首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Pigeon's choice depends primarily on the value of the signal for the outcome rather than its frequency or contrast. 鸽子的选择主要取决于信号的价值,而不是它的频率或对比度。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000316
T. Zentall, Daniel N Peng, Peyton M. Mueller
Pigeons typically prefer a 20% probability of signaled reinforcement over a 50% probability of unsignaled reinforcement. There is even evidence that they prefer 50% signaled reinforcement over 100% reinforcement. It has been suggested that this effect results from contrast between the expected probability of reinforcement (e.g., 50%) at the time of choice and the value of the positive signal for reinforcement (100%). Alternatively, it is primarily the value of the positive signal for reinforcement itself that determines suboptimal choice. To attempt to distinguish between these two hypotheses, in Experiment 1, we gave pigeons a choice between (a) a 50% reinforcement alternative that was followed by one of two signals for 100% reinforcement, each 25% of the time, or a signal for the absence of reinforcement 50% of the time (50% contrast) and (b) a 25% reinforcement alternative that was followed by a signal for 100% reinforcement 25% of the time, or a signal for the absence of reinforcement 75% of the time (75% contrast). In spite of the difference in contrast, the pigeons were indifferent between the two alternatives. In Experiment 2, when contrast was held constant at 50% and the value of the positive signals for reinforcement were different, we found support for choice based on the value of the positive signal for reinforcement. Thus, it appears that pigeons' choice depends primarily on the value of the outcome rather than its frequency or contrast. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
鸽子通常更喜欢20%的有信号的强化,而不是50%的没有信号的强化。甚至有证据表明,他们更喜欢50%的信号强化而不是100%的信号强化。有人认为,这种效应是由选择时预期强化概率(例如,50%)与强化积极信号值(100%)之间的对比造成的。或者,主要是强化本身的积极信号的值决定了次优选择。试图区分这两个假设,在实验1中,我们给鸽子(a) 50%的钢筋替代之间的选择,其次是两个信号之一100%强化,每25%的时间,或一个信号缺乏强化的50%的时间(50%对比)和(b)钢筋替代25%,紧随其后的是一个信号强化100% 25%的时间,或一个信号缺乏强化75%的时间(75%对比)。尽管对比不同,但鸽子对这两种选择无动于衷。在实验2中,当对比度保持在50%不变,强化正信号的值不同时,我们发现基于强化正信号的值支持选择。因此,鸽子的选择似乎主要取决于结果的价值,而不是其频率或对比。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Pigeon's choice depends primarily on the value of the signal for the outcome rather than its frequency or contrast.","authors":"T. Zentall, Daniel N Peng, Peyton M. Mueller","doi":"10.1037/xan0000316","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000316","url":null,"abstract":"Pigeons typically prefer a 20% probability of signaled reinforcement over a 50% probability of unsignaled reinforcement. There is even evidence that they prefer 50% signaled reinforcement over 100% reinforcement. It has been suggested that this effect results from contrast between the expected probability of reinforcement (e.g., 50%) at the time of choice and the value of the positive signal for reinforcement (100%). Alternatively, it is primarily the value of the positive signal for reinforcement itself that determines suboptimal choice. To attempt to distinguish between these two hypotheses, in Experiment 1, we gave pigeons a choice between (a) a 50% reinforcement alternative that was followed by one of two signals for 100% reinforcement, each 25% of the time, or a signal for the absence of reinforcement 50% of the time (50% contrast) and (b) a 25% reinforcement alternative that was followed by a signal for 100% reinforcement 25% of the time, or a signal for the absence of reinforcement 75% of the time (75% contrast). In spite of the difference in contrast, the pigeons were indifferent between the two alternatives. In Experiment 2, when contrast was held constant at 50% and the value of the positive signals for reinforcement were different, we found support for choice based on the value of the positive signal for reinforcement. Thus, it appears that pigeons' choice depends primarily on the value of the outcome rather than its frequency or contrast. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":"36 2 1","pages":"135-144"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"82807273","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Supplemental Material for Hierarchical and Configural Control in Conditional Discrimination Learning 条件判别学习中的层次控制和结构控制补充材料
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000342.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Hierarchical and Configural Control in Conditional Discrimination Learning","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/xan0000342.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000342.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"57378239","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of stimulus pre-exposure and conditioning on overt visual attention. 刺激前暴露和条件反射对显性视觉注意的影响。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000313
James Byron Nelson, Anton Navarro, Paula Balea, Maria Del Carmen Sanjuan

Three experiments (a, b, c) combined to provide a well-powered examination of the effects of stimulus pre-exposure and conditioning on visual attention using an eye tracker and a space-shooter video game where a colored flashing light predicted an attacking spaceship. In each, group "control" received no pre-exposure to the light, group "same" received pre-exposure in the same context as conditioning, and group "different" received pre-exposure in a different context. Experiments differed in visual details regarding the game (1a vs. 1b and 1c) or minor details in the setup of the eye tracker (1a and 1b vs. 1c). Overall, pre-exposure retarded acquisition of keyboard responding. That effect was enhanced, rather than attenuated, by a context change. Separating participants by sign and goal trackers showed the context change enhanced the pre-exposure effect in goal trackers and reduced it in sign trackers. Visual attention to the light declined during pre-exposure and did not recover with either conditioning or a context switch. Visual attention to the light decreased during conditioning. Visual goal tracking toward where the spaceship would appear was also retarded with pre-exposure. Unlike the keyboard responding, a context change led to more normal goal-tracking acquisition. Results are discussed in terms of theories of attention and the potential effects of demand characteristics on the task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

三个实验(a, b, c)结合在一起,提供了一个强有力的测试,通过使用眼动仪和太空射击视频游戏,刺激预暴露和条件反射对视觉注意力的影响,其中彩色闪光预示着飞船的攻击。在每一组中,“对照组”没有接受光的预曝光,“相同组”在与条件反射相同的环境中接受预曝光,“不同组”在不同的环境中接受预曝光。实验在游戏的视觉细节(1a vs. 1b和1c)或眼动仪设置的次要细节(1a和1b vs. 1c)上有所不同。总体而言,预暴露延缓了键盘反应的习得。环境的改变增强了这种效应,而不是减弱了。结果表明,情境变化增强了目标跟踪器的前曝光效应,降低了手势跟踪器的前曝光效应。在预曝光期间,视觉对光的注意力下降,并且在条件反射或环境切换中都无法恢复。在调节过程中,对光的视觉注意力下降。视觉目标追踪到太空船将出现的地方也延迟了预曝光。与键盘反应不同,上下文变化导致了更正常的目标跟踪获取。研究结果从注意理论和需求特征对任务的潜在影响两方面进行了讨论。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"The effects of stimulus pre-exposure and conditioning on overt visual attention.","authors":"James Byron Nelson,&nbsp;Anton Navarro,&nbsp;Paula Balea,&nbsp;Maria Del Carmen Sanjuan","doi":"10.1037/xan0000313","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000313","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Three experiments (a, b, c) combined to provide a well-powered examination of the effects of stimulus pre-exposure and conditioning on visual attention using an eye tracker and a space-shooter video game where a colored flashing light predicted an attacking spaceship. In each, group \"control\" received no pre-exposure to the light, group \"same\" received pre-exposure in the same context as conditioning, and group \"different\" received pre-exposure in a different context. Experiments differed in visual details regarding the game (1a vs. 1b and 1c) or minor details in the setup of the eye tracker (1a and 1b vs. 1c). Overall, pre-exposure retarded acquisition of keyboard responding. That effect was enhanced, rather than attenuated, by a context change. Separating participants by sign and goal trackers showed the context change enhanced the pre-exposure effect in goal trackers and reduced it in sign trackers. Visual attention to the light declined during pre-exposure and did not recover with either conditioning or a context switch. Visual attention to the light decreased during conditioning. Visual goal tracking toward where the spaceship would appear was also retarded with pre-exposure. Unlike the keyboard responding, a context change led to more normal goal-tracking acquisition. Results are discussed in terms of theories of attention and the potential effects of demand characteristics on the task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":"48 1","pages":"29-45"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39906252","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Nonreactive testing: Evaluating the effect of withholding feedback in predictive learning. 非反应性测试:评估预扣反馈在预测学习中的效果。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-01-01 Epub Date: 2021-11-29 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000311
Jessica C Lee, Mike E Le Pelley, Peter F Lovibond

Learning of cue-outcome relationships in associative learning experiments is often assessed by presenting cues without feedback about the outcome and informing participants to expect no outcomes to occur. The rationale is that this "no-feedback" testing procedure prevents new learning during testing that might contaminate the later test trials. We tested this assumption in 4 predictive learning experiments where participants were tasked with learning which foods (cues) were causing allergic reactions (the outcome) in a fictitious patient. We found that withholding feedback in a block of trials had no effect on causal ratings (Experiments 1 and 2), but it led to regression toward intermediate ratings when the missing feedback was embedded in the causal scenario and information about the outcome replaced by a "?" (Experiment 3). A factorial experiment manipulating cover story and feedback revealed that the regression-to-baseline effect was primarily driven by presentation of the "?" feedback (Experiment 4). We conclude that the procedure of testing without feedback, used widely in studies of human cognition, is an appropriate way of assessing learning, as long as the missing data are attributed to the experimenter and the absence of feedback is not highlighted in a way that induces uncertainty. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

在联想学习实验中,线索-结果关系的学习通常是通过提供线索而不反馈结果和告知参与者期望没有结果来评估的。其基本原理是,这种“无反馈”测试过程可以防止在测试过程中学习新的知识,而这些新知识可能会污染后来的测试试验。我们在4个预测学习实验中测试了这一假设,在这些实验中,参与者被要求学习哪些食物(线索)会引起一个虚构的病人的过敏反应(结果)。我们发现,在一组试验中保留反馈对因果评级没有影响(实验1和2),但当缺失的反馈嵌入到因果情景中,有关结果的信息被“?”取代时,它会导致向中间评级的回归。(实验3)。一个操纵封面故事和反馈的析因实验表明,回归基线效应主要是由“?”反馈的呈现驱动的(实验4)。我们得出结论,在人类认知研究中广泛使用的无反馈测试程序是一种适当的评估学习的方法,只要缺失的数据归因于实验者,并且反馈的缺失不会以一种引起不确定性的方式突出。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Nonreactive testing: Evaluating the effect of withholding feedback in predictive learning.","authors":"Jessica C Lee,&nbsp;Mike E Le Pelley,&nbsp;Peter F Lovibond","doi":"10.1037/xan0000311","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000311","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Learning of cue-outcome relationships in associative learning experiments is often assessed by presenting cues without feedback about the outcome and informing participants to expect no outcomes to occur. The rationale is that this \"no-feedback\" testing procedure prevents new learning during testing that might contaminate the later test trials. We tested this assumption in 4 predictive learning experiments where participants were tasked with learning which foods (cues) were causing allergic reactions (the outcome) in a fictitious patient. We found that withholding feedback in a block of trials had no effect on causal ratings (Experiments 1 and 2), but it led to regression toward intermediate ratings when the missing feedback was embedded in the causal scenario and information about the outcome replaced by a \"?\" (Experiment 3). A factorial experiment manipulating cover story and feedback revealed that the regression-to-baseline effect was primarily driven by presentation of the \"?\" feedback (Experiment 4). We conclude that the procedure of testing without feedback, used widely in studies of human cognition, is an appropriate way of assessing learning, as long as the missing data are attributed to the experimenter and the absence of feedback is not highlighted in a way that induces uncertainty. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":"48 1","pages":"17-28"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39765153","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Use of different attentional strategies by pigeons and humans in multidimensional visual search. 鸽子和人类在多维视觉搜索中不同注意策略的使用。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-01-01 Epub Date: 2021-11-15 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000310
Suzanne L Gray, Muhammad A J Qadri, Daniel I Brooks, Robert G Cook

To study comparative attentional allocation strategies, pigeons and humans were tested using simultaneously available discrimination tasks. Given visual search displays containing 32 items from two orthogonal dimensions, participants were reinforced for selecting the eight brightest (or darkest) of 16 brightness items and the eight most vertical (or horizontal) of 16 orientation items. Consistent with a sequential dimensional strategy, humans preferentially chose items from one dimension before switching to the other to complete the search. In contrast, the pigeons did not preferentially stay within one dimension over consecutive choices. Instead, they chose the items most likely to yield reward based on item discriminability. Computational models that incorporated a "dimensional staying" factor accounted best for the human data, while simulations using only discriminability reproduced the pigeons' data. These results suggest that humans are sensitive to the benefits of attentional staying and the costs of switching between dimensional tasks, while there was no evidence that these factors influenced the pigeons' choice behavior. These findings suggest fundamental differences in how pigeons and humans allocate attention in complex choice situations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

为了研究比较注意分配策略,鸽子和人类使用同时可用的辨别任务进行了测试。给定视觉搜索显示,包含来自两个正交维度的32个项目,参与者被要求从16个亮度项目中选择8个最亮(或最暗)的项目,从16个方向项目中选择8个最垂直(或水平)的项目。与顺序维度策略一致,人类优先从一个维度选择项目,然后切换到另一个维度来完成搜索。相比之下,鸽子在连续的选择中不会优先停留在一个维度上。相反,他们会根据物品的可辨别性选择最有可能产生奖励的物品。包含“维度停留”因素的计算模型最能解释人类的数据,而只使用可判别性的模拟再现了鸽子的数据。这些结果表明,人类对注意力停留的好处和在不同维度任务之间切换的代价很敏感,而没有证据表明这些因素影响了鸽子的选择行为。这些发现表明,鸽子和人类在复杂选择情况下分配注意力的方式存在根本差异。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Use of different attentional strategies by pigeons and humans in multidimensional visual search.","authors":"Suzanne L Gray,&nbsp;Muhammad A J Qadri,&nbsp;Daniel I Brooks,&nbsp;Robert G Cook","doi":"10.1037/xan0000310","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000310","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To study comparative attentional allocation strategies, pigeons and humans were tested using simultaneously available discrimination tasks. Given visual search displays containing 32 items from two orthogonal dimensions, participants were reinforced for selecting the eight brightest (or darkest) of 16 brightness items and the eight most vertical (or horizontal) of 16 orientation items. Consistent with a sequential dimensional strategy, humans preferentially chose items from one dimension before switching to the other to complete the search. In contrast, the pigeons did not preferentially stay within one dimension over consecutive choices. Instead, they chose the items most likely to yield reward based on item discriminability. Computational models that incorporated a \"dimensional staying\" factor accounted best for the human data, while simulations using only discriminability reproduced the pigeons' data. These results suggest that humans are sensitive to the benefits of attentional staying and the costs of switching between dimensional tasks, while there was no evidence that these factors influenced the pigeons' choice behavior. These findings suggest fundamental differences in how pigeons and humans allocate attention in complex choice situations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":"48 1","pages":"46-59"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39625624","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Supplemental Material for The Effects of Stimulus Pre-Exposure and Conditioning on Overt Visual Attention 刺激前暴露和条件反射对显性视觉注意影响的补充材料
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000313.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for The Effects of Stimulus Pre-Exposure and Conditioning on Overt Visual Attention","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/xan0000313.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000313.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":"16 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"57378168","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Elements of a compound elicit little conditioned reinforcement. 化合物的元素很少引起条件强化。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-01-01 Epub Date: 2021-11-29 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000296
Valeria V González, Benjamin M Seitz, Rachel Formaker, Aaron P Blaisdell

The acquisition of instrumental responding can be supported by primary reinforcers or by conditional (also known as secondary) reinforcers that themselves have an association to a primary reinforcer. While primary reinforcement has been heavily studied for the past century, the associative basis of conditioned reinforcement has received comparatively little experimental examination. Yet conditioned reinforcement has been employed as an important behavioral assay in neuroscience studies, and thus an analysis of its associative basis is called for. We evaluated the extent to which an element from a previously trained compound would facilitate conditioned reinforcement. Three groups of rats received Pavlovian conditioning with a visual-auditory compound cue followed by food. After training, a lever was made available that, when pressed, produced the same trained compound (group compound), only the auditory cue (group element), or a novel auditory cue (group control). The rats in group compound pressed the lever at a higher rate than did rats in either group element or group control, demonstrating a strong conditioned reinforcement effect only in group compound. Interestingly, there was almost no difference in responding between group element and group control. The implications of this generalization decrement in conditioned reinforcement are discussed-particularly as they relate to research in behavioral neuroscience. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

工具性反应的习得可以由初级强化物或条件强化物(也称为次级强化物)支持,这些强化物本身与初级强化物有关联。虽然初级强化在过去的一个世纪里得到了大量的研究,但条件强化的联想基础却得到了相对较少的实验检验。然而,在神经科学研究中,条件强化已被用作一种重要的行为分析,因此需要对其联想基础进行分析。我们评估了从先前训练过的化合物中提取的元素促进条件强化的程度。三组大鼠接受了巴甫洛夫条件反射,先是视觉-听觉复合提示,然后是食物。训练后,有一个杠杆,当按下时,产生相同的训练化合物(组化合物),只有听觉线索(组元素),或一个新的听觉线索(组控制)。实验组大鼠按杠杆的频率高于对照组大鼠,只有实验组有较强的条件强化效应。有趣的是,实验组和对照组在反应上几乎没有差异。讨论了条件强化中这种泛化衰减的含义,特别是当它们与行为神经科学的研究相关时。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Elements of a compound elicit little conditioned reinforcement.","authors":"Valeria V González,&nbsp;Benjamin M Seitz,&nbsp;Rachel Formaker,&nbsp;Aaron P Blaisdell","doi":"10.1037/xan0000296","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000296","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The acquisition of instrumental responding can be supported by primary reinforcers or by conditional (also known as secondary) reinforcers that themselves have an association to a primary reinforcer. While primary reinforcement has been heavily studied for the past century, the associative basis of conditioned reinforcement has received comparatively little experimental examination. Yet conditioned reinforcement has been employed as an important behavioral assay in neuroscience studies, and thus an analysis of its associative basis is called for. We evaluated the extent to which an element from a previously trained compound would facilitate conditioned reinforcement. Three groups of rats received Pavlovian conditioning with a visual-auditory compound cue followed by food. After training, a lever was made available that, when pressed, produced the same trained compound (group compound), only the auditory cue (group element), or a novel auditory cue (group control). The rats in group compound pressed the lever at a higher rate than did rats in either group element or group control, demonstrating a strong conditioned reinforcement effect only in group compound. Interestingly, there was almost no difference in responding between group element and group control. The implications of this generalization decrement in conditioned reinforcement are discussed-particularly as they relate to research in behavioral neuroscience. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":"48 1","pages":"67-72"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39765152","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Valence Generalization Across Nonrecurring Structures 非重复结构的价泛化补充材料
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000317.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Valence Generalization Across Nonrecurring Structures","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/xan0000317.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000317.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"57378223","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Generalization Following Symmetrical Intradimensional Discrimination Training 对称内维辨别训练后泛化的补充材料
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000327.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Generalization Following Symmetrical Intradimensional Discrimination Training","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/xan0000327.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000327.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"57378233","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Theory protection: Do humans protect existing associative links? 理论保护:人类是否会保护已有的联想链接?
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000314
Stuart G Spicer, Chris J Mitchell, Andy J Wills, Katie L Blake, Peter M Jones

Theories of associative learning often propose that learning is proportional to prediction error, or the difference between expected events and those that occur. Spicer et al. (2020) suggested an alternative, that humans might instead selectively attribute surprising outcomes to cues that they are not confident about, to maintain cue-outcome associations about which they are more confident. Spicer et al. reported three predictive learning experiments, the results of which were consistent with their proposal ("theory protection") rather than a prediction error account (Rescorla, 2001). The four experiments reported here further test theory protection against a prediction error account. Experiments 3 and 4 also test the proposals of Holmes et al. (2019), who suggested a function mapping learning to performance that can explain Spicer et al.'s results using a prediction-error framework. In contrast to the previous study, these experiments were based on inhibition rather than excitation. Participants were trained with a set of cues (represented by letters), each of which was followed by the presence or absence of an outcome (represented by + or -). Following this, a cue that previously caused the outcome (A+) was placed in compound with another cue (B) with an ambiguous causal status (e.g., a novel cue in Experiment 1). This compound (AB-) did not cause the outcome. Participants always learned more about B in the second training phase, despite A always having the greater prediction error. In Experiments 3 and 4, a cue with no apparent prediction error was learned about more than a cue with a large prediction error. Experiment 4 tested participants' relative confidence about the causal status of cues A and B prior to the AB- stage, producing findings that are consistent with theory protection and inconsistent with the predictions of Rescorla, and Holmes et al. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

联想学习理论经常提出,学习与预测误差或预期事件与实际发生事件之间的差异成正比。Spicer等人(2020)提出了另一种选择,即人类可能会选择性地将令人惊讶的结果归因于他们不自信的线索,以维持他们更自信的线索-结果关联。Spicer等人报告了三个预测学习实验,其结果与他们的提议(“理论保护”)一致,而不是预测误差解释(Rescorla, 2001)。这里报告的四个实验进一步验证了理论对预测误差的保护。实验3和4还测试了Holmes等人(2019)的建议,他们提出了一个将学习映射到性能的函数,可以使用预测误差框架解释Spicer等人的结果。与之前的研究相反,这些实验是基于抑制而不是兴奋。参与者接受了一组提示(用字母表示)的训练,每个提示后面都有一个结果的存在或不存在(用+或-表示)。在此之后,将先前导致结果的线索(a +)与另一个因果状态模糊的线索(B)(例如,实验1中的新线索)组合在一起。该化合物(AB-)不会导致结果。在第二个训练阶段,尽管A的预测误差总是更大,但参与者对B的了解总是更多。在实验3和实验4中,没有明显预测误差的线索比预测误差较大的线索学习得更多。实验4测试了参与者在AB-阶段之前对线索A和B的因果状态的相对信心,产生的结果与理论保护一致,与Rescorla和Holmes等人的预测不一致(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,所有权利保留)。
{"title":"Theory protection: Do humans protect existing associative links?","authors":"Stuart G Spicer,&nbsp;Chris J Mitchell,&nbsp;Andy J Wills,&nbsp;Katie L Blake,&nbsp;Peter M Jones","doi":"10.1037/xan0000314","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000314","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Theories of associative learning often propose that learning is proportional to prediction error, or the difference between expected events and those that occur. Spicer et al. (2020) suggested an alternative, that humans might instead selectively attribute surprising outcomes to cues that they are not confident about, to maintain cue-outcome associations about which they are more confident. Spicer et al. reported three predictive learning experiments, the results of which were consistent with their proposal (\"theory protection\") rather than a prediction error account (Rescorla, 2001). The four experiments reported here further test theory protection against a prediction error account. Experiments 3 and 4 also test the proposals of Holmes et al. (2019), who suggested a function mapping learning to performance that can explain Spicer et al.'s results using a prediction-error framework. In contrast to the previous study, these experiments were based on inhibition rather than excitation. Participants were trained with a set of cues (represented by letters), each of which was followed by the presence or absence of an outcome (represented by + or -). Following this, a cue that previously caused the outcome (A+) was placed in compound with another cue (B) with an ambiguous causal status (e.g., a novel cue in Experiment 1). This compound (AB-) did not cause the outcome. Participants always learned more about B in the second training phase, despite A always having the greater prediction error. In Experiments 3 and 4, a cue with no apparent prediction error was learned about more than a cue with a large prediction error. Experiment 4 tested participants' relative confidence about the causal status of cues A and B prior to the AB- stage, producing findings that are consistent with theory protection and inconsistent with the predictions of Rescorla, and Holmes et al. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":"48 1","pages":"1-16"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39906251","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1