首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Development最新文献

英文 中文
Selection of information necessary for successful self-derivation 成功自我推导所需信息的选择
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101364
Jessica A. Dugan, Katherine Lee, Melanie H. Hanft, Patricia J. Bauer

Accumulation of knowledge relies in part on self-derivation of new semantic knowledge through integration of separate yet related learning episodes. Prior research suggests that individual and developmental variability in self-derivation is due to differences in the precursor processes of encoding, reactivation, and integration. In the present research, we examined a fourth potential precursor process: selection of learning episodes most relevant to the target self-derived knowledge. In two experiments, we examined selection of information most relevant to self-derivation in 8-year-olds (Experiments 1 and 2) and 12-year-olds (Experiment 2). Both age groups self-derived even when there were several candidate facts from which to select. Older children had higher levels of self-derivation performance and made more correct selections than younger children. Within and across age groups, selection performance significantly predicted trial-level self-derivation success. These data provide evidence that selection of facts necessary for self-derivation contributes to the robust variability observed in self-derivation.

知识的积累部分依赖于新的语义知识的自我衍生,通过整合独立但相关的学习情节。先前的研究表明,自我衍生的个体和发育差异是由于编码、再激活和整合的前体过程的差异。在本研究中,我们研究了第四个潜在的前驱过程:选择与目标自我衍生知识最相关的学习情节。在两个实验中,我们研究了8岁儿童(实验1和2)和12岁儿童(实验2)对与自我推导最相关的信息的选择。即使有几个候选事实可供选择,这两个年龄组也会自我推导。年龄较大的儿童比年龄较小的儿童具有更高水平的自我推导表现,并且做出了更多的正确选择。在年龄组内和跨年龄组,选择表现显著预测试验水平的自我衍生成功。这些数据提供了证据,证明自推导所必需的事实的选择有助于自推导中观察到的鲁棒变异性。
{"title":"Selection of information necessary for successful self-derivation","authors":"Jessica A. Dugan,&nbsp;Katherine Lee,&nbsp;Melanie H. Hanft,&nbsp;Patricia J. Bauer","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101364","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101364","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Accumulation of knowledge relies in part on self-derivation of new semantic knowledge through integration of separate yet related learning episodes. Prior research suggests that individual and developmental variability in self-derivation is due to differences in the precursor processes of encoding, reactivation, and integration. In the present research, we examined a fourth potential precursor process: selection of learning episodes most relevant to the target self-derived knowledge. In two experiments, we examined selection of information most relevant to self-derivation in 8-year-olds (Experiments 1 and 2) and 12-year-olds (Experiment 2). Both age groups self-derived even when there were several candidate facts from which to select. Older children had higher levels of self-derivation performance and made more correct selections than younger children. Within and across age groups, selection performance significantly predicted trial-level self-derivation success. These data provide evidence that selection of facts necessary for self-derivation contributes to the robust variability observed in self-derivation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"68 ","pages":"Article 101364"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41372864","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children’s executive function during the COVID-19 pandemic in Argentina: Associations with home literacy, reading, and screen times COVID-19大流行期间阿根廷儿童的执行功能:与家庭扫盲、阅读和屏幕时间的关系
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101378
Ángel Tabullo , Lorena Canet-Juric , Valeria Abusamra

Several studies indicated that the COVID-19 pandemic and the containment measures it required (including social distancing, quarantine and school closure) had a significant impact on children’s mental health. The present study aimed to examine executive function difficulties at behavioural level in school children during the COVID-19 lockdown, and to analyze potential associations with home literacy environment, current reading and screen times. Data were collected from mothers of 210 children (9–12 years old) through an online survey. Incidence of EF issues was higher for fourth graders in the flexibility and working memory domains, possibly reflecting developmental differences. Significant increases in children screen times were observed, while most of them did not read for pleasure on a daily basis. Parents’ literacy beliefs and children’s current leisure reading times were negative predictors of executive function difficulties (with reading times partially mediating literacy beliefs’ effects), which increased with videogame times (particularly in 4th graders). Nevertheless, perceived changes on screen or reading times with respect to prepandemic levels were not associated with executive function scores. The results might indicate: 1) opposite effects of literacy and video game times over children’s executive functioning; 2) a preference for reading or screen recreational use according to their executive function profiles; or 3) a combination of both. Our findings highlight the relation of home literacy environment, reading and screen times with children’s cognitive development, and the importance of following their trajectory during postpandemic times.

几项研究表明,新冠肺炎大流行及其所需的遏制措施(包括保持社交距离、隔离和关闭学校)对儿童的心理健康产生了重大影响。本研究旨在研究新冠肺炎封锁期间学校儿童在行为层面的执行功能困难,并分析与家庭识字环境、当前阅读和屏幕时间的潜在关联。数据是通过一项在线调查从210名儿童(9-12岁)的母亲那里收集的。四年级学生在灵活性和工作记忆领域的EF问题发生率较高,这可能反映了发展差异。观察到孩子们看屏幕的时间显著增加,而他们中的大多数人每天都不是为了娱乐而阅读。父母的识字信念和孩子目前的休闲阅读时间是执行功能困难的负面预测因素(阅读时间部分中介了识字信念的影响),执行功能困难随着电子游戏时间的增加而增加(尤其是在四年级学生中)。然而,相对于疫情前水平,屏幕或阅读时间的感知变化与执行功能得分无关。研究结果可能表明:1)识字率和电子游戏时间对儿童执行功能的影响相反;2) 根据他们的执行功能档案,对阅读或屏幕娱乐用途的偏好;或3)两者的组合。我们的研究结果强调了家庭识字环境、阅读和屏幕时间与儿童认知发展的关系,以及在疫情后时期遵循他们轨迹的重要性。
{"title":"Children’s executive function during the COVID-19 pandemic in Argentina: Associations with home literacy, reading, and screen times","authors":"Ángel Tabullo ,&nbsp;Lorena Canet-Juric ,&nbsp;Valeria Abusamra","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101378","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101378","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Several studies indicated that the COVID-19 pandemic and the containment measures it required (including social distancing, quarantine and school closure) had a significant impact on children’s mental health. The present study aimed to examine executive function difficulties at behavioural level in school children during the COVID-19 lockdown, and to analyze potential associations with home literacy environment, current reading and screen times. Data were collected from mothers of 210 children (9–12 years old) through an online survey. Incidence of EF issues was higher for fourth graders in the flexibility and working memory domains, possibly reflecting developmental differences. Significant increases in children screen times were observed, while most of them did not read for pleasure on a daily basis. Parents’ literacy beliefs and children’s current leisure reading times were negative predictors of executive function difficulties (with reading times partially mediating literacy beliefs’ effects), which increased with videogame times (particularly in 4th graders). Nevertheless, perceived changes on screen or reading times with respect to prepandemic levels were not associated with executive function scores. The results might indicate: 1) opposite effects of literacy and video game times over children’s executive functioning; 2) a preference for reading or screen recreational use according to their executive function profiles; or 3) a combination of both. Our findings highlight the relation of home literacy environment, reading and screen times with children’s cognitive development, and the importance of following their trajectory during postpandemic times.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"68 ","pages":"Article 101378"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49718015","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Learning how to communicate: Does exposure to multiple languages promote children’s pragmatic abilities? A meta-analytic review 学习如何沟通:接触多种语言是否能提高儿童的语用能力?元分析综述
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101384
Elise van Wonderen, Kimberley Mulder, Judith Rispens, Josje Verhagen

Despite the often-reported finding that multilingual children may temporarily possess less advanced lexical or grammatical skills in at least one of their languages than monolingual peers, recent studies have found that exposure to multiple languages benefits children’s pragmatic development. To assess the generalizability of these findings, we conducted a meta-analysis of 29 studies that investigated various pragmatic abilities in both multilingual and monolingual children. In addition, we classified the investigated pragmatic abilities into three broad categories: (i) sensitivity to non-verbal information, (ii) understanding non-literal language, and (iii) informativeness in referencing. We found no effect of multilingualism on pragmatic abilities overall and also not in any of the categories separately. However, based on visual inspection of the effect sizes, we cannot rule out that multilingual children sometimes rely more on non-verbal cues during communication compared to their monolingual peers. We recommend future studies to formulate and test more specific hypotheses and to move away from the focus on a multilingual advantage.

尽管经常报道的发现是,多语言儿童在至少一种语言方面可能暂时不如单语儿童拥有更高级的词汇或语法技能,但最近的研究发现,接触多种语言有利于儿童的语用发展。为了评估这些发现的可推广性,我们对29项研究进行了荟萃分析,这些研究调查了多语和单语儿童的各种语用能力。此外,我们将所调查的语用能力分为三大类:(i)对非语言信息的敏感性,(ii)理解非文字语言,以及(iii)引用中的信息性。我们没有发现使用多种语言对整体语用能力的影响,也没有单独在任何一个类别中发现。然而,基于对效果大小的视觉检查,我们不能排除,与单语儿童相比,多语儿童有时在交流中更多地依赖非语言线索。我们建议未来的研究制定和测试更具体的假设,并摆脱对多语言优势的关注。
{"title":"Learning how to communicate: Does exposure to multiple languages promote children’s pragmatic abilities? A meta-analytic review","authors":"Elise van Wonderen,&nbsp;Kimberley Mulder,&nbsp;Judith Rispens,&nbsp;Josje Verhagen","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101384","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101384","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Despite the often-reported finding that multilingual children may temporarily possess less advanced lexical or grammatical skills in at least one of their languages than monolingual peers, recent studies have found that exposure to multiple languages benefits children’s pragmatic development. To assess the generalizability of these findings, we conducted a meta-analysis of 29 studies that investigated various pragmatic abilities in both multilingual and monolingual children. In addition, we classified the investigated pragmatic abilities into three broad categories: (i) sensitivity to non-verbal information, (ii) understanding non-literal language, and (iii) informativeness in referencing. We found no effect of multilingualism on pragmatic abilities overall and also not in any of the categories separately. However, based on visual inspection of the effect sizes, we cannot rule out that multilingual children sometimes rely more on non-verbal cues during communication compared to their monolingual peers. We recommend future studies to formulate and test more specific hypotheses and to move away from the focus on a multilingual advantage.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"68 ","pages":"Article 101384"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49718018","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children’s and adolescents’ judgments of the desirability and obligatoriness of prosocial action: Variations across helping, sharing, and comforting 儿童和青少年对亲社会行为的可取性和强制性的判断:帮助、分享和安慰的差异
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101377
Nasim Tavassoli , Holly Recchia , Kristen Dunfield

This study examined whether the intensity of need and type of prosociality differentially predicted children’s and adolescents’ desirability, obligatoriness, and permissibility judgments of costly prosocial actions. A total of 165 8–10-year-old children and 13–15-year-old adolescents evaluated six hypothetical situations wherein one child needed to be helped, shared with, or comforted and another child decided whether or not to provide that assistance. Participants evaluated whether acting prosocially was desirable-obligatory, desirable-nonobligatory, undesirable-permissible, or undesirable-impermissible, and their reasoning was coded. Prosociality was evaluated as desirable-obligatory more in high-need situations. Overall, children more than adolescents evaluated prosociality as desirable. When the need was high, comforting more than helping and sharing was evaluated as desirable, whereas when the need was low, sharing was evaluated as particularly desirable. The reasoning underlying judgments differed across types of prosociality. Findings are discussed in relation to how varied types of need are related to children’s and adolescents’ prosocial judgments.

本研究考察了需求强度和亲社会性类型是否对儿童和青少年对代价高昂的亲社会行为的可取性、强制性和可容许性判断有差异。共有165名8 - 10岁的儿童和13 - 15岁的青少年评估了六种假设的情况,其中一个孩子需要帮助、分享或安慰,另一个孩子决定是否提供这种帮助。参与者评估亲社会行为是可取的-强制性的,可取的-非强制性的,不可取的-允许的,还是不可取的-不允许的,他们的推理被编码。亲社会性在高需求情境下被评价为可取义务性。总的来说,儿童比青少年更多地认为亲社会是可取的。当需求高时,安慰多于帮助和分享被认为是可取的,而当需求低时,分享被认为是特别可取的。判断背后的推理在不同类型的亲社会行为中是不同的。研究结果讨论了不同类型的需求如何与儿童和青少年的亲社会判断相关。
{"title":"Children’s and adolescents’ judgments of the desirability and obligatoriness of prosocial action: Variations across helping, sharing, and comforting","authors":"Nasim Tavassoli ,&nbsp;Holly Recchia ,&nbsp;Kristen Dunfield","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101377","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101377","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study examined whether the intensity of need and type of prosociality differentially predicted children’s and adolescents’ desirability, obligatoriness, and permissibility judgments of costly prosocial actions. A total of 165 8–10-year-old children and 13–15-year-old adolescents evaluated six hypothetical situations wherein one child needed to be helped, shared with, or comforted and another child decided whether or not to provide that assistance. Participants evaluated whether acting prosocially was desirable-obligatory, desirable-nonobligatory, undesirable-permissible, or undesirable-impermissible, and their reasoning was coded. Prosociality was evaluated as desirable-obligatory more in high-need situations. Overall, children more than adolescents evaluated prosociality as desirable. When the need was high, comforting more than helping and sharing was evaluated as desirable, whereas when the need was low, sharing was evaluated as particularly desirable. The reasoning underlying judgments differed across types of prosociality. Findings are discussed in relation to how varied types of need are related to children’s and adolescents’ prosocial judgments.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"68 ","pages":"Article 101377"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"92046617","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The longitudinal contributions of preschool executive functions and early math abilities to arithmetic skills in elementary school 学前执行功能和早期数学能力对小学算术技能的纵向贡献
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101388
Kimia Akhavein , Caron A.C. Clark , Jennifer Mize Nelson , Kimberly Andrews Espy , Jenna E. Finch

Executive functions (EFs) are linked to children’s overall math performance, although few studies have considered the joint role of prior math abilities for specific math subskills, such as arithmetic. The current study examined the longitudinal contributions of preschool EFs and early math abilities to children’s accuracy and reaction time on arithmetic problems. Two hundred and eighty-three children completed EF and numeracy assessments at 5.25 years old. Children completed an arithmetic problem task in first (Mage = 7.14), second (Mage = 8.09), and third grade (Mage = 9.08). Results indicated that preschool EFs and math abilities are uniquely linked to children’s accuracy and reaction time at age 7, whereas preschool EFs alone continue to predict accuracy at age 8 and reaction time at age 9, even after accounting for intervening arithmetic performance. The study highlights the sustained, unique importance of early EFs for children’s arithmetic acquisition.

执行功能(EF)与儿童的整体数学表现有关,尽管很少有研究考虑先前数学能力对特定数学子技能(如算术)的共同作用。目前的研究考察了学前EFs和早期数学能力对儿童算术问题准确性和反应时间的纵向贡献。283名儿童在5.25岁时完成了EF和算术评估。儿童在一年级(Mage=7.14)、二年级(Mage=8.09)和三年级(Magy=9.08)完成了一项算术题任务。结果表明,学前EFs和数学能力与儿童7岁时的准确性和反应时间有着独特的联系,而仅学前EFs就可以继续预测8岁时的准确率和9岁时的反应时间,即使在考虑了中间的算术性能之后。这项研究强调了早期EFs对儿童算术习得的持续、独特的重要性。
{"title":"The longitudinal contributions of preschool executive functions and early math abilities to arithmetic skills in elementary school","authors":"Kimia Akhavein ,&nbsp;Caron A.C. Clark ,&nbsp;Jennifer Mize Nelson ,&nbsp;Kimberly Andrews Espy ,&nbsp;Jenna E. Finch","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101388","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101388","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Executive functions (EFs) are linked to children’s overall math performance, although few studies have considered the joint role of prior math abilities for specific math subskills, such as arithmetic. The current study examined the longitudinal contributions of preschool EFs and early math abilities to children’s accuracy and reaction time on arithmetic problems. Two hundred and eighty-three children completed EF and numeracy assessments at 5.25 years old. Children completed an arithmetic problem task in first (<em>M</em><sub><em>age</em></sub> = 7.14), second (<em>M</em><sub><em>age</em></sub> = 8.09), and third grade (<em>M</em><sub><em>age</em></sub> = 9.08). Results indicated that preschool EFs and math abilities are uniquely linked to children’s accuracy and reaction time at age 7, whereas preschool EFs alone continue to predict accuracy at age 8 and reaction time at age 9, even after accounting for intervening arithmetic performance. The study highlights the sustained, unique importance of early EFs for children’s arithmetic acquisition.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"68 ","pages":"Article 101388"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49718039","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How does intergroup familiarity moderate children’s merit-based resource allocation in the context of group-based competition? 在群体竞争背景下,群体间熟悉度如何调节儿童的择优资源分配?
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101387
Xue Xiao , Demao Zhao , Yanfang Li

Previous research has found that children can allocate resources according to competition outcomes (e.g., merit-based allocation) in interpersonal competitive contexts, but less research has investigated how children address merit-based allocations in the context of group-based competition, especially when influenced by intergroup familiarity. To address these issues, children (N = 374) aged 5–6 years old and 7–8 years old were asked to participate in an intergroup game with a familiar or unfamiliar group in the form of a competition (further divided into win and lose conditions) or noncompetition. They were then assessed on their resource allocation, reasoning and judgments of others’ allocations. We found that children as young as 5 years old can overcome group bias and adhere to allocating meritoriously in group-based competitive contexts. Furthermore, intergroup familiarity could decrease the effect of group bias on children’s merit-based allocation, but this tendency weakens with age. Finally, children consistently judged equal allocations positively regardless of actual allocation. The results indicated that with age, children were better able to weigh the moral concerns of merit and the social concerns of intergroup familiarity when determining the allocation of resources in group-based competition.

先前的研究发现,在人际竞争环境中,儿童可以根据竞争结果(例如,基于成绩的分配)来分配资源,但很少有研究调查儿童在基于群体的竞争环境中如何处理基于成绩的配置,尤其是在受到群体间熟悉度影响时。为了解决这些问题,5-6岁和7-8岁的儿童(N=374)被要求与熟悉或不熟悉的小组以竞争(进一步分为输赢条件)或非竞争的形式参加小组间游戏。然后对他们的资源分配、推理和对他人分配的判断进行评估。我们发现,5岁以下的儿童可以克服群体偏见,在基于群体的竞争环境中坚持择优分配。此外,群体间的熟悉度可以降低群体偏见对儿童基于成绩的分配的影响,但这种趋势随着年龄的增长而减弱。最后,无论实际分配情况如何,孩子们总是积极地判断平等分配。结果表明,随着年龄的增长,儿童在决定基于群体的竞争中的资源分配时,能够更好地权衡功绩的道德关注和群体间熟悉的社会关注。
{"title":"How does intergroup familiarity moderate children’s merit-based resource allocation in the context of group-based competition?","authors":"Xue Xiao ,&nbsp;Demao Zhao ,&nbsp;Yanfang Li","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101387","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101387","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous research has found that children can allocate resources according to competition outcomes (e.g., merit-based allocation) in interpersonal competitive contexts, but less research has investigated how children address merit-based allocations in the context of group-based competition, especially when influenced by intergroup familiarity. To address these issues, children (<em>N</em> = 374) aged 5–6 years old and 7–8 years old were asked to participate in an intergroup game with a familiar or unfamiliar group in the form of a competition (further divided into win and lose conditions) or noncompetition. They were then assessed on their resource allocation, reasoning and judgments of others’ allocations. We found that children as young as 5 years old can overcome group bias and adhere to allocating meritoriously in group-based competitive contexts. Furthermore, intergroup familiarity could decrease the effect of group bias on children’s merit-based allocation, but this tendency weakens with age. Finally, children consistently judged equal allocations positively regardless of actual allocation. The results indicated that with age, children were better able to weigh the moral concerns of merit and the social concerns of intergroup familiarity when determining the allocation of resources in group-based competition.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"68 ","pages":"Article 101387"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49708666","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Winning or losing: Children’s proportional reasoning across motivational contexts 输赢:儿童在动机背景下的比例推理
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101390
Karina Hamamouche , Sara Cordes

Children tend to prioritize whole number information over relational information in proportional reasoning tasks, such that they judge a spinner with 4/10 red pieces as more likely to land on red than a spinner with 2/3 red pieces, because 4 > 2 (e.g., Hurst & Cordes, 2018a; Jeong et al., 2007). This whole number bias is hypothesized to be a driven by fluency in verbal counting in early childhood, which is thought to promote attention to whole number information. In this study, we explored (1) the relation between verbal counting abilities and whole number biases and (2) whether distinct framing contexts – either encouraging children to maximize the number of stickers won, or minimizing the number of stickers lost - differentially impact children’s proportional reasoning. Three- to nine-year olds (N = 210, Mage = 5.7 years) chose which of two spinners they preferred to spin. Children in the Gain condition learned that if the spinner landed on red, they would win a sticker and if it landed on blue, nothing would happen; children in the Loss condition learned that if the spinner landed on red, nothing would happen, but if it landed on blue, they would lose a sticker. Counter to prior work, performance of both older (6–9 year olds) and younger (3–5 year olds) children revealed whole number biases. Notably, whole number biases were not related to counting abilities. Importantly, we find framing the task in a Loss scenario lessened whole number biases, suggesting that task framing can alter children’s attention to whole number information in a proportional reasoning context.

在比例推理任务中,儿童倾向于优先考虑整数信息,而不是关系信息,例如,他们判断拥有4/10个红色碎片的旋转器比拥有2/3个红色碎片的旋转器更有可能落在红色上,因为4 > 2(例如,Hurst &;Cordes, 2018;Jeong et al., 2007)。这种整数偏见被假设是由儿童早期语言计数的流畅性所驱动的,这被认为是促进对整数信息的关注。在这项研究中,我们探讨了(1)语言计数能力与整数偏差之间的关系,以及(2)不同的框架语境——鼓励儿童最大化赢得的贴纸数量或最小化失去的贴纸数量——是否会对儿童的比例推理产生不同的影响。三到九岁的孩子(N = 210,法师= 5.7岁)选择他们喜欢的两个纺纱机中的哪一个。获得组的孩子知道,如果转轮落在红色上,他们就会赢得一张贴纸,如果它落在蓝色上,什么也不会发生;“丢失”组的孩子了解到,如果旋转器落在红色上,什么也不会发生,但如果它落在蓝色上,他们就会丢失一张贴纸。与之前的研究相反,年龄较大(6-9岁)和年龄较小(3-5岁)的儿童的表现都显示出整数偏见。值得注意的是,整数偏差与计数能力无关。重要的是,我们发现在损失情境中构建任务减少了整数偏差,这表明任务框架可以改变儿童在比例推理情境中对整数信息的关注。
{"title":"Winning or losing: Children’s proportional reasoning across motivational contexts","authors":"Karina Hamamouche ,&nbsp;Sara Cordes","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101390","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101390","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Children tend to prioritize whole number information over relational information in proportional reasoning tasks, such that they judge a spinner with <span><math><mrow><mrow><mn>4</mn></mrow><mo>/</mo><mrow><mn>10</mn></mrow></mrow><mspace></mspace></math></span> red pieces as <em>more</em> likely to land on red than a spinner with <span><math><mrow><mrow><mn>2</mn></mrow><mo>/</mo><mrow><mn>3</mn></mrow></mrow></math></span> red pieces, because 4 &gt; 2 (e.g., Hurst &amp; Cordes, 2018a; Jeong et al., 2007). This whole number bias is hypothesized to be a driven by fluency in verbal counting in early childhood, which is thought to promote attention to whole number information. In this study, we explored (1) the relation between verbal counting abilities and whole number biases and (2) whether distinct framing contexts – either encouraging children to maximize the number of stickers won, or minimizing the number of stickers lost - differentially impact children’s proportional reasoning. Three- to nine-year olds (N = 210, <em>M</em><sub><em>age</em></sub> = 5.7 years) chose which of two spinners they preferred to spin. Children in the Gain condition learned that if the spinner landed on red, they would win a sticker and if it landed on blue, nothing would happen; children in the Loss condition learned that if the spinner landed on red, nothing would happen, but if it landed on blue, they would lose a sticker. Counter to prior work, performance of both older (6–9 year olds) and younger (3–5 year olds) children revealed whole number biases. Notably, whole number biases were not related to counting abilities. Importantly, we find framing the task in a Loss scenario lessened whole number biases, suggesting that task framing can alter children’s attention to whole number information in a proportional reasoning context.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"68 ","pages":"Article 101390"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"92046620","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children deny that God could change morality 孩子们否认上帝能改变道德
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101393
Madeline G. Reinecke , Larisa Heiphetz Solomon

Can moral rules change? We tested 129 children from the United States to investigate their beliefs about whether God could change widely shared moral propositions (e.g., “it’s not okay to call someone a mean name”), controversial moral propositions (e.g., “it’s not okay to tell a small lie to help someone feel happy”), and physical propositions (e.g., “fire is hotter than snow”). We observed an emerging tendency to report that God's ability to change morality is limited, suggesting that children across development find some widely shared aspects of morality to be impossible to change. Some beliefs did shift over development, however: 4- to 6-year-olds did not distinguish among God’s ability to change widely shared moral, controversial moral, and physical propositions, whereas 7- to 9-year-olds became increasingly confident that God could change physical and controversial moral propositions. Critically, however, younger children and older children alike reported that widely shared aspects of morality could not be altered. According to participants, not even God could change fundamental moral principles.

道德准则会改变吗?我们对来自美国的129名儿童进行了测试,以调查他们对上帝是否会改变广泛流传的道德命题(例如,“叫别人刻薄的名字是不对的”)、有争议的道德命题(例如,“为了让别人感到快乐而撒个小谎是不对的”)和物理命题(例如,“火比雪热”)的看法。我们观察到一种新趋势,即上帝改变道德的能力是有限的,这表明在整个发展过程中,儿童发现道德的一些普遍方面是不可能改变的。然而,有些信念确实会随着发展而改变:4到6岁的孩子无法区分上帝改变普遍存在的道德、有争议的道德和物理命题的能力,而7到9岁的孩子越来越相信上帝可以改变物理和有争议的道德命题。然而,至关重要的是,年龄较小的孩子和年龄较大的孩子都报告说,广泛共享的道德方面无法改变。根据参与者的说法,即使是上帝也不能改变基本的道德原则。
{"title":"Children deny that God could change morality","authors":"Madeline G. Reinecke ,&nbsp;Larisa Heiphetz Solomon","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101393","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101393","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Can moral rules change? We tested 129 children from the United States to investigate their beliefs about whether God could change widely shared moral propositions (e.g., “it’s not okay to call someone a mean name”), controversial moral propositions (e.g., “it’s not okay to tell a small lie to help someone feel happy”), and physical propositions (e.g., “fire is hotter than snow”). We observed an emerging tendency to report that God's ability to change morality is limited, suggesting that children across development find some widely shared aspects of morality to be impossible to change. Some beliefs did shift over development, however: 4- to 6-year-olds did not distinguish among God’s ability to change widely shared moral, controversial moral, and physical propositions, whereas 7- to 9-year-olds became increasingly confident that God could change physical and controversial moral propositions. Critically, however, younger children and older children alike reported that widely shared aspects of morality could not be altered. According to participants, not even God could change fundamental moral principles.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"68 ","pages":"Article 101393"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"92046621","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What contributes to false belief understanding in childhood? A multidimensional approach 是什么促成了儿童对错误信念的理解?多维方法
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101382
Olivier Aubuchon , Jamie Libenstein , Marina Moënner , Marilou Séguin , Jenny Bellerose , Annie Bernier , Miriam H. Beauchamp

False belief understanding (FBU), a core component of Theory of Mind (ToM), refers to the capacity to understand that other individuals act according to their beliefs even when those beliefs are inaccurate. FBU is an important aspect of socio-cognitive development in early childhood. A range of sociodemographic, temperamental, cognitive, and family factors are known to contribute separately to individual differences in FBU; however, these domains are typically studied in silos. This longitudinal study aimed to examine which of these factors are associated with FBU around the time of school entry. We evaluated 102 typically developing children at 2–5.5 years (predictive factors) and then at 4–7.5 years of age (FBU). The findings indicate that cognitive and family factors during the preschool years are associated with FBU at school entry. Specifically, child vocabulary and parent-child interaction quality were significant independent predictors of FBU.

错误信念理解(FBU)是心理理论的核心组成部分,指的是理解其他人根据自己的信念行事的能力,即使这些信念是不准确的。FBU是儿童早期社会认知发展的一个重要方面。已知一系列社会人口、气质、认知和家庭因素分别导致FBU的个体差异;然而,这些领域通常是在竖井中进行研究的。这项纵向研究旨在检验这些因素中的哪些与入学前后的FBU有关。我们评估了102名2-5岁(预测因素)和4-7.5岁(FBU)的典型发育儿童。研究结果表明,学龄前的认知和家庭因素与入学时的FBU有关。具体而言,儿童词汇和亲子互动质量是FBU的重要独立预测因素。
{"title":"What contributes to false belief understanding in childhood? A multidimensional approach","authors":"Olivier Aubuchon ,&nbsp;Jamie Libenstein ,&nbsp;Marina Moënner ,&nbsp;Marilou Séguin ,&nbsp;Jenny Bellerose ,&nbsp;Annie Bernier ,&nbsp;Miriam H. Beauchamp","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101382","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101382","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>False belief understanding (FBU), a core component of </span>Theory of Mind (ToM), refers to the capacity to understand that other individuals act according to their beliefs even when those beliefs are inaccurate. FBU is an important aspect of socio-cognitive development in early childhood. A range of sociodemographic, temperamental, cognitive, and family factors are known to contribute separately to individual differences in FBU; however, these domains are typically studied in silos. This longitudinal study aimed to examine which of these factors are associated with FBU around the time of school entry. We evaluated 102 typically developing children at 2–5.5 years (predictive factors) and then at 4–7.5 years of age (FBU). The findings indicate that cognitive and family factors during the preschool years are associated with FBU at school entry. Specifically, child vocabulary and parent-child interaction quality were significant independent predictors of FBU.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"68 ","pages":"Article 101382"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49708704","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
“Will I Get Sick?”: Parents’ explanations to children’s questions about a novel illness “我会生病吗?”:父母对孩子提出的关于一种新疾病的问题的解释
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101383
Seung Heon Yoo , Graciela Trujillo Hernandez , David Menendez , Rebecca E. Klapper , Sarah Martin , Katrina A. Nicholas , Dillanie Sumanthiran , Karl S. Rosengren

When encountered with a novel illness, children often ask for information about the illness and its impact on health from their parents. Although prior studies have explored how parents generally described the coronavirus to their children, there is an ambiguity in whether parents’ explanations about the coronavirus were about the coronavirus itself or about the pandemic more generally. Furthermore, it remains to be explored how parents responded to questions about the impact of the coronavirus on health. So, the current study explored how parents (N = 425) responded to specific questions that sought out information about the coronavirus and its impact. The results suggest that parents use their child’s specific questions as an opportunity to foster their child’s understanding about an illness. At the same time, parents also use their responses to shield their child from potential upsetting information about the impact of the coronavirus on health and well-being.

当遇到新的疾病时,孩子们经常向父母询问有关疾病及其对健康影响的信息。尽管之前的研究已经探讨了父母通常是如何向孩子描述冠状病毒的,但父母对冠状病毒的解释是关于冠状病毒本身还是关于更广泛的大流行,这一点尚不明确。此外,父母如何回答有关冠状病毒对健康影响的问题还有待探索。因此,目前的研究探讨了父母(N=425)如何回答特定问题,以寻求有关冠状病毒及其影响的信息。研究结果表明,父母利用孩子的具体问题来培养孩子对疾病的理解。与此同时,父母还利用他们的反应来保护孩子免受有关冠状病毒对健康和福祉影响的潜在令人不安的信息的影响。
{"title":"“Will I Get Sick?”: Parents’ explanations to children’s questions about a novel illness","authors":"Seung Heon Yoo ,&nbsp;Graciela Trujillo Hernandez ,&nbsp;David Menendez ,&nbsp;Rebecca E. Klapper ,&nbsp;Sarah Martin ,&nbsp;Katrina A. Nicholas ,&nbsp;Dillanie Sumanthiran ,&nbsp;Karl S. Rosengren","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101383","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101383","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>When encountered with a novel illness, children often ask for information about the illness and its impact on health from their parents. Although prior studies have explored how parents generally described the coronavirus to their children, there is an ambiguity in whether parents’ explanations about the coronavirus were about the coronavirus itself or about the pandemic more generally. Furthermore, it remains to be explored how parents responded to questions about the impact of the coronavirus on health. So, the current study explored how parents (<em>N</em> = 425) responded to specific questions that sought out information about the coronavirus and its impact. The results suggest that parents use their child’s specific questions as an opportunity to foster their child’s understanding about an illness. At the same time, parents also use their responses to shield their child from potential upsetting information about the impact of the coronavirus on health and well-being.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"68 ","pages":"Article 101383"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49708736","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1