首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Associative change in Pavlovian conditioning: A reappraisal. 巴甫洛夫条件反射中的联想变化:再评价。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-05-12 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000318
R. C. Honey, Dominic Michael Dwyer, Adela F. Iliescu
Robert A. Rescorla changed how Pavlovian conditioning was studied and interpreted. His empirical contributions were fundamental and theoretically driven. One involved testing a central tenet of the model that he developed with Allan R. Wagner. The Rescorla-Wagner learning rule uses a pooled error term to determine changes in a directional association between the representations of the conditioned stimulus (CS) and unconditioned stimulus (US). This learning rule predicts that 2 equally salient CSs (A and B) will undergo equivalent associative change when they are conditioned in compound (i.e., AB→US). Rescorla's results suggested that this was not the case (e.g., Rescorla, 2000). Here, we show that these results can be reconciled with a model that uses a learning rule with a pooled error term once that rule is applied equivalently to all of the stimuli presented on a given trial, and the resulting reciprocal associations (directly and indirectly) contribute to performance. This model, called HeiDI, integrates several features of Rescorla's research and theorizing while addressing an issue that he recognized required further analysis: how learning is translated into performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
Robert A. Rescorla改变了巴甫洛夫条件反射的研究和解释方式。他的经验性贡献是基础性和理论性的。其中一项涉及测试他与艾伦·r·瓦格纳(Allan R. Wagner)共同开发的模型的核心原则。Rescorla-Wagner学习规则使用汇总误差项来确定条件刺激(CS)和非条件刺激(US)表征之间方向关联的变化。该学习规则预测,当两个同样显著的CSs (A和B)在复合条件下(即AB→US),它们将经历等效的联想变化。Rescorla的研究结果表明,情况并非如此(例如,Rescorla, 2000)。在这里,我们表明,这些结果可以与一个模型相协调,该模型使用带有汇总误差项的学习规则,一旦该规则等效地应用于给定试验中呈现的所有刺激,并且由此产生的相互关联(直接和间接)有助于表现。这个模型被称为HeiDI,它整合了Rescorla研究的几个特点和理论,同时解决了一个他认为需要进一步分析的问题:学习如何转化为表现。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Associative change in Pavlovian conditioning: A reappraisal.","authors":"R. C. Honey, Dominic Michael Dwyer, Adela F. Iliescu","doi":"10.1037/xan0000318","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000318","url":null,"abstract":"Robert A. Rescorla changed how Pavlovian conditioning was studied and interpreted. His empirical contributions were fundamental and theoretically driven. One involved testing a central tenet of the model that he developed with Allan R. Wagner. The Rescorla-Wagner learning rule uses a pooled error term to determine changes in a directional association between the representations of the conditioned stimulus (CS) and unconditioned stimulus (US). This learning rule predicts that 2 equally salient CSs (A and B) will undergo equivalent associative change when they are conditioned in compound (i.e., AB→US). Rescorla's results suggested that this was not the case (e.g., Rescorla, 2000). Here, we show that these results can be reconciled with a model that uses a learning rule with a pooled error term once that rule is applied equivalently to all of the stimuli presented on a given trial, and the resulting reciprocal associations (directly and indirectly) contribute to performance. This model, called HeiDI, integrates several features of Rescorla's research and theorizing while addressing an issue that he recognized required further analysis: how learning is translated into performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-05-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87693456","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Inhibitory summation as a form of generalization. 抑制性总结作为概括的一种形式。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000320
Julie Y. L. Chow, Jessica C. Lee, P. Lovibond
Inhibitory learning after feature negative training (A+/AB-) is typically measured by combining the Feature B with a separately trained excitor (e.g., C) in a summation test. Reduced responding to C is taken as evidence that B has properties directly opposite to those of C. However, in human causal learning, transfer of B's inhibitory properties to another excitor is modest and depends on individual differences in inferred causal structure. Here we ask whether instead of opposing processes, a summation test might instead be thought of in terms of generalization. Using an allergist task, we tested whether inhibitory transfer would be influenced by similarity. We found that transfer was greater when the test stimuli were from the same semantic category as the training stimuli (Experiments 1 and 2) and when the test excitor had previously been associated with the same outcome (Experiment 3). We also found that the similarity effect applied across all self-reported causal structures. We conclude it may be more helpful to consider transfer of inhibition as a form of conceptual generalization rather than the arithmetic summation of opposing processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
特征负训练(A+/AB-)后的抑制学习通常通过在求和测试中将特征B与单独训练的激励器(例如,C)结合来测量。对C的反应减少被视为B具有与C相反的特性的证据。然而,在人类因果学习中,B的抑制特性向另一个兴奋物的转移是适度的,并且取决于推断因果结构的个体差异。这里我们要问的是,求和检验是否可以从泛化的角度来考虑,而不是相反的过程。使用过敏症专家任务,我们测试了抑制转移是否会受到相似性的影响。我们发现,当测试刺激来自与训练刺激相同的语义类别时(实验1和2),以及当测试刺激先前与相同的结果相关联时(实验3),迁移更大。我们还发现,相似性效应适用于所有自我报告的因果结构。我们的结论是,将抑制转移视为一种概念泛化形式而不是对立过程的算术求和可能更有帮助。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Inhibitory summation as a form of generalization.","authors":"Julie Y. L. Chow, Jessica C. Lee, P. Lovibond","doi":"10.1037/xan0000320","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000320","url":null,"abstract":"Inhibitory learning after feature negative training (A+/AB-) is typically measured by combining the Feature B with a separately trained excitor (e.g., C) in a summation test. Reduced responding to C is taken as evidence that B has properties directly opposite to those of C. However, in human causal learning, transfer of B's inhibitory properties to another excitor is modest and depends on individual differences in inferred causal structure. Here we ask whether instead of opposing processes, a summation test might instead be thought of in terms of generalization. Using an allergist task, we tested whether inhibitory transfer would be influenced by similarity. We found that transfer was greater when the test stimuli were from the same semantic category as the training stimuli (Experiments 1 and 2) and when the test excitor had previously been associated with the same outcome (Experiment 3). We also found that the similarity effect applied across all self-reported causal structures. We conclude it may be more helpful to consider transfer of inhibition as a form of conceptual generalization rather than the arithmetic summation of opposing processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"82693033","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Blocking is not 'pure' cue competition: Renewal-like effects in forward and backward blocking indicate contributions by associative cue interference. 阻塞不是“纯粹的”线索竞争:向前和向后阻塞的更新效应表明联想线索干扰的贡献。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000315
Gonzalo Miguez, Ralph R Miller

Blocking (i.e., reduced responding to cue X following YX-outcome pairings in Phase 2 as a consequence of cue Y having been paired with the outcome in Phase 1) is one of the signature phenomena in Pavlovian conditioning. Its discovery promoted the development of multiple associative models, most of which viewed blocking as an instance of pure cue competition (i.e., a decrease in responding attributable to training two conditioned stimuli in compound). Two experiments are reported in which rats were examined in a fear conditioning paradigm (i.e., lick suppression), and context dependency of retrieval at test was used as an index of associative cue interference (i.e., a decrease in responding to a target cue as a result of training a second cue with the same outcome but without concurrent presentation of the two cues). Specifically, we observed renewal of forward-blocking which parallels renewal of proactive interference, and renewal of backward-blocking which parallels renewal of retroactive interference. Thus, both backward-blocking (Experiment 1, embedded in a sensory preconditioning design) and forward-blocking (Experiment 2, conducted in first-order conditioning) appear to be influenced by retroactive and proactive interference, respectively, as well as cue competition. Consequently, blocking, long regarded as a benchmark example of pure cue competition, is sometimes a hybrid of cue competition and associative interference. Finally, the Discussion considers whether stimulus competition and associative interference are two independent phenomena or products of a single underlying process. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

阻滞(即,由于线索Y与第一阶段的结果配对,在第二阶段对Y -结果配对后对线索X的反应减少)是巴甫洛夫条件反射的标志性现象之一。它的发现促进了多种联想模型的发展,其中大多数将阻滞视为纯粹线索竞争的一个例子(即,由于复合训练两个条件刺激而导致反应减少)。本文报道了两个实验,其中在恐惧条件反射范式(即舔抑制)中对大鼠进行了检查,并使用测试时检索的上下文依赖性作为联想线索干扰的指标(即,由于训练具有相同结果但没有同时呈现两个线索的第二个线索而导致对目标线索的反应减少)。具体来说,我们观察到向前阻断的更新与主动干扰的更新平行,向后阻断的更新与追溯干扰的更新平行。因此,后向阻断(实验1,嵌入感官预处理设计)和前向阻断(实验2,在一阶条件反射中进行)似乎分别受到回溯干扰和主动干扰以及线索竞争的影响。因此,长期以来被视为纯粹球杆竞争的基准例子的阻塞,有时是球杆竞争和联想干扰的混合。最后,讨论考虑刺激竞争和联想干扰是两个独立的现象还是一个单一潜在过程的产物。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Blocking is not 'pure' cue competition: Renewal-like effects in forward and backward blocking indicate contributions by associative cue interference.","authors":"Gonzalo Miguez,&nbsp;Ralph R Miller","doi":"10.1037/xan0000315","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000315","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Blocking (i.e., reduced responding to cue X following YX-outcome pairings in Phase 2 as a consequence of cue Y having been paired with the outcome in Phase 1) is one of the signature phenomena in Pavlovian conditioning. Its discovery promoted the development of multiple associative models, most of which viewed blocking as an instance of pure cue competition (i.e., a decrease in responding attributable to training two conditioned stimuli in compound). Two experiments are reported in which rats were examined in a fear conditioning paradigm (i.e., lick suppression), and context dependency of retrieval at test was used as an index of associative cue interference (i.e., a decrease in responding to a target cue as a result of training a second cue with the same outcome but without concurrent presentation of the two cues). Specifically, we observed renewal of forward-blocking which parallels renewal of proactive interference, and renewal of backward-blocking which parallels renewal of retroactive interference. Thus, both backward-blocking (Experiment 1, embedded in a sensory preconditioning design) and forward-blocking (Experiment 2, conducted in first-order conditioning) appear to be influenced by retroactive and proactive interference, respectively, as well as cue competition. Consequently, blocking, long regarded as a benchmark example of pure cue competition, is sometimes a hybrid of cue competition and associative interference. Finally, the Discussion considers whether stimulus competition and associative interference are two independent phenomena or products of a single underlying process. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10259191/pdf/nihms-1899385.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9970546","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) perceive the Müller-Lyer illusion.
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000321
Sota Watanabe
A Müller-Lyer figure consists only of a line and arrowheads located at both ends of the line. Many comparative studies have reported that animals perceive Müller-Lyer illusion as humans, but few have used appropriate experimental designs to verify whether animal subjects actually respond to line length alone. The present study investigated whether budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) can perceive the Müller-Lyer illusion by using a method that addresses this problem. Four budgerigars were trained to select a long or short line (counterbalanced across subjects) from two horizontal lines. Next, the same task was conducted using two lines, one of which was situated between arrowheads pointing either right (>>) or left (<<). In the final training phase, the arrowheads were replaced with those pointing inward (><) or outward (<>). The performance of each subject toward each stimulus set of these trainings suggested that they did not determine the length of the line by including the arrowheads. In the test phase, response tendencies to the four figures were compared. Results suggested that budgerigars perceive the Müller-Lyer illusion in the same direction as humans; however, its magnitude is larger than that of humans. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
勒-莱尔图仅由一条线和位于线两端的箭头组成。许多比较研究已经报道了动物像人类一样感知勒-莱尔错觉,但很少有适当的实验设计来验证动物受试者是否真的对线长有反应。本研究利用一种解决这一问题的方法,研究了虎牙(Melopsittacus波动)是否能感知勒-莱尔错觉。四只虎皮鹦鹉被训练从两条水平线中选择一条长线或一条短线(在受试者之间平衡)。接下来,使用两条线执行相同的任务,其中一条线位于指向右(>>)或向左()的箭头之间。每个被试对这些训练的每个刺激集的表现表明,他们并没有通过包括箭头来确定线的长度。在测试阶段,比较了四个数字的反应趋势。结果表明,虎牙感知勒-莱尔错觉的方向与人类相同;然而,它的规模比人类更大。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) perceive the Müller-Lyer illusion.","authors":"Sota Watanabe","doi":"10.1037/xan0000321","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000321","url":null,"abstract":"A Müller-Lyer figure consists only of a line and arrowheads located at both ends of the line. Many comparative studies have reported that animals perceive Müller-Lyer illusion as humans, but few have used appropriate experimental designs to verify whether animal subjects actually respond to line length alone. The present study investigated whether budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) can perceive the Müller-Lyer illusion by using a method that addresses this problem. Four budgerigars were trained to select a long or short line (counterbalanced across subjects) from two horizontal lines. Next, the same task was conducted using two lines, one of which was situated between arrowheads pointing either right (>>) or left (<<). In the final training phase, the arrowheads were replaced with those pointing inward (><) or outward (<>). The performance of each subject toward each stimulus set of these trainings suggested that they did not determine the length of the line by including the arrowheads. In the test phase, response tendencies to the four figures were compared. Results suggested that budgerigars perceive the Müller-Lyer illusion in the same direction as humans; however, its magnitude is larger than that of humans. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"85984510","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pavlovian summation: Data and theory. 巴甫洛夫总结:数据和理论。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000265
S. Ghirlanda
In summation experiments, responding to a compound stimulus is assessed after conditioning a response to each of its components. This simple experiment poses significant challenges to models of associative learning because of substantial variability in results. Here, I introduce a new method to quantify generalization from components to compound in summation experiments, which I apply to over 250 measurements of summation in rabbits, pigeons, rats, and humans. The analysis confirms that more summation occurs with stimuli from different rather than from the same sensory modality, although this is not the sole determinant of summation. A theoretical analysis shows that this finding is best accounted for by a model that includes both element sharing (Rescorla & Wagner, 1972) and element replacement (Brandon et al., 2000) in stimulus representations. I point out remaining gaps in our empirical and theoretical understanding of summation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
在综合实验中,对复合刺激的反应是在对其每个组成部分的反应进行条件反射后评估的。这个简单的实验对联想学习模型提出了重大挑战,因为结果存在很大的可变性。在这里,我介绍了一种在求和实验中量化从组分到化合物的泛化的新方法,我将其应用于兔子,鸽子,老鼠和人类的250多个求和测量。分析证实,来自不同感官模态的刺激比来自相同感官模态的刺激产生更多的求和,尽管这不是求和的唯一决定因素。理论分析表明,刺激表征中包含元素共享(Rescorla & Wagner, 1972)和元素替换(Brandon et al., 2000)的模型最能解释这一发现。我指出了我们对求和的经验和理论理解中仍然存在的差距。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Pavlovian summation: Data and theory.","authors":"S. Ghirlanda","doi":"10.1037/xan0000265","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000265","url":null,"abstract":"In summation experiments, responding to a compound stimulus is assessed after conditioning a response to each of its components. This simple experiment poses significant challenges to models of associative learning because of substantial variability in results. Here, I introduce a new method to quantify generalization from components to compound in summation experiments, which I apply to over 250 measurements of summation in rabbits, pigeons, rats, and humans. The analysis confirms that more summation occurs with stimuli from different rather than from the same sensory modality, although this is not the sole determinant of summation. A theoretical analysis shows that this finding is best accounted for by a model that includes both element sharing (Rescorla & Wagner, 1972) and element replacement (Brandon et al., 2000) in stimulus representations. I point out remaining gaps in our empirical and theoretical understanding of summation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"85731647","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pigeon's choice depends primarily on the value of the signal for the outcome rather than its frequency or contrast. 鸽子的选择主要取决于信号的价值,而不是它的频率或对比度。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000316
T. Zentall, Daniel N Peng, Peyton M. Mueller
Pigeons typically prefer a 20% probability of signaled reinforcement over a 50% probability of unsignaled reinforcement. There is even evidence that they prefer 50% signaled reinforcement over 100% reinforcement. It has been suggested that this effect results from contrast between the expected probability of reinforcement (e.g., 50%) at the time of choice and the value of the positive signal for reinforcement (100%). Alternatively, it is primarily the value of the positive signal for reinforcement itself that determines suboptimal choice. To attempt to distinguish between these two hypotheses, in Experiment 1, we gave pigeons a choice between (a) a 50% reinforcement alternative that was followed by one of two signals for 100% reinforcement, each 25% of the time, or a signal for the absence of reinforcement 50% of the time (50% contrast) and (b) a 25% reinforcement alternative that was followed by a signal for 100% reinforcement 25% of the time, or a signal for the absence of reinforcement 75% of the time (75% contrast). In spite of the difference in contrast, the pigeons were indifferent between the two alternatives. In Experiment 2, when contrast was held constant at 50% and the value of the positive signals for reinforcement were different, we found support for choice based on the value of the positive signal for reinforcement. Thus, it appears that pigeons' choice depends primarily on the value of the outcome rather than its frequency or contrast. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
鸽子通常更喜欢20%的有信号的强化,而不是50%的没有信号的强化。甚至有证据表明,他们更喜欢50%的信号强化而不是100%的信号强化。有人认为,这种效应是由选择时预期强化概率(例如,50%)与强化积极信号值(100%)之间的对比造成的。或者,主要是强化本身的积极信号的值决定了次优选择。试图区分这两个假设,在实验1中,我们给鸽子(a) 50%的钢筋替代之间的选择,其次是两个信号之一100%强化,每25%的时间,或一个信号缺乏强化的50%的时间(50%对比)和(b)钢筋替代25%,紧随其后的是一个信号强化100% 25%的时间,或一个信号缺乏强化75%的时间(75%对比)。尽管对比不同,但鸽子对这两种选择无动于衷。在实验2中,当对比度保持在50%不变,强化正信号的值不同时,我们发现基于强化正信号的值支持选择。因此,鸽子的选择似乎主要取决于结果的价值,而不是其频率或对比。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Pigeon's choice depends primarily on the value of the signal for the outcome rather than its frequency or contrast.","authors":"T. Zentall, Daniel N Peng, Peyton M. Mueller","doi":"10.1037/xan0000316","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000316","url":null,"abstract":"Pigeons typically prefer a 20% probability of signaled reinforcement over a 50% probability of unsignaled reinforcement. There is even evidence that they prefer 50% signaled reinforcement over 100% reinforcement. It has been suggested that this effect results from contrast between the expected probability of reinforcement (e.g., 50%) at the time of choice and the value of the positive signal for reinforcement (100%). Alternatively, it is primarily the value of the positive signal for reinforcement itself that determines suboptimal choice. To attempt to distinguish between these two hypotheses, in Experiment 1, we gave pigeons a choice between (a) a 50% reinforcement alternative that was followed by one of two signals for 100% reinforcement, each 25% of the time, or a signal for the absence of reinforcement 50% of the time (50% contrast) and (b) a 25% reinforcement alternative that was followed by a signal for 100% reinforcement 25% of the time, or a signal for the absence of reinforcement 75% of the time (75% contrast). In spite of the difference in contrast, the pigeons were indifferent between the two alternatives. In Experiment 2, when contrast was held constant at 50% and the value of the positive signals for reinforcement were different, we found support for choice based on the value of the positive signal for reinforcement. Thus, it appears that pigeons' choice depends primarily on the value of the outcome rather than its frequency or contrast. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"82807273","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Supplemental Material for Hierarchical and Configural Control in Conditional Discrimination Learning 条件判别学习中的层次控制和结构控制补充材料
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000342.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Hierarchical and Configural Control in Conditional Discrimination Learning","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/xan0000342.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000342.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"57378239","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of stimulus pre-exposure and conditioning on overt visual attention. 刺激前暴露和条件反射对显性视觉注意的影响。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000313
James Byron Nelson, Anton Navarro, Paula Balea, Maria Del Carmen Sanjuan

Three experiments (a, b, c) combined to provide a well-powered examination of the effects of stimulus pre-exposure and conditioning on visual attention using an eye tracker and a space-shooter video game where a colored flashing light predicted an attacking spaceship. In each, group "control" received no pre-exposure to the light, group "same" received pre-exposure in the same context as conditioning, and group "different" received pre-exposure in a different context. Experiments differed in visual details regarding the game (1a vs. 1b and 1c) or minor details in the setup of the eye tracker (1a and 1b vs. 1c). Overall, pre-exposure retarded acquisition of keyboard responding. That effect was enhanced, rather than attenuated, by a context change. Separating participants by sign and goal trackers showed the context change enhanced the pre-exposure effect in goal trackers and reduced it in sign trackers. Visual attention to the light declined during pre-exposure and did not recover with either conditioning or a context switch. Visual attention to the light decreased during conditioning. Visual goal tracking toward where the spaceship would appear was also retarded with pre-exposure. Unlike the keyboard responding, a context change led to more normal goal-tracking acquisition. Results are discussed in terms of theories of attention and the potential effects of demand characteristics on the task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

三个实验(a, b, c)结合在一起,提供了一个强有力的测试,通过使用眼动仪和太空射击视频游戏,刺激预暴露和条件反射对视觉注意力的影响,其中彩色闪光预示着飞船的攻击。在每一组中,“对照组”没有接受光的预曝光,“相同组”在与条件反射相同的环境中接受预曝光,“不同组”在不同的环境中接受预曝光。实验在游戏的视觉细节(1a vs. 1b和1c)或眼动仪设置的次要细节(1a和1b vs. 1c)上有所不同。总体而言,预暴露延缓了键盘反应的习得。环境的改变增强了这种效应,而不是减弱了。结果表明,情境变化增强了目标跟踪器的前曝光效应,降低了手势跟踪器的前曝光效应。在预曝光期间,视觉对光的注意力下降,并且在条件反射或环境切换中都无法恢复。在调节过程中,对光的视觉注意力下降。视觉目标追踪到太空船将出现的地方也延迟了预曝光。与键盘反应不同,上下文变化导致了更正常的目标跟踪获取。研究结果从注意理论和需求特征对任务的潜在影响两方面进行了讨论。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"The effects of stimulus pre-exposure and conditioning on overt visual attention.","authors":"James Byron Nelson,&nbsp;Anton Navarro,&nbsp;Paula Balea,&nbsp;Maria Del Carmen Sanjuan","doi":"10.1037/xan0000313","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000313","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Three experiments (a, b, c) combined to provide a well-powered examination of the effects of stimulus pre-exposure and conditioning on visual attention using an eye tracker and a space-shooter video game where a colored flashing light predicted an attacking spaceship. In each, group \"control\" received no pre-exposure to the light, group \"same\" received pre-exposure in the same context as conditioning, and group \"different\" received pre-exposure in a different context. Experiments differed in visual details regarding the game (1a vs. 1b and 1c) or minor details in the setup of the eye tracker (1a and 1b vs. 1c). Overall, pre-exposure retarded acquisition of keyboard responding. That effect was enhanced, rather than attenuated, by a context change. Separating participants by sign and goal trackers showed the context change enhanced the pre-exposure effect in goal trackers and reduced it in sign trackers. Visual attention to the light declined during pre-exposure and did not recover with either conditioning or a context switch. Visual attention to the light decreased during conditioning. Visual goal tracking toward where the spaceship would appear was also retarded with pre-exposure. Unlike the keyboard responding, a context change led to more normal goal-tracking acquisition. Results are discussed in terms of theories of attention and the potential effects of demand characteristics on the task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39906252","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Use of different attentional strategies by pigeons and humans in multidimensional visual search. 鸽子和人类在多维视觉搜索中不同注意策略的使用。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-01-01 Epub Date: 2021-11-15 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000310
Suzanne L Gray, Muhammad A J Qadri, Daniel I Brooks, Robert G Cook

To study comparative attentional allocation strategies, pigeons and humans were tested using simultaneously available discrimination tasks. Given visual search displays containing 32 items from two orthogonal dimensions, participants were reinforced for selecting the eight brightest (or darkest) of 16 brightness items and the eight most vertical (or horizontal) of 16 orientation items. Consistent with a sequential dimensional strategy, humans preferentially chose items from one dimension before switching to the other to complete the search. In contrast, the pigeons did not preferentially stay within one dimension over consecutive choices. Instead, they chose the items most likely to yield reward based on item discriminability. Computational models that incorporated a "dimensional staying" factor accounted best for the human data, while simulations using only discriminability reproduced the pigeons' data. These results suggest that humans are sensitive to the benefits of attentional staying and the costs of switching between dimensional tasks, while there was no evidence that these factors influenced the pigeons' choice behavior. These findings suggest fundamental differences in how pigeons and humans allocate attention in complex choice situations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

为了研究比较注意分配策略,鸽子和人类使用同时可用的辨别任务进行了测试。给定视觉搜索显示,包含来自两个正交维度的32个项目,参与者被要求从16个亮度项目中选择8个最亮(或最暗)的项目,从16个方向项目中选择8个最垂直(或水平)的项目。与顺序维度策略一致,人类优先从一个维度选择项目,然后切换到另一个维度来完成搜索。相比之下,鸽子在连续的选择中不会优先停留在一个维度上。相反,他们会根据物品的可辨别性选择最有可能产生奖励的物品。包含“维度停留”因素的计算模型最能解释人类的数据,而只使用可判别性的模拟再现了鸽子的数据。这些结果表明,人类对注意力停留的好处和在不同维度任务之间切换的代价很敏感,而没有证据表明这些因素影响了鸽子的选择行为。这些发现表明,鸽子和人类在复杂选择情况下分配注意力的方式存在根本差异。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Use of different attentional strategies by pigeons and humans in multidimensional visual search.","authors":"Suzanne L Gray,&nbsp;Muhammad A J Qadri,&nbsp;Daniel I Brooks,&nbsp;Robert G Cook","doi":"10.1037/xan0000310","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000310","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To study comparative attentional allocation strategies, pigeons and humans were tested using simultaneously available discrimination tasks. Given visual search displays containing 32 items from two orthogonal dimensions, participants were reinforced for selecting the eight brightest (or darkest) of 16 brightness items and the eight most vertical (or horizontal) of 16 orientation items. Consistent with a sequential dimensional strategy, humans preferentially chose items from one dimension before switching to the other to complete the search. In contrast, the pigeons did not preferentially stay within one dimension over consecutive choices. Instead, they chose the items most likely to yield reward based on item discriminability. Computational models that incorporated a \"dimensional staying\" factor accounted best for the human data, while simulations using only discriminability reproduced the pigeons' data. These results suggest that humans are sensitive to the benefits of attentional staying and the costs of switching between dimensional tasks, while there was no evidence that these factors influenced the pigeons' choice behavior. These findings suggest fundamental differences in how pigeons and humans allocate attention in complex choice situations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39625624","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Nonreactive testing: Evaluating the effect of withholding feedback in predictive learning. 非反应性测试:评估预扣反馈在预测学习中的效果。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-01-01 Epub Date: 2021-11-29 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000311
Jessica C Lee, Mike E Le Pelley, Peter F Lovibond

Learning of cue-outcome relationships in associative learning experiments is often assessed by presenting cues without feedback about the outcome and informing participants to expect no outcomes to occur. The rationale is that this "no-feedback" testing procedure prevents new learning during testing that might contaminate the later test trials. We tested this assumption in 4 predictive learning experiments where participants were tasked with learning which foods (cues) were causing allergic reactions (the outcome) in a fictitious patient. We found that withholding feedback in a block of trials had no effect on causal ratings (Experiments 1 and 2), but it led to regression toward intermediate ratings when the missing feedback was embedded in the causal scenario and information about the outcome replaced by a "?" (Experiment 3). A factorial experiment manipulating cover story and feedback revealed that the regression-to-baseline effect was primarily driven by presentation of the "?" feedback (Experiment 4). We conclude that the procedure of testing without feedback, used widely in studies of human cognition, is an appropriate way of assessing learning, as long as the missing data are attributed to the experimenter and the absence of feedback is not highlighted in a way that induces uncertainty. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

在联想学习实验中,线索-结果关系的学习通常是通过提供线索而不反馈结果和告知参与者期望没有结果来评估的。其基本原理是,这种“无反馈”测试过程可以防止在测试过程中学习新的知识,而这些新知识可能会污染后来的测试试验。我们在4个预测学习实验中测试了这一假设,在这些实验中,参与者被要求学习哪些食物(线索)会引起一个虚构的病人的过敏反应(结果)。我们发现,在一组试验中保留反馈对因果评级没有影响(实验1和2),但当缺失的反馈嵌入到因果情景中,有关结果的信息被“?”取代时,它会导致向中间评级的回归。(实验3)。一个操纵封面故事和反馈的析因实验表明,回归基线效应主要是由“?”反馈的呈现驱动的(实验4)。我们得出结论,在人类认知研究中广泛使用的无反馈测试程序是一种适当的评估学习的方法,只要缺失的数据归因于实验者,并且反馈的缺失不会以一种引起不确定性的方式突出。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Nonreactive testing: Evaluating the effect of withholding feedback in predictive learning.","authors":"Jessica C Lee,&nbsp;Mike E Le Pelley,&nbsp;Peter F Lovibond","doi":"10.1037/xan0000311","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000311","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Learning of cue-outcome relationships in associative learning experiments is often assessed by presenting cues without feedback about the outcome and informing participants to expect no outcomes to occur. The rationale is that this \"no-feedback\" testing procedure prevents new learning during testing that might contaminate the later test trials. We tested this assumption in 4 predictive learning experiments where participants were tasked with learning which foods (cues) were causing allergic reactions (the outcome) in a fictitious patient. We found that withholding feedback in a block of trials had no effect on causal ratings (Experiments 1 and 2), but it led to regression toward intermediate ratings when the missing feedback was embedded in the causal scenario and information about the outcome replaced by a \"?\" (Experiment 3). A factorial experiment manipulating cover story and feedback revealed that the regression-to-baseline effect was primarily driven by presentation of the \"?\" feedback (Experiment 4). We conclude that the procedure of testing without feedback, used widely in studies of human cognition, is an appropriate way of assessing learning, as long as the missing data are attributed to the experimenter and the absence of feedback is not highlighted in a way that induces uncertainty. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39765153","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1