币,他们可以立即将代币花在不太喜欢的奖励上,也可以将代币存起来以备日后获得更喜欢的奖励。一半的孩子会收到储蓄的口头提示,而另一半孩子则没有。在提示条件下,为自己喜欢的奖励储蓄至少一个代币的儿童显著增多,这表明强调储蓄是一种选择会提高儿童的储蓄率。家长报告的抑制性控制和家长报告的与孩子一起储蓄的做法也能显著预测家长报告的儿童储蓄情况。我们将讨论我们的研究结果对培养儿童适应性未来导向推理的意义。
{"title":"The effects of cognitive and environmental factors on children’s early saving","authors":"Katherine Dueck ,&nbsp;Alex W. Castro ,&nbsp;Deepthi Kamawar ,&nbsp;Marina Milyavskaya ,&nbsp;Elena Gallitto ,&nbsp;Cristina M. Atance","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101447","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101447","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Saving plays an important role in children’s everyday lives. We examine the impact of a verbal prompt on children’s saving in a laboratory task and whether cognitive and environmental factors predict children’s performance on this task, as well as on parent-reported child saving behaviors. One hundred and eighty-seven 3- to 7-year-olds completed the saving task, in which they received tokens that they could spend immediately on a less preferred reward or save for a more preferred reward later. Half the children received a verbal prompt to save whereas the other half did not. Significantly more children in the prompt condition saved at least one token for their preferred reward, suggesting that highlighting saving as an option increases children’s saving. Parent-reported child saving was also significantly predicted by parent-reported inhibitory control and the saving practices parents reported with their child. We discuss implications of our findings for fostering adaptive future-oriented reasoning in childhood.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140878548","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Morally-relevant theory of mind is related to viewing gender inequalities as unacceptable 道德相关心智理论与将性别不平等视为不可接受有关
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101450
Jacquelyn Glidden, Kathryn M. Yee, Melanie Killen

Previous research has shown that morally-relevant theory of mind enables children to avoid blaming a peer for an accidental transgression. The current study investigated whether this form of theory of mind helps children recognize that gender inequalities are unfair and create negative emotional experiences. Further, the study examined this ability across three perspectives (for themselves, for those who have been advantaged by inequality, and for those who have been disadvantaged by inequality). Participants were 141 children (MAge = 6.67 years, 49% female, 32% ethnic/racial minority) recruited from the mid-Atlantic region of the U.S. Experience with the negative consequences of gender bias and more advanced mental state understanding was associated with more negative evaluations of gender inequalities and more neutral attributions of others’ emotions. These findings shed light on the role of different forms of mental state understanding in children’s evaluations of inequalities based on gender.

以往的研究表明,与道德相关的心智理论能使儿童避免将意外的过失归咎于同伴。本研究调查了这种形式的心智理论是否有助于儿童认识到性别不平等是不公平的,并会造成负面情绪体验。此外,本研究还从三个角度(从自身角度、从因不平等而处于有利地位的角度以及从因不平等而处于不利地位的角度)考察了这种能力。参与研究的 141 名儿童(平均年龄为 6.67 岁,49% 为女性,32% 为少数族裔)来自美国大西洋中部地区。经历过性别偏见的负面影响以及对心理状态有更深刻的理解,与对性别不平等的负面评价和对他人情绪的中性归因有关。这些发现揭示了不同形式的心理状态理解在儿童评价性别不平等中的作用。
{"title":"Morally-relevant theory of mind is related to viewing gender inequalities as unacceptable","authors":"Jacquelyn Glidden,&nbsp;Kathryn M. Yee,&nbsp;Melanie Killen","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101450","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101450","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous research has shown that morally-relevant theory of mind enables children to avoid blaming a peer for an accidental transgression. The current study investigated whether this form of theory of mind helps children recognize that gender inequalities are unfair and create negative emotional experiences. Further, the study examined this ability across three perspectives (for themselves, for those who have been advantaged by inequality, and for those who have been disadvantaged by inequality). Participants were 141 children (<em>M</em><sub>Age</sub> = 6.67 years, 49% female, 32% ethnic/racial minority) recruited from the mid-Atlantic region of the U.S. Experience with the negative consequences of gender bias and more advanced mental state understanding was associated with more negative evaluations of gender inequalities and more neutral attributions of others’ emotions. These findings shed light on the role of different forms of mental state understanding in children’s evaluations of inequalities based on gender.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140893481","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Modulation of attentional blink with sound in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) 声音对注意力缺陷多动障碍(ADHD)儿童眨眼的调节作用
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101452
Huan Zhang , Shizhong Cai , Yan Chen , Aijun Wang

Although it has been found that children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) with impaired ability of inhibition control often showed greater attentional blink (AB), there is minimal information in regards to attention deficit in the interaction of AB enhancement with sound induction. A rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task was used by presenting an audiology pure tone evaluation simultaneously with T2 to investigate how auditory-driven visual perception enhances AB in children with ADHD. The results showed that children with ADHD had greater AB, and they performed worse than typically developing children (TD) in AB amplitude and recovery time. In addtion, the sound enhanced AB in TD children but interfered in children with ADHD. These results indicate that children with ADHD with faulty attentional allocation have fewer resources available for targets in the cross-modality AB, thus leading to a severe interference effect and a larger AB. It demonstrates that sound can only enhance AB when attentional resources are previously invested and the ability to suppress interference are still sufficient after dividing attention in cross-modality.

尽管研究发现,抑制控制能力受损的注意力缺陷多动障碍(ADHD)儿童通常会表现出更多的注意力眨眼(AB),但关于注意力缺陷与声音诱导增强AB相互作用的信息却很少。研究人员利用快速序列视觉呈现(RSVP)任务,在T2的同时呈现听觉纯音评估,以研究听觉驱动的视觉感知如何增强多动症儿童的注意力眨眼。结果显示,ADHD 儿童的 AB 更大,他们在 AB 振幅和恢复时间方面的表现不如发育正常的儿童(TD)。此外,声音会增强 TD 儿童的 AB 感知,但会干扰 ADHD 儿童的 AB 感知。这些结果表明,注意力分配失误的多动症儿童在跨模态 AB 中可用于目标的资源较少,因此导致了严重的干扰效应和更大的 AB。这表明,只有当先前投入的注意资源和抑制干扰的能力在跨模态注意力分配后仍然足够时,声音才能增强AB。
{"title":"Modulation of attentional blink with sound in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)","authors":"Huan Zhang ,&nbsp;Shizhong Cai ,&nbsp;Yan Chen ,&nbsp;Aijun Wang","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101452","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101452","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Although it has been found that children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) with impaired ability of inhibition control often showed greater attentional blink (AB), there is minimal information in regards to attention deficit in the interaction of AB enhancement with sound induction. A rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task was used by presenting an audiology pure tone evaluation simultaneously with T2 to investigate how auditory-driven visual perception enhances AB in children with ADHD. The results showed that children with ADHD had greater AB, and they performed worse than typically developing children (TD) in AB amplitude and recovery time. In addtion, the sound enhanced AB in TD children but interfered in children with ADHD. These results indicate that children with ADHD with faulty attentional allocation have fewer resources available for targets in the cross-modality AB, thus leading to a severe interference effect and a larger AB. It demonstrates that sound can only enhance AB when attentional resources are previously invested and the ability to suppress interference are still sufficient after dividing attention in cross-modality.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140909957","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of mathematical vocabulary in the development of mathematical skills for Spanish-speaking students 数学词汇对西班牙语学生数学能力发展的作用
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101441
María Inés Susperreguy , Sabrina M. Di Lonardo Burr , Chang Xu , Heather P. Douglas , Taeko Bourque , M. Francisca del Río , Viviana Salinas , Jo-Anne LeFevre

Does mathematical vocabulary predict the change in students’ performance on mathematical tasks from one academic year to the next? Chilean Spanish-speaking students (N = 87) completed measures of mathematical vocabulary, mathematical skills (i.e., arithmetic fluency, calculation, and applied problems), receptive vocabulary, and working memory in Grade 2 (T1, Mage = 7:11 years:months, SD = 0:5, 46% girls). One year later (T2) they completed the same mathematical measures. Concurrent relations were found between mathematical vocabulary and the three mathematical skills at both time points. Together, general and mathematical vocabulary at T1 explained significant unique variance in the change in applied problems and calculation from T1 to T2. For calculation however, only mathematical vocabulary predicted significant unique variance in the change from T1 to T2. Change in arithmetic fluency was only predicted by working memory. These results address the roles of general and mathematical vocabulary in students’ mathematical development in elementary school.

数学词汇能否预测学生在数学任务上的表现从一学年到下一学年的变化?智利西班牙语学生(人数=87)在二年级(T1,年龄=7:11岁:月,SD=0:5,女生占46%)完成了数学词汇、数学技能(即算术流利度、计算和应用问题)、接受词汇和工作记忆的测量。一年后(T2),他们完成了同样的数学测量。在这两个时间点,数学词汇量与三种数学技能之间都存在并发关系。从 T1 到 T2,T1 阶段的普通词汇量和数学词汇量共同解释了应用题和计算能力的显著变化。然而,在计算方面,只有数学词汇能预测从 T1 到 T2 的变化中的显著独特差异。只有工作记忆能预测算术流畅性的变化。这些结果说明了一般词汇和数学词汇在小学学生数学发展中的作用。
{"title":"The role of mathematical vocabulary in the development of mathematical skills for Spanish-speaking students","authors":"María Inés Susperreguy ,&nbsp;Sabrina M. Di Lonardo Burr ,&nbsp;Chang Xu ,&nbsp;Heather P. Douglas ,&nbsp;Taeko Bourque ,&nbsp;M. Francisca del Río ,&nbsp;Viviana Salinas ,&nbsp;Jo-Anne LeFevre","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101441","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101441","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Does mathematical vocabulary predict the change in students’ performance on mathematical tasks from one academic year to the next? Chilean Spanish-speaking students (<em>N</em> = 87) completed measures of mathematical vocabulary, mathematical skills (i.e., arithmetic fluency, calculation, and applied problems), receptive vocabulary, and working memory in Grade 2 (T1, <em>M</em><sub><em>age</em></sub> = 7:11 years:months, <em>SD</em> = 0:5, 46% girls)<em>.</em> One year later (T2) they completed the same mathematical measures. Concurrent relations were found between mathematical vocabulary and the three mathematical skills at both time points. Together, general and mathematical vocabulary at T1 explained significant unique variance in the change in applied problems and calculation from T1 to T2. For calculation however, only mathematical vocabulary predicted significant unique variance in the change from T1 to T2. Change in arithmetic fluency was only predicted by working memory. These results address the roles of general and mathematical vocabulary in students’ mathematical development in elementary school.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140350040","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Two- and three-year-olds prefer mastery-oriented over outcome-oriented help 与注重结果的帮助相比,两三岁的幼儿更喜欢注重掌握的帮助
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101462
Alexandra Raport , Canan Ipek, Valentina Gomez, Henrike Moll

It has been shown that school-aged children often turn to others for help with the motive to learn and expand on their knowledge or skills (so-called “mastery-oriented” help-seeking, Nelson-Le Gall, 1985). Although help-seeking in problem-solving contexts is increasingly studied in preschoolers, there is so far no experimental evidence that young children show a preference for mastery- as opposed to outcome-driven help. In two online experiments (N = 144, 72 female), 2- and 3-year-olds from diverse ethno-racial backgrounds living in the United States (68 % White; 16 % Latine) watched an agent failing at a task (e.g., pouring water into a cup) before they selected one of two helpers for the agent: one who demonstrated how to perform the task (mastery-oriented helper) or one who completed the task without demonstration (outcome-oriented helper). Children of both ages selected mastery-oriented helpers to assist the struggling agent (Experiments 1 and 2) but chose helpers randomly in a control condition in which the agent succeeded at the task and therefore needed no mastery-oriented help (Experiment 2). The findings indicate that young children recognize that furthering one’s abilities is a core motive of help-seeking.

研究表明,学龄儿童往往出于学习和扩展知识或技能的动机而向他人寻求帮助(即所谓的 "以掌握为导向 "的寻求帮助,Nelson-Le Gall,1985 年)。尽管对学龄前儿童在问题解决情境中寻求帮助的研究越来越多,但迄今为止,还没有实验证据表明,相对于以结果为导向的帮助,幼儿更倾向于以掌握知识为导向的帮助。在两个在线实验中(N = 144,72 名女性),来自美国不同种族背景的两岁和三岁幼儿(68 % 白人;16 % 拉丁人)观看了一个代理失败的任务(例如,把水倒进杯子里),然后他们为代理选择了两个帮助者之一:一个是演示如何完成任务的帮助者(以掌握为导向的帮助者),另一个是不演示就完成任务的帮助者(以结果为导向的帮助者)。两个年龄段的幼儿都选择了以掌握为导向的帮助者来帮助陷入困境的代理(实验 1 和 2),但在对照条件下,他们是随机选择帮助者的,在对照条件下,代理成功完成了任务,因此不需要以掌握为导向的帮助(实验 2)。研究结果表明,幼儿认识到提高自身能力是寻求帮助的核心动机。
{"title":"Two- and three-year-olds prefer mastery-oriented over outcome-oriented help","authors":"Alexandra Raport ,&nbsp;Canan Ipek,&nbsp;Valentina Gomez,&nbsp;Henrike Moll","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101462","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101462","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>It has been shown that school-aged children often turn to others for help with the motive to learn and expand on their knowledge or skills (so-called “mastery-oriented” help-seeking, Nelson-Le Gall, 1985). Although help-seeking in problem-solving contexts is increasingly studied in preschoolers, there is so far no experimental evidence that young children show a preference for mastery- as opposed to outcome-driven help. In two online experiments (N = 144, 72 female), 2- and 3-year-olds from diverse ethno-racial backgrounds living in the United States (68 % White; 16 % Latine) watched an agent failing at a task (e.g., pouring water into a cup) before they selected one of two helpers for the agent: one who demonstrated how to perform the task (mastery-oriented helper) or one who completed the task without demonstration (outcome-oriented helper). Children of both ages selected mastery-oriented helpers to assist the struggling agent (Experiments 1 and 2) but chose helpers randomly in a control condition in which the agent succeeded at the task and therefore needed no mastery-oriented help (Experiment 2). The findings indicate that young children recognize that furthering one’s abilities is a core motive of help-seeking.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141163766","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Gender differences in global advantage effect in school-aged children 学龄儿童全球优势效应的性别差异
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101451
Rosario Gelpi-Trudo , Santiago Vernucci , Ana Virginia García-Coni , Hernán López-Morales , Lorena Canet-Juric

This study investigated age and gender differences in global-local processing (the perception of parts forming a whole) in middle childhood. While there is a consensus on a global advantage effect regarding priority, speed, and accuracy of processing, its universality is debated. A Navon-type selective attention task with hierarchical stimuli was administered to 199 students aged 9 to 12 years. Repeated measures mixed ANCOVA with reaction times and accuracy scores showed that only boys exhibited a global advantage effect in speed, while girls demonstrated higher accuracy in both global and local conditions. A trade-off effect was suggested by a complementary analysis using Balanced Integration Score. An improvement with age was found regarding speed and overall performance, with no differential effect between hierarchical levels. These results are presented in the hopes of providing information on gender-specific challenges that can be mitigated through future interventions.

本研究调查了儿童中期在全局-局部处理(对构成整体的部分的感知)方面的年龄和性别差异。尽管在处理的优先性、速度和准确性方面,人们对全局优势效应已达成共识,但对其普遍性还存在争议。研究人员对 199 名 9 至 12 岁的学生进行了分层刺激的纳冯式选择性注意任务。反应时间和准确性得分的重复测量混合方差分析显示,只有男生在速度上表现出整体优势效应,而女生在整体和局部条件下都表现出更高的准确性。利用平衡综合评分进行的补充分析表明了一种权衡效应。随着年龄的增长,速度和总成绩都有所提高,但不同等级之间没有差异。介绍这些结果是希望提供有关性别特有挑战的信息,以便通过未来的干预措施减轻这些挑战。
{"title":"Gender differences in global advantage effect in school-aged children","authors":"Rosario Gelpi-Trudo ,&nbsp;Santiago Vernucci ,&nbsp;Ana Virginia García-Coni ,&nbsp;Hernán López-Morales ,&nbsp;Lorena Canet-Juric","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101451","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101451","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study investigated age and gender differences in global-local processing (the perception of parts forming a whole) in middle childhood. While there is a consensus on a global advantage effect regarding priority, speed, and accuracy of processing, its universality is debated. A Navon-type selective attention task with hierarchical stimuli was administered to 199 students aged 9 to 12 years. Repeated measures mixed ANCOVA with reaction times and accuracy scores showed that only boys exhibited a global advantage effect in speed, while girls demonstrated higher accuracy in both global and local conditions. A trade-off effect was suggested by a complementary analysis using Balanced Integration Score. An improvement with age was found regarding speed and overall performance, with no differential effect between hierarchical levels. These results are presented in the hopes of providing information on gender-specific challenges that can be mitigated through future interventions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140893480","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can you teach me how to open the box?: Examining young children’s teaching behavior toward adults with and without disabilities 你能教我怎么打开盒子吗?考察幼儿对残疾和非残疾成人的教学行为
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101449
Katherine M. Walton , Alayna R. Borowy , Madison L. Fecher , Laura Wagner

This study examined how children (31–71 months; N = 85) adjusted their teaching behaviors with three learners, each posing different communicative challenges, in comparison to a control learner, who showed no communicative challenge. With a Disabled learner, children showed fewer explicit teaching behaviors, and more eye contact with their parents and/or the primary experimenter as compared to when teaching a Control learner. With a Tired learner who followed identical scripted actions as the Disabled learner, children’s overall rate of teaching behaviors was largely unchanged. With a Spanish Speaking learner, children spoke less frequently but showed more frequent eye contact with the learner. These results demonstrate that while preschool children can modify their teaching strategies to adapt to the needs of different learners, they appear to teach less effectively and show markers of uncertainty when teaching to learners with disabilities.

本研究考察了儿童(31-71 个月;N = 85)在面对三名学习者(每名学习者都面临不同的交际挑战)时,与面对一名没有交际挑战的对照组学习者时,如何调整自己的教学行为。与教导对照组学习者时相比,在教导残疾学习者时,儿童的明确教学行为较少,与父母和/或主要实验者的眼神接触较多。与一名 "疲倦 "学习者进行教学时,儿童的总体教学行为率基本保持不变。对于讲西班牙语的学习者,儿童说话的频率较低,但与学习者的眼神接触却更频繁。这些结果表明,虽然学龄前儿童可以根据不同学习者的需要修改他们的教学策略,但在对残疾学习者进行教学时,他们的教学效果似乎较差,并表现出不确定性。
{"title":"Can you teach me how to open the box?: Examining young children’s teaching behavior toward adults with and without disabilities","authors":"Katherine M. Walton ,&nbsp;Alayna R. Borowy ,&nbsp;Madison L. Fecher ,&nbsp;Laura Wagner","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101449","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101449","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study examined how children (31–71 months; <em>N</em> = 85) adjusted their teaching behaviors with three learners, each posing different communicative challenges, in comparison to a control learner, who showed no communicative challenge. With a Disabled learner, children showed fewer explicit teaching behaviors, and more eye contact with their parents and/or the primary experimenter as compared to when teaching a Control learner. With a Tired learner who followed identical scripted actions as the Disabled learner, children’s overall rate of teaching behaviors was largely unchanged. With a Spanish Speaking learner, children spoke less frequently but showed more frequent eye contact with the learner. These results demonstrate that while preschool children can modify their teaching strategies to adapt to the needs of different learners, they appear to teach less effectively and show markers of uncertainty when teaching to learners with disabilities.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000340/pdfft?md5=76cab14c0e05affb1e6904f865493b4e&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000340-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140905360","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children’s biological causal models of disability 儿童对残疾的生物学因果模型
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101448
David Menendez , Susan A. Gelman

The term “disability” encompasses many conditions (including a range of learning, intellectual, physical, sensory and socioemotional disorders) that can be caused by a variety of genetic, environmental, and unknown factors. We examine how children reason about the biological nature of disabilities, specifically the extent to which they use 'essentialist', 'infectious disease', or 'bodily damage' causal models. These models provide competing predictions regarding the biological nature of disability. The essentialist model views disabilities as caused by an internal essence, akin to genes, and entails thinking of disabilities as stable, immutable, and inheritable. The infectious disease model views disabilities as communicable, abnormal, and needing intervention. The bodily damage model views disabilities as resulting from injuries or toxins, which maybe stable but are not inheritable or transmissible. We review what is known about children's acquisition of these models, and discuss how disentangling these biological models is a fruitful avenue for future research.

残疾 "一词包含许多情况(包括一系列学习、智力、身体、感官和社会情感障碍),可由各种遗传、环境和未知因素造成。我们研究了儿童如何推理残疾的生物学性质,特别是他们在多大程度上使用了 "本质论"、"传染病 "或 "身体损伤 "因果模型。这些模式对残疾的生物学性质做出了相互竞争的预测。本质主义模式认为残疾是由类似基因的内在本质造成的,并认为残疾是稳定的、不可改变的和可遗传的。传染病模式认为残疾是可传染的、不正常的、需要干预的。身体损伤模式认为残疾是由伤害或毒素造成的,这些伤害或毒素可能是稳定的,但不能遗传或传播。我们回顾了目前已知的有关儿童获得这些模式的知识,并讨论了如何将这些生物模式区分开来是未来研究的一个富有成效的途径。
{"title":"Children’s biological causal models of disability","authors":"David Menendez ,&nbsp;Susan A. Gelman","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101448","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101448","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The term “disability” encompasses many conditions (including a range of learning, intellectual, physical, sensory and socioemotional disorders) that can be caused by a variety of genetic, environmental, and unknown factors. We examine how children reason about the biological nature of disabilities, specifically the extent to which they use 'essentialist', 'infectious disease', or 'bodily damage' causal models. These models provide competing predictions regarding the biological nature of disability. The essentialist model views disabilities as caused by an internal essence, akin to genes, and entails thinking of disabilities as stable, immutable, and inheritable. The infectious disease model views disabilities as communicable, abnormal, and needing intervention. The bodily damage model views disabilities as resulting from injuries or toxins, which maybe stable but are not inheritable or transmissible. We review what is known about children's acquisition of these models, and discuss how disentangling these biological models is a fruitful avenue for future research.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140950880","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1