首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Adult Development最新文献

英文 中文
Age Difference in the Effect of Relationship Closeness on Interpersonal Escalating Commitment 关系亲密程度对人际关系升级承诺影响的年龄差异
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10804-024-09480-z
Jingxuan Wu, Xiaoning Wen, Xueping Liu, Menghan Jin, Huamao Peng

Interpersonal escalating commitment is an individual’s continued investment in a social partner regardless of the receiver’s negative feedback. It reflects the effort that people devote into maintaining and managing interpersonal relationships. This study focused on the effect of relationship closeness on interpersonal escalating commitment among older and younger adults and the mediating role of emotional experiences. We performed a 2 (relationship closeness: close/distant) × 2 (age group: younger adults/older adults) mixed design. Forty-eight older adults (aged 60–82) and forty-nine younger adults (aged 18–28) were enrolled in this study. Participants completed eight interpersonal escalating commitment tasks that were derived from real-life situations. Participants’ final and average tendency to escalate commitment and emotional experiences was recorded. The within-subject mediation analysis results indicated that older adults showed a higher tendency to escalate commitment in close relationships through the mediation of positive emotional experiences; younger adults did not exhibit excessive interpersonal escalating commitment even though their final tendency to invest in distant relationships was relatively higher than in close relationships. Our findings revealed an age difference in the effect of relationship closeness on interpersonal escalating commitment and the driving mechanism of emotional experiences among older adults. It allowed us to understand the reasoning behind people’s seemingly irrational interpersonal investments from a perspective of lifespan motivation.

人际关系升级承诺是指一个人对社会伙伴的持续投资,而不管接收者的负面反馈如何。它反映了人们为维持和管理人际关系所付出的努力。本研究主要关注关系亲密程度对老年人和年轻人人际关系升级承诺的影响,以及情感体验的中介作用。我们采用了 2(关系亲密程度:亲密/疏远)×2(年龄组:年轻人/老年人)混合设计。四十八名老年人(60-82 岁)和四十九名年轻人(18-28 岁)参加了这项研究。受试者完成了八项源于真实情境的人际承诺升级任务。研究记录了参与者最终和平均的承诺升级倾向和情感体验。主体内中介分析结果表明,通过积极情感体验的中介作用,老年人在亲密关系中表现出更高的人际升级承诺倾向;而年轻人即使在疏远关系中的最终投资倾向相对高于亲密关系,也没有表现出过度的人际升级承诺。我们的研究结果揭示了关系亲密程度对人际关系升级承诺的影响以及情感体验在老年人中的驱动机制的年龄差异。这使我们能够从寿命动机的角度来理解人们看似非理性的人际投资背后的原因。
{"title":"Age Difference in the Effect of Relationship Closeness on Interpersonal Escalating Commitment","authors":"Jingxuan Wu, Xiaoning Wen, Xueping Liu, Menghan Jin, Huamao Peng","doi":"10.1007/s10804-024-09480-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10804-024-09480-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Interpersonal escalating commitment is an individual’s continued investment in a social partner regardless of the receiver’s negative feedback. It reflects the effort that people devote into maintaining and managing interpersonal relationships. This study focused on the effect of relationship closeness on interpersonal escalating commitment among older and younger adults and the mediating role of emotional experiences. We performed a 2 (relationship closeness: close/distant) × 2 (age group: younger adults/older adults) mixed design. Forty-eight older adults (aged 60–82) and forty-nine younger adults (aged 18–28) were enrolled in this study. Participants completed eight interpersonal escalating commitment tasks that were derived from real-life situations. Participants’ final and average tendency to escalate commitment and emotional experiences was recorded. The within-subject mediation analysis results indicated that older adults showed a higher tendency to escalate commitment in close relationships through the mediation of positive emotional experiences; younger adults did not exhibit excessive interpersonal escalating commitment even though their final tendency to invest in distant relationships was relatively higher than in close relationships. Our findings revealed an age difference in the effect of relationship closeness on interpersonal escalating commitment and the driving mechanism of emotional experiences among older adults. It allowed us to understand the reasoning behind people’s seemingly irrational interpersonal investments from a perspective of lifespan motivation.</p>","PeriodicalId":51546,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Adult Development","volume":"28 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140884192","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evaluating the Connection Between the Neighborhood Context with Daily Negative and Positive Events and Well-Being 评估邻里环境、日常消极和积极事件与幸福感之间的联系
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1007/s10804-024-09479-6
Omar E. Staben, Frank J. Infurna, Kate E. Murray, John S. Hall

An abundance of research documents that daily negative events are linked to short- and long-term health outcomes across adulthood. One key resource that is less studied in potentially mitigating the impact of daily negative events is neighborhood context. Up to this point, much of the research has included social characteristics of neighborhood context, whereas less is known regarding economic and physical characteristics. This study explores whether and to what extent social, economic, and physical neighborhood characteristics moderate the impact of daily negative and positive events on daily well-being as measured by daily levels of positive and negative affect. We use data from a sample of participants in midlife (n = 191, ages 40–65) who completed a daily survey for 30 consecutive days. Multilevel models showed that daily negative events were associated with poorer daily well-being and daily positive events were predictive of higher daily well-being. Living in a neighborhood with more income inequality was associated with stronger declines in daily well-being on days when individuals experienced a negative event. Neighborhood greenness was associated with reporting better well-being on days when a positive event occurred. The findings suggest that the neighborhood context can strengthen or mitigate changes in daily well-being when negative and positive events occur. The discussion elaborates on how the findings can inform interventions aimed at lessening the consequences of daily negative events and elucidates future research aimed at exploring potential mechanisms underlying the associations found.

大量研究表明,日常负面事件与整个成年期的短期和长期健康结果有关。在减轻日常负面事件的潜在影响方面,研究较少的一个关键资源是邻里环境。到目前为止,大部分研究都包括邻里环境的社会特征,而对经济和物理特征的研究则较少。本研究探讨了社会、经济和物理邻里特征是否以及在多大程度上减缓了日常负面和正面事件对日常幸福感的影响,日常幸福感是通过日常正面和负面情绪水平来衡量的。我们使用的数据来自连续 30 天完成日常调查的中年参与者样本(n = 191,年龄在 40-65 岁之间)。多层次模型显示,日常负面事件与较差的日常幸福感相关,而日常积极事件则预示着较高的日常幸福感。在经历负面事件的日子里,生活在收入更加不平等的社区与日常幸福感的下降有关。在发生积极事件的日子里,邻里绿化程度与较高的幸福感相关。研究结果表明,当发生消极和积极事件时,邻里环境可以加强或缓解日常幸福感的变化。讨论详细阐述了研究结果如何为旨在减轻日常负面事件后果的干预措施提供信息,并阐明了旨在探索所发现关联背后潜在机制的未来研究。
{"title":"Evaluating the Connection Between the Neighborhood Context with Daily Negative and Positive Events and Well-Being","authors":"Omar E. Staben, Frank J. Infurna, Kate E. Murray, John S. Hall","doi":"10.1007/s10804-024-09479-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10804-024-09479-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p>An abundance of research documents that daily negative events are linked to short- and long-term health outcomes across adulthood. One key resource that is less studied in potentially mitigating the impact of daily negative events is neighborhood context. Up to this point, much of the research has included social characteristics of neighborhood context, whereas less is known regarding economic and physical characteristics. This study explores whether and to what extent social, economic, and physical neighborhood characteristics moderate the impact of daily negative and positive events on daily well-being as measured by daily levels of positive and negative affect. We use data from a sample of participants in midlife (<i>n</i> = 191<i>,</i> ages 40–65) who completed a daily survey for 30 consecutive days. Multilevel models showed that daily negative events were associated with poorer daily well-being and daily positive events were predictive of higher daily well-being. Living in a neighborhood with more income inequality was associated with stronger declines in daily well-being on days when individuals experienced a negative event. Neighborhood greenness was associated with reporting better well-being on days when a positive event occurred. The findings suggest that the neighborhood context can strengthen or mitigate changes in daily well-being when negative and positive events occur. The discussion elaborates on how the findings can inform interventions aimed at lessening the consequences of daily negative events and elucidates future research aimed at exploring potential mechanisms underlying the associations found.</p>","PeriodicalId":51546,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Adult Development","volume":"18 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140836492","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Age-Differential Effects of Proactive Control in Dual Tasking: The Moderating Effect of Task Difficulty 双重任务中主动控制的年龄差异效应:任务难度的调节作用
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-12 DOI: 10.1007/s10804-024-09482-x
Yue Hu, Helene H. Fung, Xianmin Gong

Dual tasking refers to the ability to perform two concurrent tasks. Using the psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm, two experiments examined whether providing a prompt that facilitated proactive control could benefit dual-task performance among younger and older adults. In Experiment 1, difficulty-related prompt words (“difficult,” “easy,” or null) were presented before easier dual tasks with a longer stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) of 800 ms or harder tasks with a shorter SOA of 100 ms. Experiment 2 extended the investigation by presenting these prompts (“difficult” or “easy”) before dual tasks with a fixed SOA of 150 ms. It also examined the moderating effects of actual task difficulty by manipulating task congruency. Both experiments suggested that proactive control triggered by difficulty-related prompts facilitated dual-task performance in both age groups. Notably, prompts benefited younger adults’ dual-task performance only when the actual task difficulty was relatively higher, but they benefited older adults’ dual-task performance regardless of the actual task difficulty. These findings contribute to our understanding of proactive control and the different effects of prompts on cognitive performance among younger and older adults.

双重任务是指同时完成两项任务的能力。利用心理折射期(PRP)范式,有两项实验考察了提供有助于主动控制的提示词是否能提高年轻人和老年人的双重任务表现。在实验 1 中,与难度相关的提示词("难"、"易 "或空)会出现在刺激开始不同步时间(SOA)为 800 毫秒的较容易的双重任务或 SOA 为 100 毫秒的较难的任务之前。实验 2 扩展了这一研究,在固定 SOA 为 150 毫秒的双重任务前呈现这些提示("难 "或 "易")。实验 2 还通过操纵任务一致性,考察了实际任务难度的调节作用。这两项实验都表明,与难度相关的提示所引发的主动控制对两个年龄组的双任务表现都有促进作用。值得注意的是,只有当实际任务难度相对较高时,提示才会有利于年轻人的双任务表现,但无论实际任务难度如何,提示都会有利于老年人的双任务表现。这些发现有助于我们理解主动控制以及提示对年轻人和老年人认知表现的不同影响。
{"title":"Age-Differential Effects of Proactive Control in Dual Tasking: The Moderating Effect of Task Difficulty","authors":"Yue Hu, Helene H. Fung, Xianmin Gong","doi":"10.1007/s10804-024-09482-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10804-024-09482-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Dual tasking refers to the ability to perform two concurrent tasks. Using the psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm, two experiments examined whether providing a prompt that facilitated proactive control could benefit dual-task performance among younger and older adults. In Experiment 1, difficulty-related prompt words (“difficult,” “easy,” or null) were presented before easier dual tasks with a longer stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) of 800 ms or harder tasks with a shorter SOA of 100 ms. Experiment 2 extended the investigation by presenting these prompts (“difficult” or “easy”) before dual tasks with a fixed SOA of 150 ms. It also examined the moderating effects of actual task difficulty by manipulating task congruency. Both experiments suggested that proactive control triggered by difficulty-related prompts facilitated dual-task performance in both age groups. Notably, prompts benefited younger adults’ dual-task performance only when the actual task difficulty was relatively higher, but they benefited older adults’ dual-task performance regardless of the actual task difficulty. These findings contribute to our understanding of proactive control and the different effects of prompts on cognitive performance among younger and older adults.</p>","PeriodicalId":51546,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Adult Development","volume":"7 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140601602","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceived Overprotective Parenting and Identity Synthesis and Confusion in Georgian Emerging Adults: The Intervening Role of Goal Engagement and Disengagement 格鲁吉亚新兴成年人对父母过度保护的感知与身份的综合和混乱:目标参与和脱离的干预作用
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-23 DOI: 10.1007/s10804-024-09475-w
Nino Skhirtladze, Stijn Van Petegem

Overprotective parenting refers to parents’ provision of developmentally inappropriate protection and is associated with dysfunctional developmental processes and outcomes in adolescence and emerging adulthood. In the present study, we examined whether perceived overprotective parenting is potentially associated with identity synthesis and identity confusion, as the development of a coherent sense of identity is conceived as one of the primary goals of adolescence and emerging adulthood. Thereby, we also tested whether developmentally regulatory strategies of goal engagement and disengagement are intervening variables in the association between overprotective parenting and identity. Participants were 318 Georgian emerging adults (Mage = 23.41, 58% female). Path analyses indicated that perceived overprotective parenting was directly associated with less identity synthesis and more identity confusion. It was found that overprotection was unrelated to goal engagement, but was associated with more goal disengagement. Goal engagement, in turn, was positively associated with identity synthesis and negatively associated with identity confusion, whereas goal disengagement was positively associated with identity confusion. These results suggest that overprotective parenting creates a family context that is negatively associated with the successful resolution of the identity formation task. Goal disengagement, a relatively passive, avoidant developmental strategy, particularly played an intervening role in this association between overprotection and identity confusion.

过度保护性养育指的是父母提供的保护与发展不相适应,并与青春期和成年期的功能失调发展过程和结果有关。在本研究中,我们探讨了感知到的过度保护性养育是否可能与身份综合和身份混淆有关,因为发展一致的身份感被认为是青春期和新兴成人期的主要目标之一。因此,我们还测试了目标参与和脱离的发展调节策略是否是过度保护性养育与身份认同之间关系的干预变量。参与者为 318 名格鲁吉亚新成人(年龄 = 23.41,58% 为女性)。路径分析表明,感知到的过度保护性养育与较少的身份综合和较多的身份混乱直接相关。研究发现,过度保护与目标参与无关,但与更多的目标脱离相关。反过来,目标参与与身份综合呈正相关,与身份混乱呈负相关,而目标脱离与身份混乱呈正相关。这些结果表明,过度保护性的养育方式所创造的家庭环境与身份形成任务的成功完成呈负相关。目标脱离作为一种相对被动、回避的发展策略,在过度保护与身份混淆之间的关系中尤其起到了干预作用。
{"title":"Perceived Overprotective Parenting and Identity Synthesis and Confusion in Georgian Emerging Adults: The Intervening Role of Goal Engagement and Disengagement","authors":"Nino Skhirtladze, Stijn Van Petegem","doi":"10.1007/s10804-024-09475-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10804-024-09475-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Overprotective parenting refers to parents’ provision of developmentally inappropriate protection and is associated with dysfunctional developmental processes and outcomes in adolescence and emerging adulthood. In the present study, we examined whether perceived overprotective parenting is potentially associated with identity synthesis and identity confusion, as the development of a coherent sense of identity is conceived as one of the primary goals of adolescence and emerging adulthood. Thereby, we also tested whether developmentally regulatory strategies of goal engagement and disengagement are intervening variables in the association between overprotective parenting and identity. Participants were 318 Georgian emerging adults (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 23.41, 58% female). Path analyses indicated that perceived overprotective parenting was directly associated with less identity synthesis and more identity confusion. It was found that overprotection was unrelated to goal engagement, but was associated with more goal disengagement. Goal engagement, in turn, was positively associated with identity synthesis and negatively associated with identity confusion, whereas goal disengagement was positively associated with identity confusion. These results suggest that overprotective parenting creates a family context that is negatively associated with the successful resolution of the identity formation task. Goal disengagement, a relatively passive, avoidant developmental strategy, particularly played an intervening role in this association between overprotection and identity confusion.</p>","PeriodicalId":51546,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Adult Development","volume":"11 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-03-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140204532","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Childhood Experiences of Disorganization and Role Reversal Scale: Relations with Childhood Adversity, Internalizing Problems, and Well-Being in Young Adulthood 童年经历混乱和角色逆转量表:与童年逆境、内化问题和成年后福祉的关系
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-22 DOI: 10.1007/s10804-024-09474-x

Abstract

During the transition from infancy to preschool years, children develop controlling-caregiving and controlling-punitive attachments, in addition to continuing to show disorganized behaviors, as part of a larger disorganized attachment spectrum. Despite advances in understanding disorganization in young adulthood, little is known about the controlling patterns and their relations to psychological health. This is likely due to a scarcity of well-validated and cost-effective measures assessing all three disorganized patterns in young adulthood. The Childhood Disorganization and Role Reversal Scale (CDRR) addresses this need by assessing dimensions of disorganization and role reversal, such as hostility, child affective caregiving, and child punitiveness, in addition to appropriate parent–child boundaries. Since the CDRR is a recent measure, this study aimed to further validate it by (a) evaluating whether its factor structure replicates in a university student sample (N = 286, M(SD) = 19.3(1.6); 74.48% female) and (b) assessing its construct validity through associations with conceptually relevant constructs such as childhood adversity, internalizing problems (depression and anxiety), and subjective well-being (happiness, life satisfaction, and meaning in life). Confirmatory factor analyses provided evidence for the original four-factor structure CDRR with mothers (Disorganized/Punitive, Affective Caregiving, Mutual Hostility, Appropriate Boundaries) and fathers (Disorganized, Affective Caregiving, Punitive, Appropriate Boundaries). Associations with childhood adversity, internalizing problems, and subjective well-being were in the expected directions. Overall, the results provide additional evidence for the validity of the CDRR and the importance of evaluating disorganized and controlling attachments in young adulthood.

摘要 在从婴儿期向学龄前期过渡的过程中,儿童除了继续表现出无组织的行为外,还会发展出控制-照料型依恋和控制-惩罚型依恋,这是一个更大的无组织依恋谱系的一部分。尽管人们对幼年期无组织依恋的理解有所进步,但对控制模式及其与心理健康的关系却知之甚少。这可能是由于缺乏有效且具有成本效益的测量方法来评估青少年成年期的三种混乱模式。儿童期组织混乱和角色逆转量表(Childhood Disorganization and Role Reversal Scale, CDRR)通过评估组织混乱和角色逆转的各个方面,如敌意、儿童情感照顾和儿童惩罚性,以及适当的亲子界限,来满足这一需求。由于 CDRR 是一种最新的测量方法,本研究旨在通过以下方法对其进行进一步验证:(a)评估其因子结构是否能在大学生样本(人数 = 286,中位数(标准差)= 19.3(1.6);74.48% 为女性)中复制;(b)通过与童年逆境、内化问题(抑郁和焦虑)和主观幸福感(幸福感、生活满意度和人生意义)等概念相关的建构相关性来评估其建构效度。确认性因子分析证明了 CDRR 最初的四因子结构,即母亲(无组织/惩罚性、情感关怀、相互敌对、适当界限)和父亲(无组织、情感关怀、惩罚性、适当界限)。与童年逆境、内化问题和主观幸福感之间的关系符合预期方向。总之,研究结果进一步证明了 CDRR 的有效性,以及评估青少年时期无组织依恋和控制性依恋的重要性。
{"title":"The Childhood Experiences of Disorganization and Role Reversal Scale: Relations with Childhood Adversity, Internalizing Problems, and Well-Being in Young Adulthood","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s10804-024-09474-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10804-024-09474-x","url":null,"abstract":"<h3>Abstract</h3> <p>During the transition from infancy to preschool years, children develop controlling-caregiving and controlling-punitive attachments, in addition to continuing to show disorganized behaviors, as part of a larger disorganized attachment spectrum. Despite advances in understanding disorganization in young adulthood, little is known about the controlling patterns and their relations to psychological health. This is likely due to a scarcity of well-validated and cost-effective measures assessing all three disorganized patterns in young adulthood. The <em>Childhood Disorganization and Role Reversal Scale</em> (CDRR) addresses this need by assessing dimensions of disorganization and role reversal, such as hostility, child affective caregiving, and child punitiveness, in addition to appropriate parent–child boundaries. Since the CDRR is a recent measure, this study aimed to further validate it by (a) evaluating whether its factor structure replicates in a university student sample (<em>N</em> = 286, <em>M</em>(<em>SD</em>) = 19.3(1.6); 74.48% female) and (b) assessing its construct validity through associations with conceptually relevant constructs such as childhood adversity, internalizing problems (depression and anxiety), and subjective well-being (happiness, life satisfaction, and meaning in life). Confirmatory factor analyses provided evidence for the original four-factor structure CDRR with mothers (Disorganized/Punitive, Affective Caregiving, Mutual Hostility, Appropriate Boundaries) and fathers (Disorganized, Affective Caregiving, Punitive, Appropriate Boundaries). Associations with childhood adversity, internalizing problems, and subjective well-being were in the expected directions. Overall, the results provide additional evidence for the validity of the CDRR and the importance of evaluating disorganized and controlling attachments in young adulthood.</p>","PeriodicalId":51546,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Adult Development","volume":"10 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-03-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140204617","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceptions of Adulthood: What Does it Mean to be Grown-Up? 对成年的看法:长大意味着什么?
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10804-024-09477-8

Abstract

Adulthood has traditionally been defined by the attainment of socio-demographic milestones such as career, marriage, and parenthood, but these milestones are increasingly delayed or have become inaccessible for young people today. As a result, young people are likely to experience a discrepancy between their expectations for and their actual reality of adulthood, which can negatively affect their well-being and psychological development. To systematically study contemporary definitions of and attitudes towards adulthood, we assessed a sample of 722 UK adults with an age range from 18 to 77 years on: (a) subjective adult status, or the extent to which people feel like adults; (b) attitudes towards adulthood, or whether people think adulthood is a positive time of life; and (c) the characteristics that people use to define adulthood today. We found that most participants felt adult and had positive attitudes towards adulthood. Our participants defined adulthood predominantly through psychological characteristics, for example “Accepting responsibility for the consequences of my actions” (endorsed by 80% of the sample), rather than by socio-demographic milestones which were endorsed by only 22–40% of participants. Both subjective adult status and attitudes towards adulthood were significantly associated with older age and the attainment of the socio-demographic milestones of marriage and parenthood. Regression analyses revealed that having a positive attitude towards adulthood was the strongest psychological predictor of subjective adult status, accounting for 10% of the variance. This suggests that fostering positive attitudes towards adulthood may help improving the well-being of contemporary adults.

摘要 传统上,成年的定义是达到社会人口学上的里程碑,如事业、婚姻和为人父母。因此,年轻人对成年的期望与现实之间很可能存在差距,这可能会对他们的福祉和心理发展产生负面影响。为了系统地研究当代人对成年的定义和态度,我们对 722 名年龄在 18 岁至 77 岁之间的英国成年人进行了抽样评估:(a) 主观成人状态,即人们在多大程度上觉得自己是成年人;(b) 对成年的态度,即人们是否认为成年是人生中一个积极的时期;以及 (c) 人们用来定义当今成年的特征。我们发现,大多数参与者认为自己已经成年,并对成年持积极态度。我们的参与者主要通过心理特征来定义成年,例如 "对自己行为的后果承担责任"(80% 的样本表示赞同),而不是通过社会人口里程碑来定义成年,只有 22-40% 的参与者表示赞同。主观成人地位和对成人的态度都与年龄的增长以及婚姻和养育子女这些社会人口里程碑的实现有明显的关联。回归分析表明,对成年的积极态度是主观成年状态的最强心理预测因素,占方差的 10%。这表明,培养对成年的积极态度可能有助于改善当代成年人的福祉。
{"title":"Perceptions of Adulthood: What Does it Mean to be Grown-Up?","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s10804-024-09477-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10804-024-09477-8","url":null,"abstract":"<h3>Abstract</h3> <p>Adulthood has traditionally been defined by the attainment of socio-demographic milestones such as career, marriage, and parenthood, but these milestones are increasingly delayed or have become inaccessible for young people today. As a result, young people are likely to experience a discrepancy between their expectations for and their actual reality of adulthood, which can negatively affect their well-being and psychological development. To systematically study contemporary definitions of and attitudes towards adulthood, we assessed a sample of 722 UK adults with an age range from 18 to 77 years on: (a) subjective adult status, or the extent to which people feel like adults; (b) attitudes towards adulthood, or whether people think adulthood is a positive time of life; and (c) the characteristics that people use to define adulthood today. We found that most participants felt adult and had positive attitudes towards adulthood. Our participants defined adulthood predominantly through psychological characteristics, for example “Accepting responsibility for the consequences of my actions” (endorsed by 80% of the sample), rather than by socio-demographic milestones which were endorsed by only 22–40% of participants. Both subjective adult status and attitudes towards adulthood were significantly associated with older age and the attainment of the socio-demographic milestones of marriage and parenthood. Regression analyses revealed that having a positive attitude towards adulthood was the strongest psychological predictor of subjective adult status, accounting for 10% of the variance. This suggests that fostering positive attitudes towards adulthood may help improving the well-being of contemporary adults.</p>","PeriodicalId":51546,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Adult Development","volume":"103 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-03-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140046934","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Predicting Changes in Helicopter Parenting, Fear Of Missing Out (FOMO), and Social Anxiety in College Students 预测大学生直升机养育、错过恐惧(FOMO)和社交焦虑的变化
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10804-024-09478-7
Chengfei Jiao, Ming Cui, Frank D. Fincham

College life involves navigating different educational and career development options, as well as social relationships. The pressure to “fit in” can be stressful and involve the fear of missing out (FOMO) on fun, social events. The college environment is especially challenging for those who experience social anxiety – the fear of being rejected and humiliated in social encounters. One factor that could be related to social difficulties in college students is helicopter parenting – a distinct parenting style characterized by parental over-involvement and over-control, which has been shown to be harmful to college students’ development. Given that college students experience helicopter parenting, FOMO, and social anxiety simultaneously, we examined how these experiences were associated with one another, as well as their stability and change across time. We expected helicopter parenting to be related to college students’ FOMO and social anxiety. Using a 12-week longitudinal design with a sample of 282 college students, we employed an interrelated latent growth curve to determine the associations between changes in helicopter parenting, FOMO, and social anxiety. The results showed that the levels of FOMO decreased over time, and a slower decrease in helicopter parenting was associated with a slower decrease in FOMO. In addition, decreases in social anxiety were associated with decreases in helicopter parenting and FOMO. Implications for practice are discussed.

大学生活涉及不同的教育和职业发展选择以及社会关系。融入社会 "的压力可能会让人感到紧张,也会让人害怕错过有趣的社交活动(FOMO)。对于那些有社交焦虑症的人来说,大学环境尤其具有挑战性--社交焦虑症患者害怕在社交场合遭到拒绝和羞辱。与大学生社交困难有关的一个因素可能是直升机养育,这是一种独特的养育方式,其特点是父母过度介入和过度控制,已被证明对大学生的成长有害。鉴于大学生会同时经历直升机养育、FOMO 和社交焦虑,我们研究了这些经历之间的关联,以及它们在不同时期的稳定性和变化。我们预计直升机养育与大学生的 FOMO 和社交焦虑有关。通过对 282 名大学生样本进行为期 12 周的纵向设计,我们采用了相互关联的潜在增长曲线来确定直升机养育、FOMO 和社交焦虑之间的关联。结果表明,随着时间的推移,FOMO 的水平会下降,而直升机养育的下降速度越慢,FOMO 的下降速度也越慢。此外,社交焦虑的减少与直升机养育和 FOMO 的减少相关。本文讨论了对实践的启示。
{"title":"Predicting Changes in Helicopter Parenting, Fear Of Missing Out (FOMO), and Social Anxiety in College Students","authors":"Chengfei Jiao, Ming Cui, Frank D. Fincham","doi":"10.1007/s10804-024-09478-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10804-024-09478-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p>College life involves navigating different educational and career development options, as well as social relationships. The pressure to “fit in” can be stressful and involve the fear of missing out (FOMO) on fun, social events. The college environment is especially challenging for those who experience social anxiety – the fear of being rejected and humiliated in social encounters. One factor that could be related to social difficulties in college students is helicopter parenting – a distinct parenting style characterized by parental over-involvement and over-control, which has been shown to be harmful to college students’ development. Given that college students experience helicopter parenting, FOMO, and social anxiety simultaneously, we examined how these experiences were associated with one another, as well as their stability and change across time. We expected helicopter parenting to be related to college students’ FOMO and social anxiety. Using a 12-week longitudinal design with a sample of 282 college students, we employed an interrelated latent growth curve to determine the associations between changes in helicopter parenting, FOMO, and social anxiety. The results showed that the levels of FOMO decreased over time, and a slower decrease in helicopter parenting was associated with a slower decrease in FOMO. In addition, decreases in social anxiety were associated with decreases in helicopter parenting and FOMO. Implications for practice are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":51546,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Adult Development","volume":"18 5 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-03-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140047165","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social Connections and Future Horizons: Socioemotional Selectivity Among Midlife and Older Adults in Aotearoa/New Zealand During COVID-19 社会联系与未来视野:COVID-19 期间奥特亚罗瓦/新西兰中老年人的社会情感选择性
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-20 DOI: 10.1007/s10804-023-09470-7
Nicky J. Newton, Mary Breheny, Christine Stephens

The current study uses socioemotional selectivity theory (SST; Carstensen in Psychology and Aging. 7(3):331–338,1992,10.1037/0882-7974.7.3.331) as a framework to examine the experiences of older adults during COVID-19 in Aotearoa/New Zealand. SST posits that when time horizons are perceived as limited—such as with increasing age, or during times of medical uncertainty—people selectively focus on spending time with emotionally close others and in personally fulfilling activities. This may be the case particularly when uncertainty is experienced by older people. Aotearoa/New Zealand instigated an elimination strategy early in the pandemic (prior to widespread vaccine availability), employing a series of lockdowns that further challenged older adults’ perceptions of future horizons, their ability to maintain social connections with loved ones, and engagement in pleasurable activities. This study used thematic analysis to analyze existing qualitative COVID-19-related data drawn from the experiences of 510 older adults who participated in the 2021 Health, Work, and Retirement study (HWR). Participants ranged in age from 56 to 87 (Mage = 69.73); 15.9% identified as Māori, and 76.2% identified as female. Two broad themes were revealed: Social Connection and Anticipating the Future. Although most participants expressed comparatively more positive than negative experiences, some also expressed frustration and anxiety about health directive-related divisions in relationships, and the inability to share in important life events (such as weddings, births, and funerals). Findings highlight the complex impact of COVID-19 on older adults’ perceptions of time horizons, and the opportunities presented by the pandemic for reassessment of social relationships and activities.

本研究以社会情感选择性理论(SST;Carstensen in Psychology and Aging.7(3):331-338,1992,10.1037/0882-7974.7.3.331)为框架,研究奥特亚罗瓦/新西兰老年人在 COVID-19 期间的经历。SST 认为,当人们认为时间范围有限时--如随着年龄的增长或在医疗不确定时期--人们会选择性地把时间集中在与情感亲密的人在一起和从事个人满足的活动上。当老年人遇到不确定因素时,情况可能尤其如此。奥特亚罗瓦/新西兰在大流行病的早期(在疫苗普及之前)就采取了消灭策略,实施了一系列封锁措施,这进一步挑战了老年人对未来前景的看法、他们与亲人保持社会联系的能力以及参与快乐活动的能力。本研究采用主题分析法对现有的 COVID-19 相关定性数据进行分析,这些数据来自参与 2021 年健康、工作和退休研究(HWR)的 510 位老年人的经历。参与者的年龄从 56 岁到 87 岁不等(Mage = 69.73);15.9% 的参与者为毛利人,76.2% 的参与者为女性。研究揭示了两大主题:社会联系和对未来的预期。虽然大多数参与者表示积极的经历相对多于消极的经历,但也有一些人对健康指令相关的人际关系分化以及无法分享重要的生活事件(如婚礼、出生和葬礼)表示沮丧和焦虑。研究结果凸显了 COVID-19 对老年人时间观念的复杂影响,以及大流行病为重新评估社会关系和活动提供的机会。
{"title":"Social Connections and Future Horizons: Socioemotional Selectivity Among Midlife and Older Adults in Aotearoa/New Zealand During COVID-19","authors":"Nicky J. Newton, Mary Breheny, Christine Stephens","doi":"10.1007/s10804-023-09470-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10804-023-09470-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The current study uses socioemotional selectivity theory (SST; Carstensen in Psychology and Aging. 7(3):331–338,1992,10.1037/0882-7974.7.3.331) as a framework to examine the experiences of older adults during COVID-19 in Aotearoa/New Zealand. SST posits that when time horizons are perceived as limited—such as with increasing age, or during times of medical uncertainty—people selectively focus on spending time with emotionally close others and in personally fulfilling activities. This may be the case particularly when uncertainty is experienced by older people. Aotearoa/New Zealand instigated an elimination strategy early in the pandemic (prior to widespread vaccine availability), employing a series of lockdowns that further challenged older adults’ perceptions of future horizons, their ability to maintain social connections with loved ones, and engagement in pleasurable activities. This study used thematic analysis to analyze existing qualitative COVID-19-related data drawn from the experiences of 510 older adults who participated in the 2021 Health, Work, and Retirement study (HWR). Participants ranged in age from 56 to 87 (<i>M</i><sub><i>age</i></sub> = 69.73); 15.9% identified as Māori, and 76.2% identified as female. Two broad themes were revealed: Social Connection and Anticipating the Future. Although most participants expressed comparatively more positive than negative experiences, some also expressed frustration and anxiety about health directive-related divisions in relationships, and the inability to share in important life events (such as weddings, births, and funerals). Findings highlight the complex impact of COVID-19 on older adults’ perceptions of time horizons, and the opportunities presented by the pandemic for reassessment of social relationships and activities.</p>","PeriodicalId":51546,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Adult Development","volume":"29 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-01-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139506967","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive Fatigue in Young, Middle-Aged, and Older Adults: A Response Time Distribution Approach 青年、中年和老年人的认知疲劳:反应时间分布法
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-16 DOI: 10.1007/s10804-023-09471-6
Jessica Gilsoul, Vincent Libertiaux, Frédérique Depierreux, Fabienne Collette

Cognitive fatigue arises after a long-lasting task, as attested by increases in reaction times (RTs). However, most studies have focused on young adults. Therefore, we investigated cognitive fatigue through changes in RT distributions in three age groups—young, middle-aged, and older adults—during a 160-min Stroop task. Task duration was divided into four blocks and the ex-Gaussian parameters (μ, σ, τ) were extracted from individual RT distributions in each time block for each item type. The results showed a significant Group effect on μ. Young adults had smaller μ values than the other two groups, meaning that middle-aged and older people performed the whole task slower than young adults. By contrast, τ showed no Group effect but increased with Time-on-Task in middle-aged people. Older adults did not show τ increase with Time-on-Task, which echoes studies showing some resistance to task monotony in this population. Globally, our results showed dissociated age and Time-on-Task effect on the ex-Gaussian parameters, confirming the relevance of this approach in the cognitive fatigue domain. We proposed here that cognitive fatigue affects only the decision component of response production, and that midlife may be a life stage with high sensitivity to cognitive fatigue.

认知疲劳产生于长时间的任务之后,反应时间(RTs)的增加就证明了这一点。然而,大多数研究都集中在年轻人身上。因此,我们通过三个年龄组--青年、中年和老年人--在 160 分钟的 Stroop 任务中反应时间分布的变化来研究认知疲劳。任务持续时间被分为四个区块,从每个区块中每个项目类型的单个RT分布中提取前高斯参数(μ、σ、τ)。结果表明,μ具有明显的群体效应。青壮年的μ值小于其他两个群体,这意味着中老年人完成整个任务的速度比青壮年慢。相比之下,τ 在中年人中没有显示出群体效应,但随着任务时间的延长而增加。老年人的τ并没有随着任务时间的延长而增加,这与研究显示老年人对任务单调性有一定的抵抗力相呼应。总体而言,我们的研究结果表明,年龄和任务时间对前高斯参数的影响是不同的,这证实了这种方法在认知疲劳领域的相关性。我们在此提出,认知疲劳只影响反应产生的决策部分,中年可能是对认知疲劳高度敏感的人生阶段。
{"title":"Cognitive Fatigue in Young, Middle-Aged, and Older Adults: A Response Time Distribution Approach","authors":"Jessica Gilsoul, Vincent Libertiaux, Frédérique Depierreux, Fabienne Collette","doi":"10.1007/s10804-023-09471-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10804-023-09471-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Cognitive fatigue arises after a long-lasting task, as attested by increases in reaction times (RTs). However, most studies have focused on young adults. Therefore, we investigated cognitive fatigue through changes in RT distributions in three age groups—young, middle-aged, and older adults—during a 160-min Stroop task. Task duration was divided into four blocks and the ex-Gaussian parameters (μ, σ, τ) were extracted from individual RT distributions in each time block for each item type. The results showed a significant Group effect on μ. Young adults had smaller μ values than the other two groups, meaning that middle-aged and older people performed the whole task slower than young adults. By contrast, τ showed no Group effect but increased with Time-on-Task in middle-aged people. Older adults did not show τ increase with Time-on-Task, which echoes studies showing some resistance to task monotony in this population. Globally, our results showed dissociated age and Time-on-Task effect on the ex-Gaussian parameters, confirming the relevance of this approach in the cognitive fatigue domain. We proposed here that cognitive fatigue affects only the decision component of response production, and that midlife may be a life stage with high sensitivity to cognitive fatigue.</p>","PeriodicalId":51546,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Adult Development","volume":"255 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-01-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139499737","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Defining and Discussing Independence in Emerging Adult College Students 定义和讨论成年大学生的独立性
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-12 DOI: 10.1007/s10804-023-09472-5
Jenna R. LaFreniere

This research identified college students’ definitions of independence as well as topics they wish parents had discussed with them prior to college. Applying the theory of emerging adulthood to understand college students’ construal of independence, participants included 117 emerging adults, aged 18–25. Utilizing inductive coding to categorize participants’ open-ended responses from an online questionnaire, most emerging adults conceptualized independence in terms of self-reliance or freedom, and this was examined through the lens of social construction theory. The majority reported they felt their parents communicated what was necessary regarding independence with them before college. For those who did not, however, life skills and expectations were two of the topics students most wished parents would have communicated prior to college. Implications and suggestions are provided.

本研究确定了大学生对独立的定义,以及他们希望父母在上大学前与他们讨论的话题。本研究运用新兴成人理论来理解大学生对独立性的理解,参与者包括 117 名 18-25 岁的新兴成人。通过归纳编码法对在线问卷中参与者的开放式回答进行分类,大多数新兴成人将独立概念化为自立或自由,并通过社会建构理论的视角对此进行了研究。大多数人表示,他们认为父母在他们上大学之前就与他们沟通了有关独立的必要事项。然而,对于那些没有做到这一点的学生来说,生活技能和期望是他们最希望父母在上大学前与他们交流的两个话题。本报告提出了一些启示和建议。
{"title":"Defining and Discussing Independence in Emerging Adult College Students","authors":"Jenna R. LaFreniere","doi":"10.1007/s10804-023-09472-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10804-023-09472-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This research identified college students’ definitions of independence as well as topics they wish parents had discussed with them prior to college. Applying the theory of emerging adulthood to understand college students’ construal of independence, participants included 117 emerging adults, aged 18–25. Utilizing inductive coding to categorize participants’ open-ended responses from an online questionnaire, most emerging adults conceptualized independence in terms of self-reliance or freedom, and this was examined through the lens of social construction theory. The majority reported they felt their parents communicated what was necessary regarding independence with them before college. For those who did not, however, life skills and expectations were two of the topics students most wished parents would have communicated prior to college. Implications and suggestions are provided.</p>","PeriodicalId":51546,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Adult Development","volume":"2 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-01-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139459154","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Adult Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1