Pub Date : 2022-03-01Epub Date: 2021-10-25DOI: 10.1037/cep0000264
Matthew Heath, Naila Ayala, Maryam Hamidi, Benjamin Tari
Pantomime-grasping is a "simulated" motor response wherein an individual grasps to an area dissociated from a physical target. The task has been used in the apraxia literature as a proxy for natural grasping (i.e., physically grasping a target); however, it is important to recognize that the task's decoupled spatial relations between stimulus and response renders the top-down processing of target features (e.g., size) that accumulating evidence has shown to be mediated by visual information functionally distinct from natural grasping. Here, we examined whether the visual information supporting pantomime-grasps exhibits a visual resolution power commensurate with natural grasps. Participants were presented with a target and nontarget that differed in size below the perceptual threshold (i.e., 0.5 mm or ∼1.3%) and were asked to make a perceptual judgment about the target (i.e., "smaller" or "larger" than the nontarget) before and after completing natural and pantomime-grasps. Results showed that perceptual judgments "before" and "after" natural and pantomime-grasps did not reliably distinguish between target and nontarget. Natural grasp peak grip apertures (PGAs) scaled to target size and were comparable for "before" and "after" perceptual judgment trials-a result indicating that haptic feedback from physically grasping the target did not "boost" perceptual accuracy. Most notably, pantomime-grasp PGAs were insensitive to target size; that is, responses elicited a visual resolution power less than natural grasps. These results provide convergent evidence that pantomime-grasps are mediated by the same visual information as obligatory perceptions and do not provide a proxy for natural grasps. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
哑剧抓取是一种“模拟”运动反应,其中个体抓取与物理目标分离的区域。该任务在失用症文献中被用作自然抓取(即物理抓取目标)的代理;然而,重要的是要认识到,任务在刺激和反应之间的解耦空间关系使得目标特征(例如,大小)的自上而下处理,越来越多的证据表明,视觉信息在功能上与自然抓取不同。在这里,我们研究了支持哑剧掌握的视觉信息是否表现出与自然掌握相称的视觉分辨能力。在完成自然抓取和哑谜抓取之前和之后,参与者被要求对目标(即比非目标“小”或“大”)做出感知判断(即比非目标“小”或“大”),目标的大小低于感知阈值(即0.5毫米或~ 1.3%)。结果表明,自然和哑剧抓取前后的知觉判断不能可靠地区分目标和非目标。自然抓握峰值孔径(PGAs)缩放到目标大小,并且在“之前”和“之后”的感知判断试验中具有可比性——这一结果表明,物理抓握目标的触觉反馈并没有“提高”感知准确性。最值得注意的是,哑剧抓取PGAs对目标尺寸不敏感;也就是说,反应引起的视觉分辨能力低于自然的把握。这些结果提供了趋同的证据,证明哑剧掌握是由与强制性知觉相同的视觉信息介导的,而不是自然掌握的代理。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Distinct visual resolution supports aperture shaping in natural and pantomime-grasping.","authors":"Matthew Heath, Naila Ayala, Maryam Hamidi, Benjamin Tari","doi":"10.1037/cep0000264","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000264","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Pantomime-grasping is a \"simulated\" motor response wherein an individual grasps to an area dissociated from a physical target. The task has been used in the apraxia literature as a proxy for natural grasping (i.e., physically grasping a target); however, it is important to recognize that the task's decoupled spatial relations between stimulus and response renders the top-down processing of target features (e.g., size) that accumulating evidence has shown to be mediated by visual information functionally distinct from natural grasping. Here, we examined whether the visual information supporting pantomime-grasps exhibits a visual resolution power commensurate with natural grasps. Participants were presented with a target and nontarget that differed in size below the perceptual threshold (i.e., 0.5 mm or ∼1.3%) and were asked to make a perceptual judgment about the target (i.e., \"smaller\" or \"larger\" than the nontarget) before and after completing natural and pantomime-grasps. Results showed that perceptual judgments \"before\" and \"after\" natural and pantomime-grasps did not reliably distinguish between target and nontarget. Natural grasp peak grip apertures (PGAs) scaled to target size and were comparable for \"before\" and \"after\" perceptual judgment trials-a result indicating that haptic feedback from physically grasping the target did not \"boost\" perceptual accuracy. Most notably, pantomime-grasp PGAs were insensitive to target size; that is, responses elicited a visual resolution power less than natural grasps. These results provide convergent evidence that pantomime-grasps are mediated by the same visual information as obligatory perceptions and do not provide a proxy for natural grasps. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"76 1","pages":"22-28"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39555476","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
The notion that some mental processes are "automatic" while others are "controlled" is a distinction that appears in virtually all cognition textbooks, as well as in thousands of papers and book chapters. Indeed, so entrenched is the automatic side of this distinction that various leading computational accounts make no mention of it, but instead assume it implicitly. These models, and the field more generally, assume that processing is stimulus triggered and does not need any form of attention or an intention as a preliminary. Further, the fundamental processing dynamics underlying such automatic processing is widely seen as consisting of interactive activation and autonomous in that it unfolds in the same way across contexts. I review a number of findings from my lab that lead me to a different conclusion. Visual word recognition requires a consideration and integrated understanding of automaticity, attention, intention, context, and cognitive processing. I present various findings that challenge the preeminent role ascribed to interactive activation as implemented in the dominant computational models. I conclude that, going forward, the time is due for computational models of visual word recognition (and researchers in the field more generally) to acknowledge that the findings reported here constitute benchmarks that constrain theory and present opportunities for making meaningful advances in our understanding of visual word recognition (and perhaps of cognition more generally). A few proposals for how we might think about some of these processes are offered. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
一些心理过程是“自动的”,而另一些是“受控的”,这一概念几乎出现在所有的认知教科书中,也出现在成千上万的论文和书籍章节中。事实上,这种区别的自动一面是如此根深蒂固,以至于各种领先的计算账户都没有提到它,而是隐含地假设了它。这些模型,以及更广泛的领域,假设处理是由刺激触发的,不需要任何形式的注意或意图作为先决条件。此外,这种自动处理的基本处理动力学被广泛认为是由交互激活和自主组成的,因为它在不同的环境中以相同的方式展开。我回顾了实验室的一些发现,这些发现让我得出了一个不同的结论。视觉词识别需要考虑和综合理解自动性、注意力、意图、语境和认知处理。我提出了各种各样的发现,这些发现挑战了在主流计算模型中实现的交互式激活的卓越作用。我的结论是,展望未来,是时候让视觉词识别的计算模型(以及更广泛的该领域的研究人员)承认,这里报道的发现构成了约束理论的基准,并为我们对视觉词识别的理解(也许是更广泛的认知)取得有意义的进展提供了机会。对于我们如何思考这些过程,提出了一些建议。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Visual word recognition: Attention, intention, context, and processing dynamics.","authors":"D. Besner","doi":"10.1037/cep0000274","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000274","url":null,"abstract":"The notion that some mental processes are \"automatic\" while others are \"controlled\" is a distinction that appears in virtually all cognition textbooks, as well as in thousands of papers and book chapters. Indeed, so entrenched is the automatic side of this distinction that various leading computational accounts make no mention of it, but instead assume it implicitly. These models, and the field more generally, assume that processing is stimulus triggered and does not need any form of attention or an intention as a preliminary. Further, the fundamental processing dynamics underlying such automatic processing is widely seen as consisting of interactive activation and autonomous in that it unfolds in the same way across contexts. I review a number of findings from my lab that lead me to a different conclusion. Visual word recognition requires a consideration and integrated understanding of automaticity, attention, intention, context, and cognitive processing. I present various findings that challenge the preeminent role ascribed to interactive activation as implemented in the dominant computational models. I conclude that, going forward, the time is due for computational models of visual word recognition (and researchers in the field more generally) to acknowledge that the findings reported here constitute benchmarks that constrain theory and present opportunities for making meaningful advances in our understanding of visual word recognition (and perhaps of cognition more generally). A few proposals for how we might think about some of these processes are offered. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"300 1","pages":"57-74"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89037699","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-03-01Epub Date: 2022-02-10DOI: 10.1037/cep0000270
James E Vellan, Craig Leth-Steensen
Across two experiments, the numerical magnitude and the physical size of single digits presented in either two (Experiment 1) or four (Experiment 2) different font sizes were judged using either horizontally and vertically (Experiment 1) or just horizontally (Experiment 2) aligned manual responses. Such a design allowed for the simultaneous examination of the size congruity effect (SiCE), the spatial-numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect, and the more novel spatial-size association of response codes (SSARC) effect. In Experiment 1, SiCEs and SNARC effects were found that operated independently of one another but no SSARC effect occurred. In Experiment 2, separate SiCEs and SNARC effects were found when judging numerical magnitude whereas separate SiCEs and SSARC effects were found when judging physical size. As will be discussed, such findings provide important constraints on the manner in which the full set of congruency and compatibility effects between stimulus and response dimensions in such tasks may be modeled. To illustrate this point, four different versions of a general computational processing model of these effects are considered. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
在两个实验中,使用水平和垂直对齐(实验1)或水平对齐(实验2)的手动响应来判断两种(实验1)或四种(实验2)不同字体大小中呈现的单个数字的数值大小和物理大小。这样的设计允许同时检查大小一致性效应(SiCE)、反应码的空间-数值关联效应(SNARC)和更新颖的反应码的空间-大小关联效应(SSARC)。在实验1中,SiCEs效应和SNARC效应相互独立,不存在SSARC效应。在实验2中,在判断数值量级时存在独立的SiCEs和SNARC效应,在判断物理尺寸时存在独立的SiCEs和SSARC效应。正如我们将要讨论的那样,这些发现为这些任务中刺激和反应维度之间的全部一致性和兼容性效应的建模方式提供了重要的限制。为了说明这一点,考虑了这些效应的一般计算处理模型的四个不同版本。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Separate processing mechanisms for spatial-numerical compatibility and numerical-size congruity.","authors":"James E Vellan, Craig Leth-Steensen","doi":"10.1037/cep0000270","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000270","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Across two experiments, the numerical magnitude and the physical size of single digits presented in either two (Experiment 1) or four (Experiment 2) different font sizes were judged using either horizontally and vertically (Experiment 1) or just horizontally (Experiment 2) aligned manual responses. Such a design allowed for the simultaneous examination of the size congruity effect (SiCE), the spatial-numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect, and the more novel spatial-size association of response codes (SSARC) effect. In Experiment 1, SiCEs and SNARC effects were found that operated independently of one another but no SSARC effect occurred. In Experiment 2, separate SiCEs and SNARC effects were found when judging numerical magnitude whereas separate SiCEs and SSARC effects were found when judging physical size. As will be discussed, such findings provide important constraints on the manner in which the full set of congruency and compatibility effects between stimulus and response dimensions in such tasks may be modeled. To illustrate this point, four different versions of a general computational processing model of these effects are considered. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"76 1","pages":"44-56"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39611409","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
It has been observed that different linguistic experiences might exert a differential effect on general cognitive processes. For example, research has shown that language control in professional translation differs from language control applied to other types of bilingual activities. The present study focuses on the construct of automaticity and aims at determining whether different linguistic experiences might modulate the balance between automaticity and cognitive control at the general cognitive level. Hence, monolinguals, bilinguals, and professional translators performed a memory search task that has extensively been employed to observe how automaticity is acquired through consistent practice. Comparisons between the groups showed overall differences in the ease with which the task was performed and, importantly, differences in both automaticity and cognitive control. Specifically, monolinguals showed higher levels of automaticity in the learning phase of the task, while bilinguals and professional translations carried out the task in a more controlled fashion. This pattern might have implied higher cognitive costs for the monolingual group when a switched learning condition was presented. Possibly due to previous control over the initial learning phase, bilinguals and translators were less affected by the cognitive costs associated to the reversal of the learning condition. Differences are explained in terms of professional translation and everyday bilingual practice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
不同的语言经验可能对一般认知过程产生不同的影响。例如,研究表明,专业翻译中的语言控制不同于其他类型的双语活动中的语言控制。本研究着眼于自动性的结构,旨在确定不同的语言经验是否会在一般认知水平上调节自动性和认知控制之间的平衡。因此,单语者、双语者和专业翻译人员执行了一项记忆搜索任务,该任务被广泛用于观察如何通过持续的练习获得自动性。两组之间的比较显示了完成任务的难易程度的总体差异,更重要的是,在自动性和认知控制方面的差异。具体来说,单语者在任务的学习阶段表现出更高的自动性,而双语者和专业翻译者在任务的执行过程中更受控制。这种模式可能意味着,当出现切换学习条件时,单语组的认知成本更高。可能由于先前对初始学习阶段的控制,双语者和翻译者受到与学习条件逆转相关的认知成本的影响较小。从专业翻译和日常双语实践的角度来解释差异。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Automaticity and cognitive control in bilingual and translation expertise.","authors":"Giulia Togato, P. Macizo, T. Bajo","doi":"10.1037/cep0000268","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000268","url":null,"abstract":"It has been observed that different linguistic experiences might exert a differential effect on general cognitive processes. For example, research has shown that language control in professional translation differs from language control applied to other types of bilingual activities. The present study focuses on the construct of automaticity and aims at determining whether different linguistic experiences might modulate the balance between automaticity and cognitive control at the general cognitive level. Hence, monolinguals, bilinguals, and professional translators performed a memory search task that has extensively been employed to observe how automaticity is acquired through consistent practice. Comparisons between the groups showed overall differences in the ease with which the task was performed and, importantly, differences in both automaticity and cognitive control. Specifically, monolinguals showed higher levels of automaticity in the learning phase of the task, while bilinguals and professional translations carried out the task in a more controlled fashion. This pattern might have implied higher cognitive costs for the monolingual group when a switched learning condition was presented. Possibly due to previous control over the initial learning phase, bilinguals and translators were less affected by the cognitive costs associated to the reversal of the learning condition. Differences are explained in terms of professional translation and everyday bilingual practice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"12 1","pages":"29-43"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"78370110","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-12-01Epub Date: 2021-10-28DOI: 10.1037/cep0000265
Willem Millett, Daniel Voyer
This study examined potential sex differences in the application of models of curve tracing, namely the pixel-by-pixel model, the bipartite model, and the zoom lens model. The purpose of this study was therefore to determine whether sex differences existed in terms of reliance on a particular model or whether the results of each sex could be best explained by one model. This was done by examining the combined data obtained by Voyer and MacPherson (2020), consisting of 420 participants, with 194 men and 226 women. We examined only the curve-tracing task data from that study and compared the fit of the different models as well as a possible interaction with sex of participants on the proportion of correct responses and response time. Overall, sex was a significant factor, with men showing better average accuracy and faster performance than women. On accuracy, we found that the pixel-by-pixel model provided the best fit for women, whereas the zoom lens model produced the best fit for men. On response time, the zoom model was the best predictor of response time for both sexes. The discussion elaborates on an account of these findings and on how our results might generalize to other visual-spatial tasks where a performance advantage for men is found. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
本研究探讨了在曲线追踪模型的应用中潜在的性别差异,即逐像素模型、二部模型和变焦镜头模型。因此,这项研究的目的是确定性别差异是否存在于对特定模型的依赖上,或者是否每个性别的结果都可以用一个模型来最好地解释。这是通过检查Voyer和MacPherson(2020)获得的综合数据来完成的,该数据由420名参与者组成,其中194名男性和226名女性。我们只检查了来自该研究的曲线追踪任务数据,并比较了不同模型的拟合以及参与者性别对正确反应比例和反应时间的可能相互作用。总的来说,性别是一个重要因素,男性比女性表现出更好的平均准确率和更快的表现。在准确性方面,我们发现逐像素模型最适合女性,而变焦镜头模型最适合男性。在反应时间上,缩放模型是男女反应时间的最佳预测器。讨论详细阐述了这些发现,以及我们的结果如何可以推广到其他视觉空间任务中,在这些任务中发现了男性的表现优势。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Picking up the pieces: Sex differences in mechanisms of curve tracing.","authors":"Willem Millett, Daniel Voyer","doi":"10.1037/cep0000265","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000265","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study examined potential sex differences in the application of models of curve tracing, namely the pixel-by-pixel model, the bipartite model, and the zoom lens model. The purpose of this study was therefore to determine whether sex differences existed in terms of reliance on a particular model or whether the results of each sex could be best explained by one model. This was done by examining the combined data obtained by Voyer and MacPherson (2020), consisting of 420 participants, with 194 men and 226 women. We examined only the curve-tracing task data from that study and compared the fit of the different models as well as a possible interaction with sex of participants on the proportion of correct responses and response time. Overall, sex was a significant factor, with men showing better average accuracy and faster performance than women. On accuracy, we found that the pixel-by-pixel model provided the best fit for women, whereas the zoom lens model produced the best fit for men. On response time, the zoom model was the best predictor of response time for both sexes. The discussion elaborates on an account of these findings and on how our results might generalize to other visual-spatial tasks where a performance advantage for men is found. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 4","pages":"387-392"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39582958","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-12-01Epub Date: 2021-03-25DOI: 10.1037/cep0000234
Chris Westbury, Geoff Hollis
Although studies of humour are as old as the Western academic tradition, most theories are too vague to allow for modelling and prediction of humour judgments. Previous work in modelling humour judgments has succeeded by focusing on the world's worst jokes: the slight humour of single nonwords (Westbury, Shaoul, Moroschan, & Ramscar, 2016) and single words (Westbury & Hollis, 2019). Here that work is extended to the world's third-worst jokes, adjective-noun pairs such as dancing dildo, flabby goldfish, and pompous snack. Participants used best-worst scaling to rate the humour of random word pairs. Those judgments were modelled using both linear regression and genetic programming, which is not constrained by assumptions of linearity. The linear regression models were as successful as the nonlinear models at predicting humour judgments, accounting for 27% of the variance in a 540-item validation set. Predictors associated only with the noun and with the relationship between the adjective and noun accounted for much more variance (over 14% each) than predictors associated only with the adjective (6.3%). Greater cosine distance of the adjective word2vec vector from the vectors of the shared neighbors of the noun and adjective is associated with higher humour ratings, whereas the opposite relationship is true for the noun. This captures a form of incongruity not seen in single items, by which neighbours of the adjective become unexpectedly relevant only when the noun brings them into focus. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
{"title":"A pompous snack: On the unreasonable complexity of the world's third-worst jokes.","authors":"Chris Westbury, Geoff Hollis","doi":"10.1037/cep0000234","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000234","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although studies of humour are as old as the Western academic tradition, most theories are too vague to allow for modelling and prediction of humour judgments. Previous work in modelling humour judgments has succeeded by focusing on the world's worst jokes: the slight humour of single nonwords (Westbury, Shaoul, Moroschan, & Ramscar, 2016) and single words (Westbury & Hollis, 2019). Here that work is extended to the world's third-worst jokes, adjective-noun pairs such as <i>dancing dildo, flabby goldfish, and pompous snack</i>. Participants used best-worst scaling to rate the humour of random word pairs. Those judgments were modelled using both linear regression and genetic programming, which is not constrained by assumptions of linearity. The linear regression models were as successful as the nonlinear models at predicting humour judgments, accounting for 27% of the variance in a 540-item validation set. Predictors associated only with the noun and with the relationship between the adjective and noun accounted for much more variance (over 14% each) than predictors associated only with the adjective (6.3%). Greater cosine distance of the adjective word2vec vector from the vectors of the shared neighbors of the noun and adjective is associated with higher humour ratings, whereas the opposite relationship is true for the noun. This captures a form of incongruity not seen in single items, by which neighbours of the adjective become unexpectedly relevant only when the noun brings them into focus. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 4","pages":"327-347"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25514598","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-12-01Epub Date: 2021-06-07DOI: 10.1037/cep0000253
Colin M MacLeod
This article presents a survey of the first 70 years of this journal, covering (a) the origin and subsequent history of the journal, (b) who the Editors have been, (c) how the Editors have influenced the journal, (d) the most highly cited articles, and (e) consideration of the journal's content. After shifts in its purpose over its first two decades, the journal settled into being an outlet that is well respected around the world for research in the field of human experimental psychology. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
本文对该期刊的前70年进行了调查,包括(a)期刊的起源和后来的历史,(b)编辑都是谁,(c)编辑如何影响期刊,(d)被引用次数最多的文章,以及(e)对期刊内容的考虑。在最初的二十年里,该杂志的宗旨发生了变化,后来成为了一个在人类实验心理学领域受到世界尊重的研究渠道。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Canadian Journal of (Experimental) Psychology: The first 70 years.","authors":"Colin M MacLeod","doi":"10.1037/cep0000253","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000253","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This article presents a survey of the first 70 years of this journal, covering (a) the origin and subsequent history of the journal, (b) who the Editors have been, (c) how the Editors have influenced the journal, (d) the most highly cited articles, and (e) consideration of the journal's content. After shifts in its purpose over its first two decades, the journal settled into being an outlet that is well respected around the world for research in the field of human experimental psychology. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 4","pages":"393-402"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39068152","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-12-01Epub Date: 2021-03-25DOI: 10.1037/cep0000232
Anna Michelle McPhee, Merryn D Constable, Elizabeth J Saccone, Timothy N Welsh
Humans operate in complex environments where social interactions require individuals to constantly attend to people and objects around them. Despite the complexity of these interactions from a visuomotor perspective, humans can engage and thrive in social settings. The purpose of the current study was to examine the simultaneous influence of multiple social cues (i.e., ownership and the presence of a coactor) on the processing of objects. Participants performed an object-based compatibility task in the presence and absence of a coacting confederate. Participants indicated whether pictures of mugs (that were either self-owned or unowned) were upright or inverted. The pictures appeared at one of 2 locations (a near or far location relative to the participant) on a computer screen laid flat on (parallel to) the tabletop. When present, the coactor stood on the opposite side of the screen/table. Analysis of response times (RTs) indicated that the processing of objects was influenced by the object's ownership status, the presence of the coactor, and where the object was located on the screen. Specifically, RTs for pictures of self-owned mugs were shorter than unowned mugs, but only when the pictures were located at the near location. Further, the presence of a confederate resulted in shorter RTs for pictures located at the near but not the far location. These findings suggest that when objects were placed at the far location, the additional social cues of ownership and social context did not influence visuomotor processing of the objects. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
人类在复杂的环境中活动,社会互动需要个体不断地关注周围的人和物。尽管从视觉运动的角度来看,这些互动是复杂的,但人类可以在社会环境中参与并茁壮成长。本研究的目的是考察多种社会线索(即所有权和合作者的存在)对物体加工的同时影响。参与者在有或没有合作伙伴的情况下执行基于对象的兼容性任务。参与者指出马克杯的图片(无论是自己的还是没有自己的)是直立的还是倒置的。图片出现在与桌面平行的电脑屏幕上的两个位置之一(相对于参与者的近或远位置)。在场时,摄影师站在屏幕/桌子的对面。对响应时间(RTs)的分析表明,对象的处理受到对象的所有权状态、协同器的存在以及对象在屏幕上的位置的影响。具体来说,自持马克杯照片的时间比无主马克杯短,但只有当照片位于附近位置时才如此。此外,同盟者的存在会导致位于近而非远位置的图片的RTs更短。这些发现表明,当物体被放置在较远的位置时,额外的社会线索(所有权和社会背景)并不影响物体的视觉运动加工。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"The influence of location, ownership, and the presence of a coactor on the processing of objects.","authors":"Anna Michelle McPhee, Merryn D Constable, Elizabeth J Saccone, Timothy N Welsh","doi":"10.1037/cep0000232","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000232","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Humans operate in complex environments where social interactions require individuals to constantly attend to people and objects around them. Despite the complexity of these interactions from a visuomotor perspective, humans can engage and thrive in social settings. The purpose of the current study was to examine the simultaneous influence of multiple social cues (i.e., ownership and the presence of a coactor) on the processing of objects. Participants performed an object-based compatibility task in the presence and absence of a coacting confederate. Participants indicated whether pictures of mugs (that were either self-owned or unowned) were upright or inverted. The pictures appeared at one of 2 locations (a near or far location relative to the participant) on a computer screen laid flat on (parallel to) the tabletop. When present, the coactor stood on the opposite side of the screen/table. Analysis of response times (RTs) indicated that the processing of objects was influenced by the object's ownership status, the presence of the coactor, and where the object was located on the screen. Specifically, RTs for pictures of self-owned mugs were shorter than unowned mugs, but only when the pictures were located at the near location. Further, the presence of a confederate resulted in shorter RTs for pictures located at the near but not the far location. These findings suggest that when objects were placed at the far location, the additional social cues of ownership and social context did not influence visuomotor processing of the objects. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 4","pages":"362-373"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25514151","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-12-01Epub Date: 2021-06-07DOI: 10.1037/cep0000262
Ge Xu, Jiexuan Lin, Yanping Dong
The issue of bilingual phonological access remains unclear for bilinguals with cross-script language systems, which is especially true when the time course of phonological activation is involved. To investigate the time course of cross-script phonological activation, the present study asked Chinese-English bilinguals to complete a word naming task that was conducted in a forward-masked phonological priming paradigm in three stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) conditions. By comparing the interlingual and intralingual phonological priming effects in a within-subjects design, we found that (a) target naming in Chinese and English was facilitated by a phonologically similar English or Chinese prime in the three SOA conditions (43 ms, 75 ms, and 150 ms) and the facilitation effect of the prime reached the peak when the pronunciation of the prime-target pair most resembled each other and (b) manipulation of the SOAs affected both the naming latencies of target words and the sizes of the phonological priming effect. In particular, naming latencies in each prime-target type displayed an increasing tendency as the SOA prolonged. Moreover, despite the varied sizes of the priming effect in the three SOA conditions, we found a consistent pattern that the priming effects in two interlingual conditions resembled their respective intralingual conditions along the time course. Taken together, these findings provide strong support for an integrated phonological representation of bilinguals and further extend the language nonselective access hypothesis to language pairs with very different orthographic systems. Implications for the manipulation of the SOAs in the masked priming paradigm are also discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
对于具有跨文字语言系统的双语者来说,双语语音通路的问题仍然不清楚,特别是当涉及语音激活的时间过程时。为了研究跨文字语音激活的时间过程,本研究要求中英双语者在三种刺激启动异步(SOA)条件下,在前向掩蔽语音启动范式下完成单词命名任务。通过比较被试内部设计中的语际和语内语音启动效应,我们发现(a)在3种SOA条件下(43 ms、75 ms和75 ms),语音相似的英语或汉语启动效应促进了汉语和英语目标的命名。当启动词和目标词对的发音最相似时,启动词的促进效应达到峰值。(b)对目标词的命名潜伏期和语音启动效应的大小,都受到对目标词的命名潜伏期的影响。特别是,随着SOA的延长,每种主要目标类型中的命名延迟呈现出增加的趋势。此外,尽管三种SOA条件下的启动效应大小不同,但我们发现了一个一致的模式,即两种语际条件下的启动效应在时间过程中与其各自的语内条件相似。综上所述,这些发现为双语者的综合语音表征提供了强有力的支持,并进一步将语言非选择获取假说扩展到具有不同正字法系统的语言对。还讨论了在掩蔽启动范式中对soa进行操作的含义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Cross-script phonological activation in Chinese-English bilinguals: The effect of SOA from masked priming.","authors":"Ge Xu, Jiexuan Lin, Yanping Dong","doi":"10.1037/cep0000262","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000262","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The issue of bilingual phonological access remains unclear for bilinguals with cross-script language systems, which is especially true when the time course of phonological activation is involved. To investigate the time course of cross-script phonological activation, the present study asked Chinese-English bilinguals to complete a word naming task that was conducted in a forward-masked phonological priming paradigm in three stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) conditions. By comparing the interlingual and intralingual phonological priming effects in a within-subjects design, we found that (a) target naming in Chinese and English was facilitated by a phonologically similar English or Chinese prime in the three SOA conditions (43 ms, 75 ms, and 150 ms) and the facilitation effect of the prime reached the peak when the pronunciation of the prime-target pair most resembled each other and (b) manipulation of the SOAs affected both the naming latencies of target words and the sizes of the phonological priming effect. In particular, naming latencies in each prime-target type displayed an increasing tendency as the SOA prolonged. Moreover, despite the varied sizes of the priming effect in the three SOA conditions, we found a consistent pattern that the priming effects in two interlingual conditions resembled their respective intralingual conditions along the time course. Taken together, these findings provide strong support for an integrated phonological representation of bilinguals and further extend the language nonselective access hypothesis to language pairs with very different orthographic systems. Implications for the manipulation of the SOAs in the masked priming paradigm are also discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 4","pages":"374-386"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39068153","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
We explored whether control processes could account for age-related differences in internal strategy use, which in turn would contribute to episodic and working memory decline in aging. Young and older adults completed the internal strategy subscale of the Metamemory in Adulthood (MIA) questionnaire, a free-recall task (FRT), a reading span task (RST), and 3 executive control tasks (the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, the Initial Letter Fluency Test, and the Digit Symbol Substitution Test) allowing us to calculate a composite index of control processes. Results indicated that both self-reported internal strategy use and control processes index accounted for a significant proportion of the age-related variance in the FRT and the RST. However, once the control processes index was controlled for, variance in both the FRT and RST explained by internal strategy use were significantly reduced. Additionally, age-related variance in internal strategy use was mediated by the control processes index. These results suggest a cascade model in which individual control level would mediate age-related differences in internal strategy use, which in turn would mediate age-related differences in episodic and working memory performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
{"title":"Effect of self-reported internal memory strategy use on age-related episodic and working memory decline: Contribution of control processes.","authors":"Lina Guerrero, Michel Isingrini, Lucie Angel, Séverine Fay, Laurence Taconnat, Badiâa Bouazzaoui","doi":"10.1037/cep0000240","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000240","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We explored whether control processes could account for age-related differences in internal strategy use, which in turn would contribute to episodic and working memory decline in aging. Young and older adults completed the internal strategy subscale of the Metamemory in Adulthood (MIA) questionnaire, a free-recall task (FRT), a reading span task (RST), and 3 executive control tasks (the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, the Initial Letter Fluency Test, and the Digit Symbol Substitution Test) allowing us to calculate a composite index of control processes. Results indicated that both self-reported internal strategy use and control processes index accounted for a significant proportion of the age-related variance in the FRT and the RST. However, once the control processes index was controlled for, variance in both the FRT and RST explained by internal strategy use were significantly reduced. Additionally, age-related variance in internal strategy use was mediated by the control processes index. These results suggest a cascade model in which individual control level would mediate age-related differences in internal strategy use, which in turn would mediate age-related differences in episodic and working memory performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 4","pages":"348-361"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39209290","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}