首页 > 最新文献

Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale最新文献

英文 中文
Differential effects of agency, animacy, and syntactic prominence on production and comprehension: Evidence from a verb-initial language. 能动性、能动性和句法突出对产生和理解的不同影响:来自动词开头语言的证据。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-04-28 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000280
I. Bondoc, A. Schafer
Effects of animacy and agent prominence in linguistic and cognitive processing are well-established in the literature. However, it is less clear how strongly an agent argument will influence production and comprehension when a sentence also contains another prominent argument. We examine this question with Tagalog, a verb-initial language, which designates a syntactically prominent, subject-like element (the pivot) without demoting the grammatical status of the core agent. We implemented two experiments that investigated the influences of agent and pivot prominence on syntactic linear word order patterns in production and on anticipatory gaze patterns in comprehension. Tagalog's grammar allowed us to separate the influence of agentivity from animacy by manipulating the animacy of the pivot (animate pivots: agent and benefactive voices; inanimate pivots: patient and instrument voices). The production results contrasted with the comprehension results: agent and pivot prominence both emerged strongly in a fragment-completion production task, but animacy dominated anticipatory gaze patterns in a visual-world comprehension task. The results of these experiments demonstrate variability in production and comprehension outcomes as well as an apparent mismatch between the constraints that shape these two systems, which we attribute to contrasting goals in production versus comprehension and to the organization of information in verb-initial languages. The investigation highlights the value of research on languages with typologically understudied structural properties in revealing mechanisms of the production and comprehension systems. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
在语言和认知加工中,动画性和主体突出的影响在文献中已经得到了证实。然而,当一个句子还包含另一个突出的论点时,我们就不太清楚一个主体论点对产出和理解的影响有多大了。我们用他加禄语来研究这个问题,他加禄语是一种动词开头的语言,它指定了一个语法上突出的、类似主语的元素(支点),而不降低核心主体的语法地位。我们实施了两个实验,研究了主体和支点突出对生产过程中句法线性语序模式和理解过程中预期注视模式的影响。他加禄语的语法允许我们通过操纵枢轴的枢轴来分离能动性和animanim性的影响(有生命的枢轴:代理和有益的声音;无生命的支点:耐心和乐器的声音)。生成结果与理解结果形成对比:主体和支点突出在片段完成生成任务中都强烈出现,而动画在视觉世界理解任务中主导预期注视模式。这些实验的结果证明了产生和理解结果的可变性,以及形成这两个系统的约束之间的明显不匹配,我们将其归因于产生与理解的不同目标以及动词开头语言中的信息组织。本研究强调了对类型学上未被充分研究的语言结构特性的研究在揭示其产生和理解系统的机制方面的价值。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Differential effects of agency, animacy, and syntactic prominence on production and comprehension: Evidence from a verb-initial language.","authors":"I. Bondoc, A. Schafer","doi":"10.1037/cep0000280","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000280","url":null,"abstract":"Effects of animacy and agent prominence in linguistic and cognitive processing are well-established in the literature. However, it is less clear how strongly an agent argument will influence production and comprehension when a sentence also contains another prominent argument. We examine this question with Tagalog, a verb-initial language, which designates a syntactically prominent, subject-like element (the pivot) without demoting the grammatical status of the core agent. We implemented two experiments that investigated the influences of agent and pivot prominence on syntactic linear word order patterns in production and on anticipatory gaze patterns in comprehension. Tagalog's grammar allowed us to separate the influence of agentivity from animacy by manipulating the animacy of the pivot (animate pivots: agent and benefactive voices; inanimate pivots: patient and instrument voices). The production results contrasted with the comprehension results: agent and pivot prominence both emerged strongly in a fragment-completion production task, but animacy dominated anticipatory gaze patterns in a visual-world comprehension task. The results of these experiments demonstrate variability in production and comprehension outcomes as well as an apparent mismatch between the constraints that shape these two systems, which we attribute to contrasting goals in production versus comprehension and to the organization of information in verb-initial languages. The investigation highlights the value of research on languages with typologically understudied structural properties in revealing mechanisms of the production and comprehension systems. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"48 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-04-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"76397651","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Another look at the contribution of motoric fluency to metacognitive monitoring. 再看看运动流畅性对元认知监测的贡献。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-04-28 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000278
Michelle A Dollois, Cole J Poore-Buchhaupt, Chris M. Fiacconi
Recent evidence indicates that fluent motor interactions with studied information can increase confidence in how well that information will be later remembered, as measured by judgements of learning (JOLs). However, it remains unclear whether such metacognitive assessments are based on experienced motoric fluency or on explicit, analytic beliefs regarding the mnemonic impact of the experimental manipulations used to enhance fluency. Here, we introduce a new approach to examine the extent to which experience-based processes alone underlie this effect by manipulating motoric fluency outside of participants' awareness. Across two experiments, we examined typing speed for both real-word and nonword verbal stimuli following a training phase in which participants typed items consisting of a restricted subset of letters. Despite faster typing times (i.e., greater motoric fluency) for new items comprised of the same subset of letters used during training, we found no evidence that these items were perceived as more memorable. For real words, linear mixed-effect model analyses at the item level relating typing speed and JOLs also revealed no evidence that motoric fluency increased perceived memorability. Similar analyses conducted for nonwords did, however, reveal a modest positive relation between these constructs. Together, these findings suggest that the contribution of motoric fluency to metacognitive monitoring in the absence of analytic beliefs is minimal. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
最近的证据表明,通过学习判断(JOLs)来衡量,流畅的运动与学习信息的互动可以增加对以后记忆信息的信心。然而,目前尚不清楚这种元认知评估是基于经验运动流畅性还是基于关于用于提高流畅性的实验操作的助记影响的明确分析信念。在这里,我们引入了一种新的方法来检验基于经验的过程在多大程度上通过操纵参与者意识之外的运动流畅性来实现这种效果。在两个实验中,我们测试了真实单词和非单词语言刺激的打字速度,在这个训练阶段,参与者输入由有限的字母子集组成的项目。尽管在训练中使用的由相同字母子集组成的新项目的打字时间更快(即更大的运动流畅性),但我们没有发现证据表明这些项目被认为更容易记住。对于真实单词,项目水平的线性混合效应模型分析也显示,运动流畅性没有增加感知记忆的证据。然而,对非词进行的类似分析确实揭示了这些构念之间存在适度的正相关。总之,这些发现表明,在缺乏分析信念的情况下,运动流畅性对元认知监测的贡献是微乎其微的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Another look at the contribution of motoric fluency to metacognitive monitoring.","authors":"Michelle A Dollois, Cole J Poore-Buchhaupt, Chris M. Fiacconi","doi":"10.1037/cep0000278","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000278","url":null,"abstract":"Recent evidence indicates that fluent motor interactions with studied information can increase confidence in how well that information will be later remembered, as measured by judgements of learning (JOLs). However, it remains unclear whether such metacognitive assessments are based on experienced motoric fluency or on explicit, analytic beliefs regarding the mnemonic impact of the experimental manipulations used to enhance fluency. Here, we introduce a new approach to examine the extent to which experience-based processes alone underlie this effect by manipulating motoric fluency outside of participants' awareness. Across two experiments, we examined typing speed for both real-word and nonword verbal stimuli following a training phase in which participants typed items consisting of a restricted subset of letters. Despite faster typing times (i.e., greater motoric fluency) for new items comprised of the same subset of letters used during training, we found no evidence that these items were perceived as more memorable. For real words, linear mixed-effect model analyses at the item level relating typing speed and JOLs also revealed no evidence that motoric fluency increased perceived memorability. Similar analyses conducted for nonwords did, however, reveal a modest positive relation between these constructs. Together, these findings suggest that the contribution of motoric fluency to metacognitive monitoring in the absence of analytic beliefs is minimal. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"24 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-04-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"81996136","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A curved honulo improves your short-term and long-term memory. 弯曲的火虹可以改善你的短期和长期记忆。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-04-28 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000279
J. Saint-Aubin, D. Guitard, M. Poirier
During his distinguished career, Bill Hockley contributed to memory research in many ways, with work characterized by rigorous and innovative experimental designs. One of the areas he has explored is that of memory for associative information. We echo this interest here and attempt to emulate his careful experimental attitude. We report four experiments which examined how previously established links can support the development of new episodic associations. More specifically, we tested the idea that sound-symbolism links can support learning of new associations. Sound-symbolism links are relationships between phonemes and object characteristics that participants find natural-even if they have never encountered the items before. For instance, the nonword "honulo" is more readily seen to refer to a shape with curved contours than to a shape that has sharp angles. In Experiment 1, 70 participants studied three pairs and their memory for the associations between the members of each pair was tested in a paired-recognition task. Results demonstrate that sound-symbolism associations support the learning of new associations. Experiment 2 confirmed that the effect is replicated in a between-participants design. In Experiment 3, we replicated the findings with a 30-s filled interval between presentation and test, and in Experiment 4, we extended the delay to 2 min, establishing that the pattern is also found with a paradigm more typical of episodic memory. The results are discussed in terms of the importance of associative memory, while referring to some of the ideas Bill Hockley championed in his own work. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
在他杰出的职业生涯中,比尔·霍克利在许多方面对记忆研究做出了贡献,他的工作以严谨和创新的实验设计为特点。他探索的一个领域是联想信息的记忆。我们在这里呼应这种兴趣,并试图模仿他谨慎的实验态度。我们报告了四个实验,这些实验检查了先前建立的联系如何支持新情景关联的发展。更具体地说,我们测试了声音象征联系可以支持新联想学习的观点。声音符号联系是指音素和物体特征之间的关系,即使参与者以前从未见过这些物品,他们也会觉得很自然。例如,nonword“honulo”更容易被看作是指具有弯曲轮廓的形状,而不是具有尖锐角度的形状。在实验1中,70名参与者研究了三对,并在配对识别任务中测试了他们对每对成员之间联系的记忆。结果表明,声音-符号联想支持新联想的学习。实验2证实,这种效应在参与者之间的设计中得到了复制。在实验3中,我们将演示和测试之间的间隔延长至30秒,并在实验4中将延迟延长至2分钟,以确定该模式也存在于更典型的情景记忆范式中。研究结果从联想记忆的重要性的角度进行了讨论,同时参考了Bill Hockley在他自己的工作中所倡导的一些观点。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"A curved honulo improves your short-term and long-term memory.","authors":"J. Saint-Aubin, D. Guitard, M. Poirier","doi":"10.1037/cep0000279","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000279","url":null,"abstract":"During his distinguished career, Bill Hockley contributed to memory research in many ways, with work characterized by rigorous and innovative experimental designs. One of the areas he has explored is that of memory for associative information. We echo this interest here and attempt to emulate his careful experimental attitude. We report four experiments which examined how previously established links can support the development of new episodic associations. More specifically, we tested the idea that sound-symbolism links can support learning of new associations. Sound-symbolism links are relationships between phonemes and object characteristics that participants find natural-even if they have never encountered the items before. For instance, the nonword \"honulo\" is more readily seen to refer to a shape with curved contours than to a shape that has sharp angles. In Experiment 1, 70 participants studied three pairs and their memory for the associations between the members of each pair was tested in a paired-recognition task. Results demonstrate that sound-symbolism associations support the learning of new associations. Experiment 2 confirmed that the effect is replicated in a between-participants design. In Experiment 3, we replicated the findings with a 30-s filled interval between presentation and test, and in Experiment 4, we extended the delay to 2 min, establishing that the pattern is also found with a paradigm more typical of episodic memory. The results are discussed in terms of the importance of associative memory, while referring to some of the ideas Bill Hockley championed in his own work. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"38 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-04-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86468497","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Valence does not affect recognition. 效价不影响识别。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-14 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000275
M. Macmillan, Haylee R Field, I. Neath, A. Surprenant
Valence refers to the extent to which a stimulus is viewed as negative or positive. One recent model of valence, the NEVER model (Bowen et al., 2018), predicts that in general negative words will be better remembered than positive or neutral words. However, this prediction is difficult to validate for recognition tests because the literature reports inconsistent findings. Three experiments reexamined whether valence affects recognition of words by taking advantage of the recent increase in the number of high-quality norms and databases, which allow for the construct ion of three sets of stimuli that differ in valence, but are equated on numerous other dimensions known to affect memory. Experiment 1 found no difference in recognition performance between positive and negative words; Experiment 2 found no difference between positive and neutral words; and Experiment 3 found no difference between neutral and negative words. The results disconfirm a prediction of the NEVER model and suggest that previous demonstrations of an effect of valence are due to confounding other dimensions with valence. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
效价指的是刺激被视为消极或积极的程度。最近的一个效价模型,NEVER模型(Bowen et al., 2018)预测,一般来说,消极词汇比积极词汇或中性词汇更容易被记住。然而,由于文献报告的结果不一致,这种预测很难在识别测试中得到验证。三个实验利用最近高质量规范和数据库数量的增加,重新检查了价是否影响单词的识别,这些标准和数据库允许构建三组在价上不同的刺激,但在许多已知的影响记忆的其他维度上是相等的。实验1发现,积极词与消极词的识别效果无显著差异;实验2发现积极词和中性词之间没有差异;实验3发现中性词和否定词之间没有差异。结果否定了NEVER模型的预测,并表明先前的价效应的证明是由于将其他维度与价混淆。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Valence does not affect recognition.","authors":"M. Macmillan, Haylee R Field, I. Neath, A. Surprenant","doi":"10.1037/cep0000275","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000275","url":null,"abstract":"Valence refers to the extent to which a stimulus is viewed as negative or positive. One recent model of valence, the NEVER model (Bowen et al., 2018), predicts that in general negative words will be better remembered than positive or neutral words. However, this prediction is difficult to validate for recognition tests because the literature reports inconsistent findings. Three experiments reexamined whether valence affects recognition of words by taking advantage of the recent increase in the number of high-quality norms and databases, which allow for the construct ion of three sets of stimuli that differ in valence, but are equated on numerous other dimensions known to affect memory. Experiment 1 found no difference in recognition performance between positive and negative words; Experiment 2 found no difference between positive and neutral words; and Experiment 3 found no difference between neutral and negative words. The results disconfirm a prediction of the NEVER model and suggest that previous demonstrations of an effect of valence are due to confounding other dimensions with valence. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"136 22","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72370290","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Beauty and truth, truth and beauty: Chiastic structure increases the subjective accuracy of statements. 美与真,真与美:交错结构增加了陈述的主观准确性。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-10 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000277
M. Kara-Yakoubian, A. C. Walker, Konstantyn Sharpinskyi, Garni Assadourian, Jonathan A. Fugelsang, R. Harris
The Keats heuristic suggests that people find esthetically pleasing expressions more accurate than mundane expressions. We test this notion with chiastic statements. Chiasmus is a stylistic phenomenon in which at least two linguistic constituents are repeated in reverse order, conventionally represented by the formula A-B-B-A. Our study focuses on the specific form of chiasmus known as antimetabole, in which the reverse-repeated constituents are words (e.g., All for one and one for all; A = all, B = one). In three out of four experiments (N = 797), we find evidence that people judge antimetabolic statements (e.g., Success is getting what you want. Happiness is wanting what you get.) as more accurate than semantically equivalent nonantimetabolic statements (e.g., Success is getting what you wish. Happiness is wanting what you receive.). Furthermore, we evaluate fluency as a potential mechanism explaining the observed accuracy benefit afforded to antimetabolic statements, finding that the increased speed (i.e., fluency) with which antimetabolic statements were processed predicted judgments of accuracy. Overall, the present work is consistent with the growing literature on stylistic factors biasing assessments of truth, using the distinctive stylistic pattern of antimetabole. We find that information communicated using an antimetabolic structure is judged to be more accurate than nonantimetabolic paraphrases. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
济慈的启发式表明,人们发现美学上令人愉悦的表达比世俗的表达更准确。我们用交错语句来检验这个概念。交错是一种文体现象,其中至少两个语言成分以相反的顺序重复,通常用公式a - b - b - a来表示。我们的研究重点是被称为抗代谢的特定形式的交错,其中反向重复的成分是单词(例如,所有为我,我为所有;A =全部,B =一个)。在四分之三的实验(N = 797)中,我们发现了人们判断抗代谢陈述的证据(例如,成功是得到你想要的。幸福是想要你所得到的)比语义上等价的非抗代谢语句(例如,成功是得到你想要的)更准确。幸福就是想得到你所得到的。此外,我们评估流畅性作为一种潜在的机制来解释观察到的抗代谢语句提供的准确性优势,发现处理抗代谢语句的速度(即流畅性)的增加预测了准确性的判断。总的来说,目前的工作与越来越多的关于风格因素的文献是一致的,这些文献使用了独特的抗代谢的风格模式。我们发现使用抗代谢结构传达的信息被认为比非抗代谢释义更准确。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Beauty and truth, truth and beauty: Chiastic structure increases the subjective accuracy of statements.","authors":"M. Kara-Yakoubian, A. C. Walker, Konstantyn Sharpinskyi, Garni Assadourian, Jonathan A. Fugelsang, R. Harris","doi":"10.1037/cep0000277","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000277","url":null,"abstract":"The Keats heuristic suggests that people find esthetically pleasing expressions more accurate than mundane expressions. We test this notion with chiastic statements. Chiasmus is a stylistic phenomenon in which at least two linguistic constituents are repeated in reverse order, conventionally represented by the formula A-B-B-A. Our study focuses on the specific form of chiasmus known as antimetabole, in which the reverse-repeated constituents are words (e.g., All for one and one for all; A = all, B = one). In three out of four experiments (N = 797), we find evidence that people judge antimetabolic statements (e.g., Success is getting what you want. Happiness is wanting what you get.) as more accurate than semantically equivalent nonantimetabolic statements (e.g., Success is getting what you wish. Happiness is wanting what you receive.). Furthermore, we evaluate fluency as a potential mechanism explaining the observed accuracy benefit afforded to antimetabolic statements, finding that the increased speed (i.e., fluency) with which antimetabolic statements were processed predicted judgments of accuracy. Overall, the present work is consistent with the growing literature on stylistic factors biasing assessments of truth, using the distinctive stylistic pattern of antimetabole. We find that information communicated using an antimetabolic structure is judged to be more accurate than nonantimetabolic paraphrases. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"38 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"78358601","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Individual differences in the allocation of visual attention during navigation. 导航过程中视觉注意分配的个体差异。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-01 Epub Date: 2021-03-25 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000247
Mikayla Keller, Jennifer E Sutton

Individual differences exist in the ability to create an accurate mental survey representation (i.e., a cognitive map) of a novel environment, yet the mechanisms underlying differences in cognitive map accuracy are still under investigation. To determine whether differences in overt attention allocation contribute to these individual differences, the current study examined whether looking times to landmarks and other objects while navigating in a dynamic virtual environment were related to cognitive map accuracy. Participants completed a battery of spatial tests; some tests assessed spatial skills prior to the navigation task (the Santa Barbara Sense of Direction Scale and the Spatial Orientation Test), and others tested memory of the virtual environment Silcton after an exploration period (a landmark recognition task, a direction estimation task, a map-building task, and a route construction task). Individuals with inaccurate cognitive maps of Silcton, as measured by the direction estimation and map-building tasks, showed equivalent eye fixations to buildings and objects when exploring Silcton as those with accurate maps. Despite similar looking times, the inaccurate mappers were significantly worse at judgments of relative direction between landmarks in Silcton and showed poorer memory for landmarks in Silcton than accurate mappers. These findings suggest that cognitive mechanisms, such as mental perspective-taking, occurring after attention allocation underlie differences in cognitive map accuracy. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

在创造新环境的准确心理调查表征(即认知地图)的能力方面存在个体差异,但认知地图准确性差异的潜在机制仍在研究中。为了确定显性注意力分配的差异是否会导致这些个体差异,目前的研究调查了在动态虚拟环境中导航时,寻找地标和其他物体的时间是否与认知地图的准确性有关。参与者完成了一系列空间测试;一些测试评估了在导航任务之前的空间技能(圣巴巴拉方向感量表和空间方向测试),其他测试测试了在探索一段时间后对虚拟环境silton的记忆(地标识别任务、方向估计任务、地图构建任务和路线构建任务)。根据方向估计和地图绘制任务的测量,拥有不准确的Silcton认知地图的人在探索Silcton时,与拥有准确地图的人一样,眼睛盯着建筑物和物体。尽管看地图的时间相似,但不准确的地图绘制者在判断西雅图地标之间的相对方向方面明显比准确的地图绘制者差,而且对西雅图地标的记忆也比准确的地图绘制者差。这些发现表明,注意力分配后发生的认知机制,如心理换位思考,是认知地图准确性差异的基础。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Individual differences in the allocation of visual attention during navigation.","authors":"Mikayla Keller,&nbsp;Jennifer E Sutton","doi":"10.1037/cep0000247","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000247","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Individual differences exist in the ability to create an accurate mental survey representation (i.e., a cognitive map) of a novel environment, yet the mechanisms underlying differences in cognitive map accuracy are still under investigation. To determine whether differences in overt attention allocation contribute to these individual differences, the current study examined whether looking times to landmarks and other objects while navigating in a dynamic virtual environment were related to cognitive map accuracy. Participants completed a battery of spatial tests; some tests assessed spatial skills prior to the navigation task (the Santa Barbara Sense of Direction Scale and the Spatial Orientation Test), and others tested memory of the virtual environment <i>Silcton</i> after an exploration period (a landmark recognition task, a direction estimation task, a map-building task, and a route construction task). Individuals with inaccurate cognitive maps of Silcton, as measured by the direction estimation and map-building tasks, showed equivalent eye fixations to buildings and objects when exploring Silcton as those with accurate maps. Despite similar looking times, the inaccurate mappers were significantly worse at judgments of relative direction between landmarks in Silcton and showed poorer memory for landmarks in Silcton than accurate mappers. These findings suggest that cognitive mechanisms, such as mental perspective-taking, occurring after attention allocation underlie differences in cognitive map accuracy. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"76 1","pages":"10-21"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25514599","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Memory without retrieval: Testing the direct-access account of the missing item task. 没有检索的记忆:测试缺失项任务的直接访问帐户。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-01 Epub Date: 2021-07-22 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000263
Ian Neath

In the missing item task, two short lists are presented. The test list contains all but one of the items from the study list in a new random order and the task is to report which item from the study list is missing. Murdock and Smith (2005) found that the time to correctly respond with the missing item was independent of the position of the missing item and was also independent of the list length. They argued that these data are difficult to accommodate by models that include a search process but are consistent with models that posit "direct access" such as the power set version of Theory of Distributed Associative Memory (TODAM). If direct access is occurring, redintegration cannot be occurring. Two experiments test the direct access account by determining whether two effects commonly ascribed to redintegration occur in the missing item task. Experiment 1 found a semantic relatedness effect and Experiment 2 found a word frequency effect. The presence of these effects is consistent with a redintegration account. Implications for TODAM and for an explanation based on the Feature Model are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

在缺失项目任务中,出现了两个短列表。测试列表以新的随机顺序包含研究列表中的所有项目,只有一个项目除外,任务是报告研究列表中的哪个项目丢失了。Murdock和Smith(2005)发现,正确回应缺失项目的时间与缺失项目的位置无关,也与列表长度无关。他们认为,包含搜索过程的模型很难容纳这些数据,但与假设“直接访问”的模型(如分布式联想记忆理论(TODAM)的幂集版本)是一致的。如果发生直接访问,则不能发生重新集成。两个实验通过确定两种通常归因于重新整合的效应是否发生在缺失项目任务中来检验直接访问说。实验1发现语义关联效应,实验2发现词频效应。这些影响的存在与重新整合的说法相一致。讨论了TODAM的含义以及基于特征模型的解释。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Memory without retrieval: Testing the direct-access account of the missing item task.","authors":"Ian Neath","doi":"10.1037/cep0000263","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000263","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the missing item task, two short lists are presented. The test list contains all but one of the items from the study list in a new random order and the task is to report which item from the study list is missing. Murdock and Smith (2005) found that the time to correctly respond with the missing item was independent of the position of the missing item and was also independent of the list length. They argued that these data are difficult to accommodate by models that include a search process but are consistent with models that posit \"direct access\" such as the power set version of Theory of Distributed Associative Memory (TODAM). If direct access is occurring, redintegration cannot be occurring. Two experiments test the direct access account by determining whether two effects commonly ascribed to redintegration occur in the missing item task. Experiment 1 found a semantic relatedness effect and Experiment 2 found a word frequency effect. The presence of these effects is consistent with a redintegration account. Implications for TODAM and for an explanation based on the Feature Model are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"76 1","pages":"1-9"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39209289","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distinct visual resolution supports aperture shaping in natural and pantomime-grasping. 独特的视觉分辨率支持孔径塑造自然和哑剧抓取。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-01 Epub Date: 2021-10-25 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000264
Matthew Heath, Naila Ayala, Maryam Hamidi, Benjamin Tari

Pantomime-grasping is a "simulated" motor response wherein an individual grasps to an area dissociated from a physical target. The task has been used in the apraxia literature as a proxy for natural grasping (i.e., physically grasping a target); however, it is important to recognize that the task's decoupled spatial relations between stimulus and response renders the top-down processing of target features (e.g., size) that accumulating evidence has shown to be mediated by visual information functionally distinct from natural grasping. Here, we examined whether the visual information supporting pantomime-grasps exhibits a visual resolution power commensurate with natural grasps. Participants were presented with a target and nontarget that differed in size below the perceptual threshold (i.e., 0.5 mm or ∼1.3%) and were asked to make a perceptual judgment about the target (i.e., "smaller" or "larger" than the nontarget) before and after completing natural and pantomime-grasps. Results showed that perceptual judgments "before" and "after" natural and pantomime-grasps did not reliably distinguish between target and nontarget. Natural grasp peak grip apertures (PGAs) scaled to target size and were comparable for "before" and "after" perceptual judgment trials-a result indicating that haptic feedback from physically grasping the target did not "boost" perceptual accuracy. Most notably, pantomime-grasp PGAs were insensitive to target size; that is, responses elicited a visual resolution power less than natural grasps. These results provide convergent evidence that pantomime-grasps are mediated by the same visual information as obligatory perceptions and do not provide a proxy for natural grasps. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

哑剧抓取是一种“模拟”运动反应,其中个体抓取与物理目标分离的区域。该任务在失用症文献中被用作自然抓取(即物理抓取目标)的代理;然而,重要的是要认识到,任务在刺激和反应之间的解耦空间关系使得目标特征(例如,大小)的自上而下处理,越来越多的证据表明,视觉信息在功能上与自然抓取不同。在这里,我们研究了支持哑剧掌握的视觉信息是否表现出与自然掌握相称的视觉分辨能力。在完成自然抓取和哑谜抓取之前和之后,参与者被要求对目标(即比非目标“小”或“大”)做出感知判断(即比非目标“小”或“大”),目标的大小低于感知阈值(即0.5毫米或~ 1.3%)。结果表明,自然和哑剧抓取前后的知觉判断不能可靠地区分目标和非目标。自然抓握峰值孔径(PGAs)缩放到目标大小,并且在“之前”和“之后”的感知判断试验中具有可比性——这一结果表明,物理抓握目标的触觉反馈并没有“提高”感知准确性。最值得注意的是,哑剧抓取PGAs对目标尺寸不敏感;也就是说,反应引起的视觉分辨能力低于自然的把握。这些结果提供了趋同的证据,证明哑剧掌握是由与强制性知觉相同的视觉信息介导的,而不是自然掌握的代理。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Distinct visual resolution supports aperture shaping in natural and pantomime-grasping.","authors":"Matthew Heath,&nbsp;Naila Ayala,&nbsp;Maryam Hamidi,&nbsp;Benjamin Tari","doi":"10.1037/cep0000264","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000264","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Pantomime-grasping is a \"simulated\" motor response wherein an individual grasps to an area dissociated from a physical target. The task has been used in the apraxia literature as a proxy for natural grasping (i.e., physically grasping a target); however, it is important to recognize that the task's decoupled spatial relations between stimulus and response renders the top-down processing of target features (e.g., size) that accumulating evidence has shown to be mediated by visual information functionally distinct from natural grasping. Here, we examined whether the visual information supporting pantomime-grasps exhibits a visual resolution power commensurate with natural grasps. Participants were presented with a target and nontarget that differed in size below the perceptual threshold (i.e., 0.5 mm or ∼1.3%) and were asked to make a perceptual judgment about the target (i.e., \"smaller\" or \"larger\" than the nontarget) before and after completing natural and pantomime-grasps. Results showed that perceptual judgments \"before\" and \"after\" natural and pantomime-grasps did not reliably distinguish between target and nontarget. Natural grasp peak grip apertures (PGAs) scaled to target size and were comparable for \"before\" and \"after\" perceptual judgment trials-a result indicating that haptic feedback from physically grasping the target did not \"boost\" perceptual accuracy. Most notably, pantomime-grasp PGAs were insensitive to target size; that is, responses elicited a visual resolution power less than natural grasps. These results provide convergent evidence that pantomime-grasps are mediated by the same visual information as obligatory perceptions and do not provide a proxy for natural grasps. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"76 1","pages":"22-28"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39555476","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual word recognition: Attention, intention, context, and processing dynamics. 视觉词识别:注意、意图、语境和处理动态。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-01 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000274
D. Besner
The notion that some mental processes are "automatic" while others are "controlled" is a distinction that appears in virtually all cognition textbooks, as well as in thousands of papers and book chapters. Indeed, so entrenched is the automatic side of this distinction that various leading computational accounts make no mention of it, but instead assume it implicitly. These models, and the field more generally, assume that processing is stimulus triggered and does not need any form of attention or an intention as a preliminary. Further, the fundamental processing dynamics underlying such automatic processing is widely seen as consisting of interactive activation and autonomous in that it unfolds in the same way across contexts. I review a number of findings from my lab that lead me to a different conclusion. Visual word recognition requires a consideration and integrated understanding of automaticity, attention, intention, context, and cognitive processing. I present various findings that challenge the preeminent role ascribed to interactive activation as implemented in the dominant computational models. I conclude that, going forward, the time is due for computational models of visual word recognition (and researchers in the field more generally) to acknowledge that the findings reported here constitute benchmarks that constrain theory and present opportunities for making meaningful advances in our understanding of visual word recognition (and perhaps of cognition more generally). A few proposals for how we might think about some of these processes are offered. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
一些心理过程是“自动的”,而另一些是“受控的”,这一概念几乎出现在所有的认知教科书中,也出现在成千上万的论文和书籍章节中。事实上,这种区别的自动一面是如此根深蒂固,以至于各种领先的计算账户都没有提到它,而是隐含地假设了它。这些模型,以及更广泛的领域,假设处理是由刺激触发的,不需要任何形式的注意或意图作为先决条件。此外,这种自动处理的基本处理动力学被广泛认为是由交互激活和自主组成的,因为它在不同的环境中以相同的方式展开。我回顾了实验室的一些发现,这些发现让我得出了一个不同的结论。视觉词识别需要考虑和综合理解自动性、注意力、意图、语境和认知处理。我提出了各种各样的发现,这些发现挑战了在主流计算模型中实现的交互式激活的卓越作用。我的结论是,展望未来,是时候让视觉词识别的计算模型(以及更广泛的该领域的研究人员)承认,这里报道的发现构成了约束理论的基准,并为我们对视觉词识别的理解(也许是更广泛的认知)取得有意义的进展提供了机会。对于我们如何思考这些过程,提出了一些建议。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Visual word recognition: Attention, intention, context, and processing dynamics.","authors":"D. Besner","doi":"10.1037/cep0000274","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000274","url":null,"abstract":"The notion that some mental processes are \"automatic\" while others are \"controlled\" is a distinction that appears in virtually all cognition textbooks, as well as in thousands of papers and book chapters. Indeed, so entrenched is the automatic side of this distinction that various leading computational accounts make no mention of it, but instead assume it implicitly. These models, and the field more generally, assume that processing is stimulus triggered and does not need any form of attention or an intention as a preliminary. Further, the fundamental processing dynamics underlying such automatic processing is widely seen as consisting of interactive activation and autonomous in that it unfolds in the same way across contexts. I review a number of findings from my lab that lead me to a different conclusion. Visual word recognition requires a consideration and integrated understanding of automaticity, attention, intention, context, and cognitive processing. I present various findings that challenge the preeminent role ascribed to interactive activation as implemented in the dominant computational models. I conclude that, going forward, the time is due for computational models of visual word recognition (and researchers in the field more generally) to acknowledge that the findings reported here constitute benchmarks that constrain theory and present opportunities for making meaningful advances in our understanding of visual word recognition (and perhaps of cognition more generally). A few proposals for how we might think about some of these processes are offered. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"300 1","pages":"57-74"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89037699","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Separate processing mechanisms for spatial-numerical compatibility and numerical-size congruity. 空间-数值相容性和数值-尺寸一致性的分离处理机制。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-01 Epub Date: 2022-02-10 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000270
James E Vellan, Craig Leth-Steensen

Across two experiments, the numerical magnitude and the physical size of single digits presented in either two (Experiment 1) or four (Experiment 2) different font sizes were judged using either horizontally and vertically (Experiment 1) or just horizontally (Experiment 2) aligned manual responses. Such a design allowed for the simultaneous examination of the size congruity effect (SiCE), the spatial-numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect, and the more novel spatial-size association of response codes (SSARC) effect. In Experiment 1, SiCEs and SNARC effects were found that operated independently of one another but no SSARC effect occurred. In Experiment 2, separate SiCEs and SNARC effects were found when judging numerical magnitude whereas separate SiCEs and SSARC effects were found when judging physical size. As will be discussed, such findings provide important constraints on the manner in which the full set of congruency and compatibility effects between stimulus and response dimensions in such tasks may be modeled. To illustrate this point, four different versions of a general computational processing model of these effects are considered. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

在两个实验中,使用水平和垂直对齐(实验1)或水平对齐(实验2)的手动响应来判断两种(实验1)或四种(实验2)不同字体大小中呈现的单个数字的数值大小和物理大小。这样的设计允许同时检查大小一致性效应(SiCE)、反应码的空间-数值关联效应(SNARC)和更新颖的反应码的空间-大小关联效应(SSARC)。在实验1中,SiCEs效应和SNARC效应相互独立,不存在SSARC效应。在实验2中,在判断数值量级时存在独立的SiCEs和SNARC效应,在判断物理尺寸时存在独立的SiCEs和SSARC效应。正如我们将要讨论的那样,这些发现为这些任务中刺激和反应维度之间的全部一致性和兼容性效应的建模方式提供了重要的限制。为了说明这一点,考虑了这些效应的一般计算处理模型的四个不同版本。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Separate processing mechanisms for spatial-numerical compatibility and numerical-size congruity.","authors":"James E Vellan,&nbsp;Craig Leth-Steensen","doi":"10.1037/cep0000270","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000270","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Across two experiments, the numerical magnitude and the physical size of single digits presented in either two (Experiment 1) or four (Experiment 2) different font sizes were judged using either horizontally and vertically (Experiment 1) or just horizontally (Experiment 2) aligned manual responses. Such a design allowed for the simultaneous examination of the size congruity effect (SiCE), the spatial-numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect, and the more novel spatial-size association of response codes (SSARC) effect. In Experiment 1, SiCEs and SNARC effects were found that operated independently of one another but no SSARC effect occurred. In Experiment 2, separate SiCEs and SNARC effects were found when judging numerical magnitude whereas separate SiCEs and SSARC effects were found when judging physical size. As will be discussed, such findings provide important constraints on the manner in which the full set of congruency and compatibility effects between stimulus and response dimensions in such tasks may be modeled. To illustrate this point, four different versions of a general computational processing model of these effects are considered. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"76 1","pages":"44-56"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39611409","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
期刊
Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1