Pub Date : 2021-10-01Epub Date: 2021-09-13DOI: 10.1037/xan0000295
Mark E Bouton, Sean M Allan, Armin Tavakkoli, Michael R Steinfeld, Eric A Thrailkill
Four experiments manipulated the context in which taste-aversion conditioning occurred when the reinforcer was devalued after instrumental learning. In all experiments, rats learned to lever press in an operant conditioning chamber and then had an aversion to the food-pellet reinforcer conditioned by pairing it with lithium chloride (LiCl) in either that context or a different context. Lever pressing was then tested in extinction to assess its status as a goal-directed action. In Experiment 1, aversion conditioning in the operant conditioning chamber suppressed lever-pressing during the test, but aversion conditioning in the home cage did not. Exposure to the averted pellet in the operant conditioning chamber after conditioning in the home cage did not change this effect (Experiment 2). The same pattern was observed when the different context was a second operant-style chamber (counterbalanced), exposure to the contexts was controlled, and pellets were presented in them in the same manner (Experiment 3). The greater effect of aversion conditioning in the instrumental context was not merely due to potentiated contextual conditioning (Experiment 4). Importantly, consumption tests revealed that the aversion conditioned in the different context had transferred to the test context. Thus, when reinforcer devaluation occurred in a different context, the rats lever pressed in extinction for a reinforcer they would otherwise reject. The results suggest that animals encode contextual information about the reinforcer during instrumental learning and suggest caution in making inferences about action versus habit learning when the reinforcer is devalued in a different context. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
四个实验操纵了当强化物在工具性学习后被贬低时,味觉厌恶条件反射发生的情境。在所有实验中,大鼠在操作性条件反射室中学习杠杆按压,然后在该环境或不同环境中对食物颗粒强化物与氯化锂(LiCl)配对产生厌恶。然后在灭绝中测试杠杆按压,以评估其作为目标导向动作的状态。在实验1中,操作性条件反射室中的厌恶条件反射抑制了实验期间的杠杆按压,而家笼中的厌恶条件反射没有抑制杠杆按压。在家庭笼中条件反射后,在操作性条件反射室中暴露于避过的颗粒并没有改变这种效果(实验2)。当不同的环境是第二个操作性条件反射室(平衡)时,观察到同样的模式,对环境的暴露是受控的。和颗粒以相同的方式呈现在它们中(实验3)。厌恶条件反射在工具情境中的更大影响不仅仅是由于增强的情境条件反射(实验4)。重要的是,消费测试显示,在不同情境中条件反射的厌恶已经转移到测试情境中。因此,当强化物贬值发生在不同的环境中时,大鼠会对它们本来会拒绝的强化物施加灭绝压力。结果表明,动物在工具学习过程中对强化物的语境信息进行编码,当强化物在不同的语境中被贬低时,在对行为和习惯学习进行推断时要谨慎。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Effect of context on the instrumental reinforcer devaluation effect produced by taste-aversion learning.","authors":"Mark E Bouton, Sean M Allan, Armin Tavakkoli, Michael R Steinfeld, Eric A Thrailkill","doi":"10.1037/xan0000295","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000295","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Four experiments manipulated the context in which taste-aversion conditioning occurred when the reinforcer was devalued after instrumental learning. In all experiments, rats learned to lever press in an operant conditioning chamber and then had an aversion to the food-pellet reinforcer conditioned by pairing it with lithium chloride (LiCl) in either that context or a different context. Lever pressing was then tested in extinction to assess its status as a goal-directed action. In Experiment 1, aversion conditioning in the operant conditioning chamber suppressed lever-pressing during the test, but aversion conditioning in the home cage did not. Exposure to the averted pellet in the operant conditioning chamber after conditioning in the home cage did not change this effect (Experiment 2). The same pattern was observed when the different context was a second operant-style chamber (counterbalanced), exposure to the contexts was controlled, and pellets were presented in them in the same manner (Experiment 3). The greater effect of aversion conditioning in the instrumental context was not merely due to potentiated contextual conditioning (Experiment 4). Importantly, consumption tests revealed that the aversion conditioned in the different context had transferred to the test context. Thus, when reinforcer devaluation occurred in a different context, the rats lever pressed in extinction for a reinforcer they would otherwise reject. The results suggest that animals encode contextual information about the reinforcer during instrumental learning and suggest caution in making inferences about action versus habit learning when the reinforcer is devalued in a different context. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8713511/pdf/nihms-1738628.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39411983","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-10-01Epub Date: 2021-09-02DOI: 10.1037/xan0000305
Douglas R Schuweiler, Manaahil Rao, Heather J Pribut, Matthew R Roesch
The rat is a common animal model used to uncover the neural underpinnings of decision making and their disruption in psychiatric illness. Here, we ask if rats can perform a decision-making task that assesses self-control by delayed gratification in the context of diminishing returns. In this task, rats could choose to press one of two levers. One lever was associated with a fixed delay (FD) schedule that delivered reward after a fixed time delay (10 s). The other lever was associated with a progressive delay (PD) schedule; the delay increased by a fixed amount of time (1 s) after each PD lever press. Rats were tested under two conditions: a reset condition where rats could reset the PD schedule back to its initial 0-s delay by pressing the FD lever and a no-reset condition in which resetting the PD schedule was unavailable. We found that rats adapted behavior within reset sessions by delaying gratification to obtain more reward in the long run. That is, they selected the FD lever with the longer delay to reset the PD delay back to zero prior to the equality point, thus achieving more reward over the course of the session. These results are consistent with other species, demonstrating that rats can also maximize the net rate of reward by selecting an option that is not immediately beneficial. Moreover, use of this task in rodents might provide insights into how the brain governs normal and abnormal behavior, as well as treatments that can improve self-control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
{"title":"Rats delay gratification during a time-based diminishing returns task.","authors":"Douglas R Schuweiler, Manaahil Rao, Heather J Pribut, Matthew R Roesch","doi":"10.1037/xan0000305","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xan0000305","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The rat is a common animal model used to uncover the neural underpinnings of decision making and their disruption in psychiatric illness. Here, we ask if rats can perform a decision-making task that assesses self-control by delayed gratification in the context of diminishing returns. In this task, rats could choose to press one of two levers. One lever was associated with a fixed delay (FD) schedule that delivered reward after a fixed time delay (10 s). The other lever was associated with a progressive delay (PD) schedule; the delay increased by a fixed amount of time (1 s) after each PD lever press. Rats were tested under two conditions: a reset condition where rats could reset the PD schedule back to its initial 0-s delay by pressing the FD lever and a no-reset condition in which resetting the PD schedule was unavailable. We found that rats adapted behavior within reset sessions by delaying gratification to obtain more reward in the long run. That is, they selected the FD lever with the longer delay to reset the PD delay back to zero prior to the equality point, thus achieving more reward over the course of the session. These results are consistent with other species, demonstrating that rats can also maximize the net rate of reward by selecting an option that is not immediately beneficial. Moreover, use of this task in rodents might provide insights into how the brain governs normal and abnormal behavior, as well as treatments that can improve self-control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8639657/pdf/nihms-1738622.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39377416","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Given a choice, pigeons prefer an initial-link stimulus that is followed by reliable signals that food will be delivered (S+) or not (S-) after a delay, over an alternative initial-link stimulus that is followed by unreliable signals of food, even when the former yields a lower overall probability of food. This suboptimal preference has been attributed to the combination of a biased attraction to the S+ and ignoring the S-. We evaluated the inhibitory properties of the S- in three experiments to investigate its role in suboptimal choice. In Experiment 1, pigeons were trained in an autoshaping procedure with the four terminal link stimuli of the suboptimal choice task; S+ was continuously reinforced, S3 and S4 were each partially reinforced on a 50% schedule, and S- was never reinforced. Summation tests showed that S- acquired inhibitory properties during training. Experiment 2 replicated the summation tests after training on the full suboptimal choice procedure and found that S- inhibition was not attributable to external inhibition. In Experiment 3, pigeons were trained on the suboptimal choice procedure and the development of inhibition was assessed throughout training. An analysis of individual differences across birds revealed that the response rates to S- were negatively correlated with the strength of suboptimal preference, both within subject as each bird acquired suboptimal preference, and across subjects once all birds had reached asymptotic levels of suboptimal preference. Thus, rather than ignoring the S-, we found evidence that birds attended to S- as an inhibitory stimulus. Future models explaining performance in the suboptimal choice task should consider inhibition to the S- as a factor in suboptimal choice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
{"title":"The role of inhibition in the suboptimal choice task.","authors":"Valeria V González, Aaron P Blaisdell","doi":"10.1037/xan0000300","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xan0000300","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Given a choice, pigeons prefer an initial-link stimulus that is followed by reliable signals that food will be delivered (S+) or not (S-) after a delay, over an alternative initial-link stimulus that is followed by unreliable signals of food, even when the former yields a lower overall probability of food. This suboptimal preference has been attributed to the combination of a biased attraction to the S+ and ignoring the S-. We evaluated the inhibitory properties of the S- in three experiments to investigate its role in suboptimal choice. In Experiment 1, pigeons were trained in an autoshaping procedure with the four terminal link stimuli of the suboptimal choice task; S+ was continuously reinforced, S3 and S4 were each partially reinforced on a 50% schedule, and S- was never reinforced. Summation tests showed that S- acquired inhibitory properties during training. Experiment 2 replicated the summation tests after training on the full suboptimal choice procedure and found that S- inhibition was not attributable to external inhibition. In Experiment 3, pigeons were trained on the suboptimal choice procedure and the development of inhibition was assessed throughout training. An analysis of individual differences across birds revealed that the response rates to S- were negatively correlated with the strength of suboptimal preference, both within subject as each bird acquired suboptimal preference, and across subjects once all birds had reached asymptotic levels of suboptimal preference. Thus, rather than ignoring the S-, we found evidence that birds attended to S- as an inhibitory stimulus. Future models explaining performance in the suboptimal choice task should consider inhibition to the S- as a factor in suboptimal choice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39684981","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-10-01Epub Date: 2021-09-13DOI: 10.1037/xan0000263
Cristina Santos, Marco Vasconcelos, Armando Machado
We used a midsession reversal task to investigate how temporal and situational cues may combine to determine choice in frequently changing environments. Pigeons learned a simultaneous discrimination with 2 stimuli: S1 and S2. Choices of S1 were reinforced only during the first trials, and choices of S2 were reinforced only during the last trials of the session, that is, the reinforcement contingencies reversed once during the session. To weaken the temporal cue (time into the session) that signaled the reversal trial, we varied the location of reversal trial randomly across sessions; to weaken the situational cue (the outcome of the previous trials that might support a win-stay/loose shift strategy), we varied the payoff probabilities associated with S1 and S2. Performance was consistent neither with the exclusive use of a timing strategy, nor with the exclusive use of a situational, win-stay/lose-shift strategy. Instead, choice seemed to be under joint control of both cues. The relative influence of these cues was dynamic: When payoff was higher for S1 than S2, behavior was less time-controlled than when the payoff was higher for S2 that S1, or when they were equal. We advance a descriptive mixture model of joint control for the midsession reversal task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
我们使用中间反转任务来研究时间和情境线索如何结合在一起,在频繁变化的环境中决定选择。鸽子在S1和S2两种刺激下学会了同时辨别。S1的选择只在第一次试验中被强化,S2的选择只在最后一次试验中被强化,即强化随因在试验中反转一次。为了削弱提示逆转试验的时间线索(进入会话的时间),我们在会话中随机改变逆转试验的位置;为了弱化情境提示(之前试验的结果可能支持赢-留/松-换策略),我们改变了与S1和S2相关的支付概率。性能既不与单独使用定时策略一致,也不与单独使用情境策略一致,赢-留/输-换策略。相反,选择似乎是由两种线索共同控制的。这些线索的相对影响是动态的:当S1的回报高于S2时,行为的时间控制比S2的回报高于S1时或两者相等时要少。我们提出了一种用于会话中反转任务的联合控制的描述性混合模型。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Constantly timing, but not always controlled by time: Evidence from the midsession reversal task.","authors":"Cristina Santos, Marco Vasconcelos, Armando Machado","doi":"10.1037/xan0000263","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000263","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We used a midsession reversal task to investigate how temporal and situational cues may combine to determine choice in frequently changing environments. Pigeons learned a simultaneous discrimination with 2 stimuli: S1 and S2. Choices of S1 were reinforced only during the first trials, and choices of S2 were reinforced only during the last trials of the session, that is, the reinforcement contingencies reversed once during the session. To weaken the temporal cue (time into the session) that signaled the reversal trial, we varied the location of reversal trial randomly across sessions; to weaken the situational cue (the outcome of the previous trials that might support a win-stay/loose shift strategy), we varied the payoff probabilities associated with S1 and S2. Performance was consistent neither with the exclusive use of a timing strategy, nor with the exclusive use of a situational, win-stay/lose-shift strategy. Instead, choice seemed to be under joint control of both cues. The relative influence of these cues was dynamic: When payoff was higher for S1 than S2, behavior was less time-controlled than when the payoff was higher for S2 that S1, or when they were equal. We advance a descriptive mixture model of joint control for the midsession reversal task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39411985","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-10-01Epub Date: 2021-09-13DOI: 10.1037/xan0000297
Francisca Diaz, Ellen M O'Donoghue, Edward A Wasserman
Research on same-different categorization has shown that mastery of tasks of this kind can be strongly affected by the number of items in the training arrays-for both humans and nonhuman animals. Evidence for two-item same-different categorization in pigeons is decidedly mixed: although some investigations have succeeded, others have failed. To date, no research has documented successful conditional same-different categorization using just two items, nor has research explored how pigeons' responses in this paradigm might be influenced by perceptual characteristics of the training stimuli. Through a series of methodological modifications, we provide the first successful documentation that pigeons can perform two-item conditional same-different categorization to a high degree of accuracy; further, they can do so without the support of item repetition. We also show for the first time that the perceptual disparity between the items in pairs of different stimuli plays a key part in pigeons' same-different categorization performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
对相同-不同分类的研究表明,对这类任务的掌握程度会受到训练数组中项目数量的强烈影响——对人类和非人类动物都是如此。鸽子的两项相同-不同分类的证据显然是混杂的:尽管一些调查成功了,但另一些却失败了。迄今为止,还没有研究成功地记录了仅使用两个项目的条件相同-不同分类,也没有研究探索在这种范式下鸽子的反应如何受到训练刺激的感知特征的影响。通过一系列的方法修改,我们提供了第一个成功的文献,鸽子可以执行两项条件相同-不同分类,准确度很高;此外,他们可以在没有项目重复的支持下做到这一点。我们还首次证明了不同刺激物对之间的知觉差异在鸽子的相同-不同分类表现中起着关键作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Two-item conditional same-different categorization in pigeons: Finding differences.","authors":"Francisca Diaz, Ellen M O'Donoghue, Edward A Wasserman","doi":"10.1037/xan0000297","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000297","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research on same-different categorization has shown that mastery of tasks of this kind can be strongly affected by the number of items in the training arrays-for both humans and nonhuman animals. Evidence for two-item same-different categorization in pigeons is decidedly mixed: although some investigations have succeeded, others have failed. To date, no research has documented successful conditional same-different categorization using just two items, nor has research explored how pigeons' responses in this paradigm might be influenced by perceptual characteristics of the training stimuli. Through a series of methodological modifications, we provide the first successful documentation that pigeons can perform two-item conditional same-different categorization to a high degree of accuracy; further, they can do so without the support of item repetition. We also show for the first time that the perceptual disparity between the items in pairs of different stimuli plays a key part in pigeons' same-different categorization performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8639609/pdf/nihms-1738629.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39411984","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Rats Delay Gratification During a Time-Based Diminishing Returns Task","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/xan0000305.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000305.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-08-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"57377785","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
It has been suggested that schedule-induced behaviors allow organisms to adapt better to temporal regularities of the environment. The main goal of the present study was to observe the effect of schedule-induced drinking (SID) on the performance in fixed-interval (FI) schedules. Rats were exposed to a FI 15-, 30-, or 60-s food reinforcement schedule, and only half of them had access to water in the experimental chamber. Rats with access to water developed SID, which occurred in the first part of the interval, regardless of the FI value, and was followed by an increase in lever pressing rate. There were no substantial differences in the quantitative measures of timing between groups that had or did not have access to water, except for the rats in the FI 15-s group with access to water, who showed longer postreinforcement pauses, possibly attributable to competition between SID and lever pressing. SID did not manifest the scalar property, contrary to lever pressing, but it is proposed that behaviors are displayed serially until the last behavior before the target operant response becomes a discriminative stimulus for that behavior. It is not assumed that the purpose of schedule-induced behaviors is to aid timing, but the development of behavioral patterns might determine the performance of organisms on temporal tasks. Additionally, in some cases competition between responses might exert more control on when the operant behavior occurs than timing. Timing seems to consist in the temporal organization of available behaviors that leads to a specific behavior occurring at a specified time, a single characteristic that typically had come to indicate accurate timing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
有人认为,时间表诱导的行为使生物体能够更好地适应环境的时间规律。本研究的主要目的是观察计划性饮酒(SID)对固定间隔(FI)时间表表现的影响。大鼠被暴露在FI 15、30或60秒的食物强化计划中,只有一半的大鼠在实验室内可以获得水。与FI值无关,接触水的大鼠发生SID,发生在间隔的前半段,随后是杠杆按压率的增加。在有水或没有水的组之间,时间的定量测量没有实质性差异,除了有水的FI 15-s组的大鼠,它们表现出更长的强化后暂停,可能归因于SID和杠杆按压之间的竞争。与杠杆按压相反,SID没有表现出标量性质,但有人提出,行为是连续显示的,直到最后一个行为,然后目标操作反应成为该行为的判别刺激。我们并不认为时间安排诱导行为的目的是帮助安排时间,但行为模式的发展可能决定生物体在时间任务中的表现。此外,在某些情况下,反应之间的竞争可能对操作行为发生的时间施加更多的控制,而不是时间。时间似乎存在于导致特定行为在特定时间发生的可用行为的时间组织中,这是一个通常表明准确时间的单一特征。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Assessment of the 'timing' function of schedule-induced behavior on fixed-interval performance.","authors":"Gabriela E López-Tolsa, Ricardo Pellón","doi":"10.1037/xan0000308","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000308","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It has been suggested that schedule-induced behaviors allow organisms to adapt better to temporal regularities of the environment. The main goal of the present study was to observe the effect of schedule-induced drinking (SID) on the performance in fixed-interval (FI) schedules. Rats were exposed to a FI 15-, 30-, or 60-s food reinforcement schedule, and only half of them had access to water in the experimental chamber. Rats with access to water developed SID, which occurred in the first part of the interval, regardless of the FI value, and was followed by an increase in lever pressing rate. There were no substantial differences in the quantitative measures of timing between groups that had or did not have access to water, except for the rats in the FI 15-s group with access to water, who showed longer postreinforcement pauses, possibly attributable to competition between SID and lever pressing. SID did not manifest the scalar property, contrary to lever pressing, but it is proposed that behaviors are displayed serially until the last behavior before the target operant response becomes a discriminative stimulus for that behavior. It is not assumed that the purpose of schedule-induced behaviors is to aid timing, but the development of behavioral patterns might determine the performance of organisms on temporal tasks. Additionally, in some cases competition between responses might exert more control on when the operant behavior occurs than timing. Timing seems to consist in the temporal organization of available behaviors that leads to a specific behavior occurring at a specified time, a single characteristic that typically had come to indicate accurate timing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39493515","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
This special issue originally placed a Call for Papers that emphasized the importance of "Conceptual and Methodological" advances in the field of Comparative Cognition. Represented here is a collection of 14 papers that helps to display some of the diversity of ideas and approaches within this flourishing research area. The first paper in this issue, by Gazes and Lazareva (2021), discusses transitive inference learning from the perspectives of: identifying the problems of contextual variables in studying different species; whether associative processes can or cannot fully account for the behavior and, if not, what alternative representational mechanisms might be at work; and, finally, how ecological considerations may support comparative research by suggesting novel theoretical and empirical questions. The next paper, by Loy et al. (2021) investigates questions related to the complexity of learning in invertebrate species, single-celled organisms, and plants. The paper by Rawlings et al. (2021) reviews the literature on cumulative cultural evolution, primarily in nonhuman primate species, and critically evaluates the importance of identifying the essential conceptual and methodological issues in what many have deemed to be a uniquely human form of behavior. The paper by Goto and Watanabe (2021) explores whether the mouse visual system is sensitive to Gestalt principles, using operant discrimination learning tasks similar to those used previously to document Gestalt processing in chimpanzees and humans. Qadri and Cook (2021) use the innovative approach of "adaptive genetic algorithms" to assess the relative importance of different features of a stimulus in controlling organisms' discrimination learning performance. Wittek et al. (2021) introduce a novel method for studying the importance of visual accumulation processes in pigeons when information is presented to a single hemisphere at a time. The paper by Cowie et al. (2021) focuses on a misallocation model of two-step sequence learning in young children and explores from a behavioranalytic viewpoint the implications of assuming that reinforcement might be misattributed to a misremembered response at the beginning of the behavioral sequence. The paper by López-Tolsa and Pellón (2021) explores whether the opportunity to display schedule-induced drinking as an early response within a behavioral sequence might alter the accuracy of temporal control in different-length fixed-interval schedule tasks with rats. Crystal (2021) reviews the literature examining episodic memory in nonhuman species and considers a variety of criteria and methods thought to be crucial for establishing empirical evidence for episodic memory in nonhumans, in general, and rats, in particular. Vila et al. (2021) discuss the use a novel 'hide-and-seek' task in preschool age children to study episodic-like memory; their work illustrates how memory dynamics can change over time in a manner not very unlike what has been demons
这期特刊最初是征稿,强调了比较认知领域“概念和方法”进步的重要性。这里代表的是14篇论文的集合,这些论文有助于展示这个蓬勃发展的研究领域中一些思想和方法的多样性。本期的第一篇论文由Gazes和Lazareva(2021)撰写,从以下几个角度讨论了传递推理学习:在研究不同物种时识别上下文变量的问题;联想过程是否能或不能完全解释行为,如果不能,有哪些替代的表征机制可能在起作用;最后,生态考虑如何通过提出新颖的理论和实证问题来支持比较研究。下一篇论文由Loy等人(2021)撰写,研究了与无脊椎动物、单细胞生物和植物学习复杂性相关的问题。罗林斯等人(2021)的论文回顾了关于累积文化进化的文献,主要是在非人类灵长类动物物种中,并批判性地评估了识别基本概念和方法问题的重要性,许多人认为这是一种独特的人类行为形式。Goto和Watanabe(2021)的论文探讨了小鼠视觉系统是否对格式塔原则敏感,使用了类似于之前用于记录黑猩猩和人类格式塔处理的操作性区分学习任务。Qadri和Cook(2021)使用了“自适应遗传算法”的创新方法来评估刺激的不同特征在控制生物体辨别学习表现中的相对重要性。Wittek等人(2021)引入了一种新方法,用于研究当信息一次呈现给单个半球时,鸽子视觉积累过程的重要性。Cowie等人(2021)的论文侧重于幼儿两步序列学习的错误分配模型,并从行为分析的角度探讨了假设强化可能被错误归因于行为序列开始时的错误记忆反应的含义。López-Tolsa和Pellón(2021)的论文探讨了将计划诱发饮酒作为行为序列中的早期反应的机会是否会改变大鼠在不同长度的固定间隔计划任务中时间控制的准确性。Crystal(2021)回顾了研究非人类物种情景记忆的文献,并认为各种标准和方法对于建立非人类情景记忆的经验证据至关重要,尤其是大鼠。Vila等人(2021)讨论了在学龄前儿童中使用一种新颖的“捉迷藏”任务来研究情景记忆;他们的工作说明了记忆动态如何随着时间的推移而改变,这与其他非人类研究范式所证明的情况并没有太大的不同。Krichbaum等人(2021)的论文讨论了研究犬类空间认知时面临的一些方法上的困难。Castro等人(2021)的论文使用了一个复杂的分类学习任务,在这个任务中,不同的显示特征集取决于上下文,是对类别掌握的诊断,还是不是。Vernouillet等人(2021)的论文探讨了两种鸦类(小松鸦和加利福尼亚灌丛鸦)相同/不同概念学习的形成。Lazarowski等人(2021)的最后一篇论文使用嗅觉刺激的试验-独特训练匹配-样本程序,研究了犬类抽象相同/不同学习的可能性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Comparative cognition-Conceptual and methodological advancements.","authors":"Andrew R Delamater, Edward A Wasserman","doi":"10.1037/xan0000309","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000309","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This special issue originally placed a Call for Papers that emphasized the importance of \"Conceptual and Methodological\" advances in the field of Comparative Cognition. Represented here is a collection of 14 papers that helps to display some of the diversity of ideas and approaches within this flourishing research area. The first paper in this issue, by Gazes and Lazareva (2021), discusses transitive inference learning from the perspectives of: identifying the problems of contextual variables in studying different species; whether associative processes can or cannot fully account for the behavior and, if not, what alternative representational mechanisms might be at work; and, finally, how ecological considerations may support comparative research by suggesting novel theoretical and empirical questions. The next paper, by Loy et al. (2021) investigates questions related to the complexity of learning in invertebrate species, single-celled organisms, and plants. The paper by Rawlings et al. (2021) reviews the literature on cumulative cultural evolution, primarily in nonhuman primate species, and critically evaluates the importance of identifying the essential conceptual and methodological issues in what many have deemed to be a uniquely human form of behavior. The paper by Goto and Watanabe (2021) explores whether the mouse visual system is sensitive to Gestalt principles, using operant discrimination learning tasks similar to those used previously to document Gestalt processing in chimpanzees and humans. Qadri and Cook (2021) use the innovative approach of \"adaptive genetic algorithms\" to assess the relative importance of different features of a stimulus in controlling organisms' discrimination learning performance. Wittek et al. (2021) introduce a novel method for studying the importance of visual accumulation processes in pigeons when information is presented to a single hemisphere at a time. The paper by Cowie et al. (2021) focuses on a misallocation model of two-step sequence learning in young children and explores from a behavioranalytic viewpoint the implications of assuming that reinforcement might be misattributed to a misremembered response at the beginning of the behavioral sequence. The paper by López-Tolsa and Pellón (2021) explores whether the opportunity to display schedule-induced drinking as an early response within a behavioral sequence might alter the accuracy of temporal control in different-length fixed-interval schedule tasks with rats. Crystal (2021) reviews the literature examining episodic memory in nonhuman species and considers a variety of criteria and methods thought to be crucial for establishing empirical evidence for episodic memory in nonhumans, in general, and rats, in particular. Vila et al. (2021) discuss the use a novel 'hide-and-seek' task in preschool age children to study episodic-like memory; their work illustrates how memory dynamics can change over time in a manner not very unlike what has been demons","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39496110","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Sarah Krichbaum, Jordan G Smith, Lucia Lazarowski, Jeffrey S Katz
Short assessments of spatial working memory (SWM) in dogs are becoming popular evaluations of canine aging and individual differences. In a typical SWM task, an experimenter hides a reward inside of a bucket at a specific stimulus position while the dog watches. Then, following a varying delay interval, the dog is released to choose a bucket. The longest delay at which the dog can successfully choose the bucket containing the reward is considered to reflect the dog's SWM duration. Although past studies were informative, the tasks often lacked a valid measure of SWM due to dogs' ability to use nonmnemonic strategies, such as body orientation or sustained attention, to successfully solve these tasks without relying on working memory mechanisms. Therefore, we designed the first study to assess the internal validity of these tasks by directly comparing dogs' performance on two experimental conditions. We found that dogs performed worse in a control condition in which nonmnemonic strategies were eliminated compared with a typical SWM task condition. In addition, our results indicate a strong relationship between the percentage of delay time that a dog spends orienting its head or body to the correct bucket and performance in a typical SWM task. These findings were the first to show a difference in SWM performance when dogs' use of nonmnemonic strategies was controlled and stress the importance of considering the internal validity of these tasks if used to examine SWM in future work. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
犬空间工作记忆的短时间评估已成为犬类衰老和个体差异的常用评估方法。在一个典型的SWM任务中,实验者将奖励藏在一个特定刺激位置的桶里,而狗则在一旁观看。然后,在一个不同的延迟间隔后,狗被释放去选择一个桶。狗成功选择包含奖励的桶的最长延迟被认为反映了狗的SWM持续时间。尽管过去的研究提供了信息,但由于狗能够使用非助记策略(如身体定向或持续注意)来成功解决这些任务,而不依赖于工作记忆机制,因此这些任务往往缺乏对SWM的有效衡量。因此,我们设计了第一个研究,通过直接比较狗在两种实验条件下的表现来评估这些任务的内部效度。我们发现,与典型的SWM任务条件相比,在消除非助记策略的控制条件下,狗的表现更差。此外,我们的结果表明,在典型的SWM任务中,狗花在将头部或身体定位到正确桶上的延迟时间百分比与表现之间存在很强的关系。这些发现首次表明,当狗使用非助记策略时,SWM的表现有所不同,并强调了考虑这些任务的内部有效性的重要性,如果在未来的工作中用于检查SWM。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Controlling for dogs' (Canis familiaris) use of nonmnemonic strategies in a spatial working memory task.","authors":"Sarah Krichbaum, Jordan G Smith, Lucia Lazarowski, Jeffrey S Katz","doi":"10.1037/xan0000293","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000293","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Short assessments of spatial working memory (SWM) in dogs are becoming popular evaluations of canine aging and individual differences. In a typical SWM task, an experimenter hides a reward inside of a bucket at a specific stimulus position while the dog watches. Then, following a varying delay interval, the dog is released to choose a bucket. The longest delay at which the dog can successfully choose the bucket containing the reward is considered to reflect the dog's SWM duration. Although past studies were informative, the tasks often lacked a valid measure of SWM due to dogs' ability to use nonmnemonic strategies, such as body orientation or sustained attention, to successfully solve these tasks without relying on working memory mechanisms. Therefore, we designed the first study to assess the internal validity of these tasks by directly comparing dogs' performance on two experimental conditions. We found that dogs performed worse in a control condition in which nonmnemonic strategies were eliminated compared with a typical SWM task condition. In addition, our results indicate a strong relationship between the percentage of delay time that a dog spends orienting its head or body to the correct bucket and performance in a typical SWM task. These findings were the first to show a difference in SWM performance when dogs' use of nonmnemonic strategies was controlled and stress the importance of considering the internal validity of these tasks if used to examine SWM in future work. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39493518","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
An innovative adaptive discrimination procedure examined how two bird species, pigeons and starlings, recognize and discriminate two-dimensional (2D) visual shapes. Prior results suggest a comparative divergence between mammals and birds in their relative reliance on vertices versus line segments to mediate discrimination. To address this potentially important difference, four pigeons and five starlings were tested with a square versus triangle discrimination in two experiments. An adaptive genetic algorithm guided the selection and organization of the training and test stimuli. Both species showed considerable flexibility in accurately selecting triangles despite wide variation in stimulus appearance and location. Most critically, Experiment 2 revealed that both bird species relied more on the figures' vertices during successful discrimination than their connecting line segments. This reliance was revealed by both traditional accuracy differences using contour-deleted displays and genetic algorithm-based shifts in "gene values" caused by the birds' selection. These results, in contrast to previous findings, indicate that mammals and birds likely converge in their reliance on vertices as a highly critical feature in visual shape discrimination. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
一种创新的适应性识别程序研究了鸽子和椋鸟两种鸟类如何识别和区分二维视觉形状。先前的结果表明,哺乳动物和鸟类在相对依赖顶点和线段来调解歧视方面存在比较差异。为了解决这个潜在的重要差异,在两个实验中对四只鸽子和五只椋鸟进行了正方形和三角形的区分测试。一种自适应遗传算法指导训练和测试刺激的选择和组织。尽管刺激的外观和位置存在很大差异,但这两个物种在准确选择三角形方面都表现出相当大的灵活性。最关键的是,实验2揭示了两种鸟类在成功识别时更多地依赖于图形的顶点,而不是它们的连接线段。这种依赖通过使用轮廓删除显示的传统精度差异和由鸟类选择引起的基于遗传算法的“基因值”变化来揭示。这些结果与之前的研究结果相反,表明哺乳动物和鸟类可能在依赖顶点这一高度关键的视觉形状识别特征上趋于一致。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Adaptive testing of the critical features in 2D-shape discrimination by pigeons and starlings.","authors":"Muhammad A J Qadri, Robert G Cook","doi":"10.1037/xan0000307","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000307","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>An innovative adaptive discrimination procedure examined how two bird species, pigeons and starlings, recognize and discriminate two-dimensional (2D) visual shapes. Prior results suggest a comparative divergence between mammals and birds in their relative reliance on vertices versus line segments to mediate discrimination. To address this potentially important difference, four pigeons and five starlings were tested with a square versus triangle discrimination in two experiments. An adaptive genetic algorithm guided the selection and organization of the training and test stimuli. Both species showed considerable flexibility in accurately selecting triangles despite wide variation in stimulus appearance and location. Most critically, Experiment 2 revealed that both bird species relied more on the figures' vertices during successful discrimination than their connecting line segments. This reliance was revealed by both traditional accuracy differences using contour-deleted displays and genetic algorithm-based shifts in \"gene values\" caused by the birds' selection. These results, in contrast to previous findings, indicate that mammals and birds likely converge in their reliance on vertices as a highly critical feature in visual shape discrimination. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39496115","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}