首页 > 最新文献

Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale最新文献

英文 中文
Pupil dilation during recognition reflects the subjective recollection/familiarity experience at test rather than the level of processing at encoding. 识别时瞳孔扩张反映的是测试时的主观回忆/熟悉体验,而不是编码时的加工水平。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-05-12 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000283
Alexander Taikh, Glen E. Bodner
Pupil dilation provides a window into recognition memory processes. During a recognition test, the pupil dilates more in response to a recognized studied item than to a correctly rejected new item. Various explanations for this pupil old/new effect have been offered. By a retrieval effort account, the pupil's response on a recognition test reflects the cognitive effort needed to retrieve items from memory. By a memory strength account, pupil dilation reflects the strength of the subjective memory experience elicited by items at test. To compare these accounts, we varied levels of processing (LOP) at study, then measured pupil dilation on a delayed recognition test during which participants made recollection/familiarity judgements. Pupil dilation at test was similar whether test items had been studied in a deep or shallow LOP task, but was greater when deep, shallow, and new test items were experienced as recollected rather than as familiar. This pattern supports the memory strength account rather than the retrieval effort account of pupil dilation during a recognition test. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
瞳孔扩张为识别记忆过程提供了一个窗口。在识别测试中,瞳孔在被识别的研究物品上的反应比在被正确拒绝的新物品上的反应更大。对于这种学生新旧效应,人们提出了各种各样的解释。根据检索努力的说法,学生在识别测试中的反应反映了从记忆中检索项目所需的认知努力。根据记忆强度的说法,瞳孔扩张反映了测试项目引起的主观记忆体验的强度。为了比较这些描述,我们在研究中改变了处理水平(LOP),然后在延迟识别测试中测量瞳孔扩张,参与者在此期间进行回忆/熟悉度判断。无论测试项目是在深或浅LOP任务中研究的,测试时的瞳孔扩张都是相似的,但当深、浅和新测试项目被回忆而不是被熟悉时,瞳孔扩张更大。这种模式支持记忆强度解释,而不是在识别测试中瞳孔扩张的检索努力解释。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Pupil dilation during recognition reflects the subjective recollection/familiarity experience at test rather than the level of processing at encoding.","authors":"Alexander Taikh, Glen E. Bodner","doi":"10.1037/cep0000283","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000283","url":null,"abstract":"Pupil dilation provides a window into recognition memory processes. During a recognition test, the pupil dilates more in response to a recognized studied item than to a correctly rejected new item. Various explanations for this pupil old/new effect have been offered. By a retrieval effort account, the pupil's response on a recognition test reflects the cognitive effort needed to retrieve items from memory. By a memory strength account, pupil dilation reflects the strength of the subjective memory experience elicited by items at test. To compare these accounts, we varied levels of processing (LOP) at study, then measured pupil dilation on a delayed recognition test during which participants made recollection/familiarity judgements. Pupil dilation at test was similar whether test items had been studied in a deep or shallow LOP task, but was greater when deep, shallow, and new test items were experienced as recollected rather than as familiar. This pattern supports the memory strength account rather than the retrieval effort account of pupil dilation during a recognition test. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"112 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-05-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89539162","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Judgments of alphabetical order and mechanisms of congruity effects. 字母顺序判断与一致性效应机制。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-04-28 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000276
Yang S. Liu, J. Caplan
The congruity effect is a highly replicated feature of comparative judgments, and has been recently found in memory judgments of relative temporal order. Specifically, asking "Which came earlier?" versus "Which came later?" facilitates response times and sometimes error rates on judgments toward the beginning or end of the list, respectively. This suggests memory judgments of relative temporal order may be part of a broader class of comparative judgments. If so, the same congruity effect should also be found with the English alphabet, despite the alphabet being a longer, semantic-memory list, with forward directional encoding. A large-sample study (N = 340) produced a clear congruity effect in response time and even error rate (when controlled for response time). The large number of serial positions afforded by the alphabet enabled us to test a repertoire of mathematical models instantiating four distinct mechanisms of the congruity effect, against the empirical serial-position effects. The best-performing model assumed a response bias toward a discrete set of letters conceived of as "early" versus "late," respectively, an account that had previously been ruled out for typical comparative-judgment paradigms. In contrast, models implementing congruity effect mechanisms supported for conventional comparative judgment paradigms (based on reference-point theory or positional discriminability) produced quantitatively poorer fits, with more curvilinear serial-position effects that deviated from the data. The congruity effect thus extends to long, highly directional semantic-memory lists. However, qualitatively different serial-position effects across models suggest that, despite the superficial similarity, there are probably several quite different mechanisms that produce congruity effects, which may, in turn, depend on specific task characteristics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
一致性效应是比较判断的一个高度复制的特征,最近在相对时间顺序的记忆判断中被发现。具体来说,询问“哪个来得更早?”和“哪个来得更晚?”分别促进了对列表开头或结尾的判断的响应时间和有时错误率。这表明相对时间顺序的记忆判断可能是更广泛的比较判断的一部分。如果是这样的话,同样的一致性效应也应该在英语字母表中发现,尽管字母表是一个更长的语义记忆列表,具有正向编码。一项大样本研究(N = 340)在反应时间甚至错误率(在控制反应时间的情况下)上产生了明显的一致性效应。字母表提供的大量序列位置使我们能够测试一系列数学模型,这些模型实例化了同余效应的四种不同机制,而不是经验序列位置效应。表现最好的模型假设反应偏向于一组离散的字母,分别被认为是“早”和“晚”,这一解释在之前被典型的比较判断范式所排除。相比之下,实施一致性效应机制的模型支持传统的比较判断范式(基于参考点理论或位置可辨别性)产生的定量拟合较差,与数据偏离的曲线序列位置效应更多。因此,一致性效应扩展到长,高度定向的语义记忆列表。然而,不同模型的序列位置效应在质量上的不同表明,尽管表面上相似,但可能存在几种产生一致性效应的完全不同的机制,这可能反过来取决于特定的任务特征。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Judgments of alphabetical order and mechanisms of congruity effects.","authors":"Yang S. Liu, J. Caplan","doi":"10.1037/cep0000276","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000276","url":null,"abstract":"The congruity effect is a highly replicated feature of comparative judgments, and has been recently found in memory judgments of relative temporal order. Specifically, asking \"Which came earlier?\" versus \"Which came later?\" facilitates response times and sometimes error rates on judgments toward the beginning or end of the list, respectively. This suggests memory judgments of relative temporal order may be part of a broader class of comparative judgments. If so, the same congruity effect should also be found with the English alphabet, despite the alphabet being a longer, semantic-memory list, with forward directional encoding. A large-sample study (N = 340) produced a clear congruity effect in response time and even error rate (when controlled for response time). The large number of serial positions afforded by the alphabet enabled us to test a repertoire of mathematical models instantiating four distinct mechanisms of the congruity effect, against the empirical serial-position effects. The best-performing model assumed a response bias toward a discrete set of letters conceived of as \"early\" versus \"late,\" respectively, an account that had previously been ruled out for typical comparative-judgment paradigms. In contrast, models implementing congruity effect mechanisms supported for conventional comparative judgment paradigms (based on reference-point theory or positional discriminability) produced quantitatively poorer fits, with more curvilinear serial-position effects that deviated from the data. The congruity effect thus extends to long, highly directional semantic-memory lists. However, qualitatively different serial-position effects across models suggest that, despite the superficial similarity, there are probably several quite different mechanisms that produce congruity effects, which may, in turn, depend on specific task characteristics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-04-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"76687316","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
A computational model of item-based directed forgetting. 基于项目的定向遗忘的计算模型。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-04-28 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000281
J. Reid, Randall K. Jamieson
Montagliani and Hockley (2019) presented evidence that item-method directed forgetting not only leads to worse recognition of forget-cued targets than remember-cued targets but also better rejection of foils associated with forget-cued targets than remember-cued targets. Based on that result, they proposed that participants elaboratively encode more category-level information about R-cued targets. We present a retrieval-based explanation of the result within an instance-based memory model. The model imports word representations from two distributional semantic models, latent semantic analysis (LSA) and random permutation model (RPM), into an instance-based model of memory, MINERVA 2. The model reproduced Montagliani and Hockley's results without requiring assumptions about elaborated encoding of category-level information at study. The simulations demonstrate that whereas Montagliani and Hockley's findings are consistent with an account grounded in elaborated encoding of words at study, the results do not force that conclusion. Instead, better encoding of remember-cued targets at study establishes the conditions for retrieval-time effects at test to produce a corresponding influence on false recognition for category-related foils. Our model can be used as a formal tool to think about and study the incidental consequences of item directed forgetting in recognition memory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
Montagliani和Hockley(2019)提出的证据表明,项目法定向遗忘不仅导致对遗忘线索目标的识别比记忆线索目标差,而且比记忆线索目标更好地拒绝与遗忘线索目标相关的箔。基于这个结果,他们建议参与者仔细编码更多关于r提示目标的类别级信息。我们在基于实例的内存模型中给出了基于检索的结果解释。该模型将来自两个分布式语义模型(潜在语义分析(LSA)和随机排列模型(RPM))的单词表示导入到基于实例的内存模型MINERVA 2中。该模型重现了Montagliani和Hockley的结果,而不需要对研究中类别级信息的详细编码进行假设。模拟表明,尽管蒙塔利亚尼和霍克利的发现与研究中对单词进行详细编码的说法一致,但结果并不能支持这一结论。相反,在研究中对记忆线索目标进行更好的编码,为测试中的检索时间效应创造了条件,从而对类别相关箔的错误识别产生相应的影响。我们的模型可以作为一个正式的工具来思考和研究项目定向遗忘在识别记忆中的附带后果。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"A computational model of item-based directed forgetting.","authors":"J. Reid, Randall K. Jamieson","doi":"10.1037/cep0000281","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000281","url":null,"abstract":"Montagliani and Hockley (2019) presented evidence that item-method directed forgetting not only leads to worse recognition of forget-cued targets than remember-cued targets but also better rejection of foils associated with forget-cued targets than remember-cued targets. Based on that result, they proposed that participants elaboratively encode more category-level information about R-cued targets. We present a retrieval-based explanation of the result within an instance-based memory model. The model imports word representations from two distributional semantic models, latent semantic analysis (LSA) and random permutation model (RPM), into an instance-based model of memory, MINERVA 2. The model reproduced Montagliani and Hockley's results without requiring assumptions about elaborated encoding of category-level information at study. The simulations demonstrate that whereas Montagliani and Hockley's findings are consistent with an account grounded in elaborated encoding of words at study, the results do not force that conclusion. Instead, better encoding of remember-cued targets at study establishes the conditions for retrieval-time effects at test to produce a corresponding influence on false recognition for category-related foils. Our model can be used as a formal tool to think about and study the incidental consequences of item directed forgetting in recognition memory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"56 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-04-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"90908527","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Differential effects of agency, animacy, and syntactic prominence on production and comprehension: Evidence from a verb-initial language. 能动性、能动性和句法突出对产生和理解的不同影响:来自动词开头语言的证据。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-04-28 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000280
I. Bondoc, A. Schafer
Effects of animacy and agent prominence in linguistic and cognitive processing are well-established in the literature. However, it is less clear how strongly an agent argument will influence production and comprehension when a sentence also contains another prominent argument. We examine this question with Tagalog, a verb-initial language, which designates a syntactically prominent, subject-like element (the pivot) without demoting the grammatical status of the core agent. We implemented two experiments that investigated the influences of agent and pivot prominence on syntactic linear word order patterns in production and on anticipatory gaze patterns in comprehension. Tagalog's grammar allowed us to separate the influence of agentivity from animacy by manipulating the animacy of the pivot (animate pivots: agent and benefactive voices; inanimate pivots: patient and instrument voices). The production results contrasted with the comprehension results: agent and pivot prominence both emerged strongly in a fragment-completion production task, but animacy dominated anticipatory gaze patterns in a visual-world comprehension task. The results of these experiments demonstrate variability in production and comprehension outcomes as well as an apparent mismatch between the constraints that shape these two systems, which we attribute to contrasting goals in production versus comprehension and to the organization of information in verb-initial languages. The investigation highlights the value of research on languages with typologically understudied structural properties in revealing mechanisms of the production and comprehension systems. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
在语言和认知加工中,动画性和主体突出的影响在文献中已经得到了证实。然而,当一个句子还包含另一个突出的论点时,我们就不太清楚一个主体论点对产出和理解的影响有多大了。我们用他加禄语来研究这个问题,他加禄语是一种动词开头的语言,它指定了一个语法上突出的、类似主语的元素(支点),而不降低核心主体的语法地位。我们实施了两个实验,研究了主体和支点突出对生产过程中句法线性语序模式和理解过程中预期注视模式的影响。他加禄语的语法允许我们通过操纵枢轴的枢轴来分离能动性和animanim性的影响(有生命的枢轴:代理和有益的声音;无生命的支点:耐心和乐器的声音)。生成结果与理解结果形成对比:主体和支点突出在片段完成生成任务中都强烈出现,而动画在视觉世界理解任务中主导预期注视模式。这些实验的结果证明了产生和理解结果的可变性,以及形成这两个系统的约束之间的明显不匹配,我们将其归因于产生与理解的不同目标以及动词开头语言中的信息组织。本研究强调了对类型学上未被充分研究的语言结构特性的研究在揭示其产生和理解系统的机制方面的价值。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Differential effects of agency, animacy, and syntactic prominence on production and comprehension: Evidence from a verb-initial language.","authors":"I. Bondoc, A. Schafer","doi":"10.1037/cep0000280","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000280","url":null,"abstract":"Effects of animacy and agent prominence in linguistic and cognitive processing are well-established in the literature. However, it is less clear how strongly an agent argument will influence production and comprehension when a sentence also contains another prominent argument. We examine this question with Tagalog, a verb-initial language, which designates a syntactically prominent, subject-like element (the pivot) without demoting the grammatical status of the core agent. We implemented two experiments that investigated the influences of agent and pivot prominence on syntactic linear word order patterns in production and on anticipatory gaze patterns in comprehension. Tagalog's grammar allowed us to separate the influence of agentivity from animacy by manipulating the animacy of the pivot (animate pivots: agent and benefactive voices; inanimate pivots: patient and instrument voices). The production results contrasted with the comprehension results: agent and pivot prominence both emerged strongly in a fragment-completion production task, but animacy dominated anticipatory gaze patterns in a visual-world comprehension task. The results of these experiments demonstrate variability in production and comprehension outcomes as well as an apparent mismatch between the constraints that shape these two systems, which we attribute to contrasting goals in production versus comprehension and to the organization of information in verb-initial languages. The investigation highlights the value of research on languages with typologically understudied structural properties in revealing mechanisms of the production and comprehension systems. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"48 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-04-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"76397651","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Another look at the contribution of motoric fluency to metacognitive monitoring. 再看看运动流畅性对元认知监测的贡献。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-04-28 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000278
Michelle A Dollois, Cole J Poore-Buchhaupt, Chris M. Fiacconi
Recent evidence indicates that fluent motor interactions with studied information can increase confidence in how well that information will be later remembered, as measured by judgements of learning (JOLs). However, it remains unclear whether such metacognitive assessments are based on experienced motoric fluency or on explicit, analytic beliefs regarding the mnemonic impact of the experimental manipulations used to enhance fluency. Here, we introduce a new approach to examine the extent to which experience-based processes alone underlie this effect by manipulating motoric fluency outside of participants' awareness. Across two experiments, we examined typing speed for both real-word and nonword verbal stimuli following a training phase in which participants typed items consisting of a restricted subset of letters. Despite faster typing times (i.e., greater motoric fluency) for new items comprised of the same subset of letters used during training, we found no evidence that these items were perceived as more memorable. For real words, linear mixed-effect model analyses at the item level relating typing speed and JOLs also revealed no evidence that motoric fluency increased perceived memorability. Similar analyses conducted for nonwords did, however, reveal a modest positive relation between these constructs. Together, these findings suggest that the contribution of motoric fluency to metacognitive monitoring in the absence of analytic beliefs is minimal. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
最近的证据表明,通过学习判断(JOLs)来衡量,流畅的运动与学习信息的互动可以增加对以后记忆信息的信心。然而,目前尚不清楚这种元认知评估是基于经验运动流畅性还是基于关于用于提高流畅性的实验操作的助记影响的明确分析信念。在这里,我们引入了一种新的方法来检验基于经验的过程在多大程度上通过操纵参与者意识之外的运动流畅性来实现这种效果。在两个实验中,我们测试了真实单词和非单词语言刺激的打字速度,在这个训练阶段,参与者输入由有限的字母子集组成的项目。尽管在训练中使用的由相同字母子集组成的新项目的打字时间更快(即更大的运动流畅性),但我们没有发现证据表明这些项目被认为更容易记住。对于真实单词,项目水平的线性混合效应模型分析也显示,运动流畅性没有增加感知记忆的证据。然而,对非词进行的类似分析确实揭示了这些构念之间存在适度的正相关。总之,这些发现表明,在缺乏分析信念的情况下,运动流畅性对元认知监测的贡献是微乎其微的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Another look at the contribution of motoric fluency to metacognitive monitoring.","authors":"Michelle A Dollois, Cole J Poore-Buchhaupt, Chris M. Fiacconi","doi":"10.1037/cep0000278","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000278","url":null,"abstract":"Recent evidence indicates that fluent motor interactions with studied information can increase confidence in how well that information will be later remembered, as measured by judgements of learning (JOLs). However, it remains unclear whether such metacognitive assessments are based on experienced motoric fluency or on explicit, analytic beliefs regarding the mnemonic impact of the experimental manipulations used to enhance fluency. Here, we introduce a new approach to examine the extent to which experience-based processes alone underlie this effect by manipulating motoric fluency outside of participants' awareness. Across two experiments, we examined typing speed for both real-word and nonword verbal stimuli following a training phase in which participants typed items consisting of a restricted subset of letters. Despite faster typing times (i.e., greater motoric fluency) for new items comprised of the same subset of letters used during training, we found no evidence that these items were perceived as more memorable. For real words, linear mixed-effect model analyses at the item level relating typing speed and JOLs also revealed no evidence that motoric fluency increased perceived memorability. Similar analyses conducted for nonwords did, however, reveal a modest positive relation between these constructs. Together, these findings suggest that the contribution of motoric fluency to metacognitive monitoring in the absence of analytic beliefs is minimal. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"24 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-04-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"81996136","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A curved honulo improves your short-term and long-term memory. 弯曲的火虹可以改善你的短期和长期记忆。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-04-28 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000279
J. Saint-Aubin, D. Guitard, M. Poirier
During his distinguished career, Bill Hockley contributed to memory research in many ways, with work characterized by rigorous and innovative experimental designs. One of the areas he has explored is that of memory for associative information. We echo this interest here and attempt to emulate his careful experimental attitude. We report four experiments which examined how previously established links can support the development of new episodic associations. More specifically, we tested the idea that sound-symbolism links can support learning of new associations. Sound-symbolism links are relationships between phonemes and object characteristics that participants find natural-even if they have never encountered the items before. For instance, the nonword "honulo" is more readily seen to refer to a shape with curved contours than to a shape that has sharp angles. In Experiment 1, 70 participants studied three pairs and their memory for the associations between the members of each pair was tested in a paired-recognition task. Results demonstrate that sound-symbolism associations support the learning of new associations. Experiment 2 confirmed that the effect is replicated in a between-participants design. In Experiment 3, we replicated the findings with a 30-s filled interval between presentation and test, and in Experiment 4, we extended the delay to 2 min, establishing that the pattern is also found with a paradigm more typical of episodic memory. The results are discussed in terms of the importance of associative memory, while referring to some of the ideas Bill Hockley championed in his own work. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
在他杰出的职业生涯中,比尔·霍克利在许多方面对记忆研究做出了贡献,他的工作以严谨和创新的实验设计为特点。他探索的一个领域是联想信息的记忆。我们在这里呼应这种兴趣,并试图模仿他谨慎的实验态度。我们报告了四个实验,这些实验检查了先前建立的联系如何支持新情景关联的发展。更具体地说,我们测试了声音象征联系可以支持新联想学习的观点。声音符号联系是指音素和物体特征之间的关系,即使参与者以前从未见过这些物品,他们也会觉得很自然。例如,nonword“honulo”更容易被看作是指具有弯曲轮廓的形状,而不是具有尖锐角度的形状。在实验1中,70名参与者研究了三对,并在配对识别任务中测试了他们对每对成员之间联系的记忆。结果表明,声音-符号联想支持新联想的学习。实验2证实,这种效应在参与者之间的设计中得到了复制。在实验3中,我们将演示和测试之间的间隔延长至30秒,并在实验4中将延迟延长至2分钟,以确定该模式也存在于更典型的情景记忆范式中。研究结果从联想记忆的重要性的角度进行了讨论,同时参考了Bill Hockley在他自己的工作中所倡导的一些观点。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"A curved honulo improves your short-term and long-term memory.","authors":"J. Saint-Aubin, D. Guitard, M. Poirier","doi":"10.1037/cep0000279","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000279","url":null,"abstract":"During his distinguished career, Bill Hockley contributed to memory research in many ways, with work characterized by rigorous and innovative experimental designs. One of the areas he has explored is that of memory for associative information. We echo this interest here and attempt to emulate his careful experimental attitude. We report four experiments which examined how previously established links can support the development of new episodic associations. More specifically, we tested the idea that sound-symbolism links can support learning of new associations. Sound-symbolism links are relationships between phonemes and object characteristics that participants find natural-even if they have never encountered the items before. For instance, the nonword \"honulo\" is more readily seen to refer to a shape with curved contours than to a shape that has sharp angles. In Experiment 1, 70 participants studied three pairs and their memory for the associations between the members of each pair was tested in a paired-recognition task. Results demonstrate that sound-symbolism associations support the learning of new associations. Experiment 2 confirmed that the effect is replicated in a between-participants design. In Experiment 3, we replicated the findings with a 30-s filled interval between presentation and test, and in Experiment 4, we extended the delay to 2 min, establishing that the pattern is also found with a paradigm more typical of episodic memory. The results are discussed in terms of the importance of associative memory, while referring to some of the ideas Bill Hockley championed in his own work. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"38 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-04-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86468497","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Valence does not affect recognition. 效价不影响识别。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-14 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000275
M. Macmillan, Haylee R Field, I. Neath, A. Surprenant
Valence refers to the extent to which a stimulus is viewed as negative or positive. One recent model of valence, the NEVER model (Bowen et al., 2018), predicts that in general negative words will be better remembered than positive or neutral words. However, this prediction is difficult to validate for recognition tests because the literature reports inconsistent findings. Three experiments reexamined whether valence affects recognition of words by taking advantage of the recent increase in the number of high-quality norms and databases, which allow for the construct ion of three sets of stimuli that differ in valence, but are equated on numerous other dimensions known to affect memory. Experiment 1 found no difference in recognition performance between positive and negative words; Experiment 2 found no difference between positive and neutral words; and Experiment 3 found no difference between neutral and negative words. The results disconfirm a prediction of the NEVER model and suggest that previous demonstrations of an effect of valence are due to confounding other dimensions with valence. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
效价指的是刺激被视为消极或积极的程度。最近的一个效价模型,NEVER模型(Bowen et al., 2018)预测,一般来说,消极词汇比积极词汇或中性词汇更容易被记住。然而,由于文献报告的结果不一致,这种预测很难在识别测试中得到验证。三个实验利用最近高质量规范和数据库数量的增加,重新检查了价是否影响单词的识别,这些标准和数据库允许构建三组在价上不同的刺激,但在许多已知的影响记忆的其他维度上是相等的。实验1发现,积极词与消极词的识别效果无显著差异;实验2发现积极词和中性词之间没有差异;实验3发现中性词和否定词之间没有差异。结果否定了NEVER模型的预测,并表明先前的价效应的证明是由于将其他维度与价混淆。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Valence does not affect recognition.","authors":"M. Macmillan, Haylee R Field, I. Neath, A. Surprenant","doi":"10.1037/cep0000275","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000275","url":null,"abstract":"Valence refers to the extent to which a stimulus is viewed as negative or positive. One recent model of valence, the NEVER model (Bowen et al., 2018), predicts that in general negative words will be better remembered than positive or neutral words. However, this prediction is difficult to validate for recognition tests because the literature reports inconsistent findings. Three experiments reexamined whether valence affects recognition of words by taking advantage of the recent increase in the number of high-quality norms and databases, which allow for the construct ion of three sets of stimuli that differ in valence, but are equated on numerous other dimensions known to affect memory. Experiment 1 found no difference in recognition performance between positive and negative words; Experiment 2 found no difference between positive and neutral words; and Experiment 3 found no difference between neutral and negative words. The results disconfirm a prediction of the NEVER model and suggest that previous demonstrations of an effect of valence are due to confounding other dimensions with valence. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"136 22","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72370290","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Beauty and truth, truth and beauty: Chiastic structure increases the subjective accuracy of statements. 美与真,真与美:交错结构增加了陈述的主观准确性。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-10 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000277
M. Kara-Yakoubian, A. C. Walker, Konstantyn Sharpinskyi, Garni Assadourian, Jonathan A. Fugelsang, R. Harris
The Keats heuristic suggests that people find esthetically pleasing expressions more accurate than mundane expressions. We test this notion with chiastic statements. Chiasmus is a stylistic phenomenon in which at least two linguistic constituents are repeated in reverse order, conventionally represented by the formula A-B-B-A. Our study focuses on the specific form of chiasmus known as antimetabole, in which the reverse-repeated constituents are words (e.g., All for one and one for all; A = all, B = one). In three out of four experiments (N = 797), we find evidence that people judge antimetabolic statements (e.g., Success is getting what you want. Happiness is wanting what you get.) as more accurate than semantically equivalent nonantimetabolic statements (e.g., Success is getting what you wish. Happiness is wanting what you receive.). Furthermore, we evaluate fluency as a potential mechanism explaining the observed accuracy benefit afforded to antimetabolic statements, finding that the increased speed (i.e., fluency) with which antimetabolic statements were processed predicted judgments of accuracy. Overall, the present work is consistent with the growing literature on stylistic factors biasing assessments of truth, using the distinctive stylistic pattern of antimetabole. We find that information communicated using an antimetabolic structure is judged to be more accurate than nonantimetabolic paraphrases. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
济慈的启发式表明,人们发现美学上令人愉悦的表达比世俗的表达更准确。我们用交错语句来检验这个概念。交错是一种文体现象,其中至少两个语言成分以相反的顺序重复,通常用公式a - b - b - a来表示。我们的研究重点是被称为抗代谢的特定形式的交错,其中反向重复的成分是单词(例如,所有为我,我为所有;A =全部,B =一个)。在四分之三的实验(N = 797)中,我们发现了人们判断抗代谢陈述的证据(例如,成功是得到你想要的。幸福是想要你所得到的)比语义上等价的非抗代谢语句(例如,成功是得到你想要的)更准确。幸福就是想得到你所得到的。此外,我们评估流畅性作为一种潜在的机制来解释观察到的抗代谢语句提供的准确性优势,发现处理抗代谢语句的速度(即流畅性)的增加预测了准确性的判断。总的来说,目前的工作与越来越多的关于风格因素的文献是一致的,这些文献使用了独特的抗代谢的风格模式。我们发现使用抗代谢结构传达的信息被认为比非抗代谢释义更准确。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Beauty and truth, truth and beauty: Chiastic structure increases the subjective accuracy of statements.","authors":"M. Kara-Yakoubian, A. C. Walker, Konstantyn Sharpinskyi, Garni Assadourian, Jonathan A. Fugelsang, R. Harris","doi":"10.1037/cep0000277","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000277","url":null,"abstract":"The Keats heuristic suggests that people find esthetically pleasing expressions more accurate than mundane expressions. We test this notion with chiastic statements. Chiasmus is a stylistic phenomenon in which at least two linguistic constituents are repeated in reverse order, conventionally represented by the formula A-B-B-A. Our study focuses on the specific form of chiasmus known as antimetabole, in which the reverse-repeated constituents are words (e.g., All for one and one for all; A = all, B = one). In three out of four experiments (N = 797), we find evidence that people judge antimetabolic statements (e.g., Success is getting what you want. Happiness is wanting what you get.) as more accurate than semantically equivalent nonantimetabolic statements (e.g., Success is getting what you wish. Happiness is wanting what you receive.). Furthermore, we evaluate fluency as a potential mechanism explaining the observed accuracy benefit afforded to antimetabolic statements, finding that the increased speed (i.e., fluency) with which antimetabolic statements were processed predicted judgments of accuracy. Overall, the present work is consistent with the growing literature on stylistic factors biasing assessments of truth, using the distinctive stylistic pattern of antimetabole. We find that information communicated using an antimetabolic structure is judged to be more accurate than nonantimetabolic paraphrases. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"38 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"78358601","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Individual differences in the allocation of visual attention during navigation. 导航过程中视觉注意分配的个体差异。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-01 Epub Date: 2021-03-25 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000247
Mikayla Keller, Jennifer E Sutton

Individual differences exist in the ability to create an accurate mental survey representation (i.e., a cognitive map) of a novel environment, yet the mechanisms underlying differences in cognitive map accuracy are still under investigation. To determine whether differences in overt attention allocation contribute to these individual differences, the current study examined whether looking times to landmarks and other objects while navigating in a dynamic virtual environment were related to cognitive map accuracy. Participants completed a battery of spatial tests; some tests assessed spatial skills prior to the navigation task (the Santa Barbara Sense of Direction Scale and the Spatial Orientation Test), and others tested memory of the virtual environment Silcton after an exploration period (a landmark recognition task, a direction estimation task, a map-building task, and a route construction task). Individuals with inaccurate cognitive maps of Silcton, as measured by the direction estimation and map-building tasks, showed equivalent eye fixations to buildings and objects when exploring Silcton as those with accurate maps. Despite similar looking times, the inaccurate mappers were significantly worse at judgments of relative direction between landmarks in Silcton and showed poorer memory for landmarks in Silcton than accurate mappers. These findings suggest that cognitive mechanisms, such as mental perspective-taking, occurring after attention allocation underlie differences in cognitive map accuracy. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

在创造新环境的准确心理调查表征(即认知地图)的能力方面存在个体差异,但认知地图准确性差异的潜在机制仍在研究中。为了确定显性注意力分配的差异是否会导致这些个体差异,目前的研究调查了在动态虚拟环境中导航时,寻找地标和其他物体的时间是否与认知地图的准确性有关。参与者完成了一系列空间测试;一些测试评估了在导航任务之前的空间技能(圣巴巴拉方向感量表和空间方向测试),其他测试测试了在探索一段时间后对虚拟环境silton的记忆(地标识别任务、方向估计任务、地图构建任务和路线构建任务)。根据方向估计和地图绘制任务的测量,拥有不准确的Silcton认知地图的人在探索Silcton时,与拥有准确地图的人一样,眼睛盯着建筑物和物体。尽管看地图的时间相似,但不准确的地图绘制者在判断西雅图地标之间的相对方向方面明显比准确的地图绘制者差,而且对西雅图地标的记忆也比准确的地图绘制者差。这些发现表明,注意力分配后发生的认知机制,如心理换位思考,是认知地图准确性差异的基础。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Individual differences in the allocation of visual attention during navigation.","authors":"Mikayla Keller,&nbsp;Jennifer E Sutton","doi":"10.1037/cep0000247","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000247","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Individual differences exist in the ability to create an accurate mental survey representation (i.e., a cognitive map) of a novel environment, yet the mechanisms underlying differences in cognitive map accuracy are still under investigation. To determine whether differences in overt attention allocation contribute to these individual differences, the current study examined whether looking times to landmarks and other objects while navigating in a dynamic virtual environment were related to cognitive map accuracy. Participants completed a battery of spatial tests; some tests assessed spatial skills prior to the navigation task (the Santa Barbara Sense of Direction Scale and the Spatial Orientation Test), and others tested memory of the virtual environment <i>Silcton</i> after an exploration period (a landmark recognition task, a direction estimation task, a map-building task, and a route construction task). Individuals with inaccurate cognitive maps of Silcton, as measured by the direction estimation and map-building tasks, showed equivalent eye fixations to buildings and objects when exploring Silcton as those with accurate maps. Despite similar looking times, the inaccurate mappers were significantly worse at judgments of relative direction between landmarks in Silcton and showed poorer memory for landmarks in Silcton than accurate mappers. These findings suggest that cognitive mechanisms, such as mental perspective-taking, occurring after attention allocation underlie differences in cognitive map accuracy. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"76 1","pages":"10-21"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25514599","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Memory without retrieval: Testing the direct-access account of the missing item task. 没有检索的记忆:测试缺失项任务的直接访问帐户。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-01 Epub Date: 2021-07-22 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000263
Ian Neath

In the missing item task, two short lists are presented. The test list contains all but one of the items from the study list in a new random order and the task is to report which item from the study list is missing. Murdock and Smith (2005) found that the time to correctly respond with the missing item was independent of the position of the missing item and was also independent of the list length. They argued that these data are difficult to accommodate by models that include a search process but are consistent with models that posit "direct access" such as the power set version of Theory of Distributed Associative Memory (TODAM). If direct access is occurring, redintegration cannot be occurring. Two experiments test the direct access account by determining whether two effects commonly ascribed to redintegration occur in the missing item task. Experiment 1 found a semantic relatedness effect and Experiment 2 found a word frequency effect. The presence of these effects is consistent with a redintegration account. Implications for TODAM and for an explanation based on the Feature Model are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

在缺失项目任务中,出现了两个短列表。测试列表以新的随机顺序包含研究列表中的所有项目,只有一个项目除外,任务是报告研究列表中的哪个项目丢失了。Murdock和Smith(2005)发现,正确回应缺失项目的时间与缺失项目的位置无关,也与列表长度无关。他们认为,包含搜索过程的模型很难容纳这些数据,但与假设“直接访问”的模型(如分布式联想记忆理论(TODAM)的幂集版本)是一致的。如果发生直接访问,则不能发生重新集成。两个实验通过确定两种通常归因于重新整合的效应是否发生在缺失项目任务中来检验直接访问说。实验1发现语义关联效应,实验2发现词频效应。这些影响的存在与重新整合的说法相一致。讨论了TODAM的含义以及基于特征模型的解释。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Memory without retrieval: Testing the direct-access account of the missing item task.","authors":"Ian Neath","doi":"10.1037/cep0000263","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000263","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the missing item task, two short lists are presented. The test list contains all but one of the items from the study list in a new random order and the task is to report which item from the study list is missing. Murdock and Smith (2005) found that the time to correctly respond with the missing item was independent of the position of the missing item and was also independent of the list length. They argued that these data are difficult to accommodate by models that include a search process but are consistent with models that posit \"direct access\" such as the power set version of Theory of Distributed Associative Memory (TODAM). If direct access is occurring, redintegration cannot be occurring. Two experiments test the direct access account by determining whether two effects commonly ascribed to redintegration occur in the missing item task. Experiment 1 found a semantic relatedness effect and Experiment 2 found a word frequency effect. The presence of these effects is consistent with a redintegration account. Implications for TODAM and for an explanation based on the Feature Model are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"76 1","pages":"1-9"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39209289","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1