首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Behavioral studies of spinal conditioning: The spinal cord is smarter than you think it is. 脊髓条件反射的行为研究:脊髓比你想象的更聪明。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-10-01 Epub Date: 2022-07-28 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000332
James W Grau, Kelsey E Hudson, Megan M Tarbet, Misty M Strain

In 1988 Robert Rescorla published an article in the Annual Review of Neuroscience that addressed the circumstances under which learning occurs, some key methodological issues, and what constitutes an example of learning. The article has inspired a generation of neuroscientists, opening the door to a wider range of learning phenomena. After reviewing the historical context for his article, its key points are briefly reviewed. The perspective outlined enabled the study of learning in simpler preparations, such as the spinal cord. The period after 1988 revealed that pain (nociceptive) stimuli can induce a lasting sensitization of spinal cord circuits, laying down a kind of memory mediated by signal pathways analogous to those implicated in brain dependent learning and memory. Evidence suggests that the spinal cord is sensitive to instrumental response-outcome (R-O) relations, that learning can induce a peripheral modification (muscle memory) that helps maintain the learned response, and that learning can promote adaptive plasticity (a form of metaplasticity). Conversely, exposure to uncontrollable stimulation disables the capacity to learn. Spinal cord neurons can also abstract that stimuli occur in a regular (predictable) manner, a capacity that appears linked to a neural oscillator (central pattern generator). Disrupting communication with the brain has been shown to transform how GABA affects neuronal function (an example of ionic plasticity), releasing a brake that enables plasticity. We conclude by presenting a framework for understanding these findings and the implications for the broader study of learning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

1988年,Robert Rescorla在《神经科学年度评论》上发表了一篇文章,阐述了学习发生的环境、一些关键的方法论问题以及学习的例子。这篇文章启发了一代神经科学家,为更广泛的学习现象打开了大门。在回顾其文章的历史背景后,对文章的要点作了简要的回顾。概述的观点使研究能够在更简单的准备中学习,例如脊髓。1988年之后的一段时间表明,疼痛(伤害性)刺激可以诱导脊髓回路的持久敏化,从而建立一种由类似于大脑依赖性学习和记忆的信号通路介导的记忆。有证据表明,脊髓对工具反应-结果(R-O)关系很敏感,学习可以诱导有助于维持学习反应的外周改变(肌肉记忆),学习可以促进适应性可塑性(一种化塑性)。相反,暴露于无法控制的刺激会削弱学习能力。脊髓神经元也可以抽象出刺激以有规律(可预测)的方式发生,这种能力似乎与神经振荡器(中央模式发生器)有关。破坏与大脑的交流已被证明可以改变GABA对神经元功能的影响(离子可塑性的一个例子),释放使可塑性发挥作用的制动器。最后,我们提出了一个框架来理解这些发现以及对更广泛的学习研究的影响。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2022 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Behavioral studies of spinal conditioning: The spinal cord is smarter than you think it is.","authors":"James W Grau,&nbsp;Kelsey E Hudson,&nbsp;Megan M Tarbet,&nbsp;Misty M Strain","doi":"10.1037/xan0000332","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xan0000332","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In 1988 Robert Rescorla published an article in the <i>Annual Review of Neuroscience</i> that addressed the circumstances under which learning occurs, some key methodological issues, and what constitutes an example of learning. The article has inspired a generation of neuroscientists, opening the door to a wider range of learning phenomena. After reviewing the historical context for his article, its key points are briefly reviewed. The perspective outlined enabled the study of learning in simpler preparations, such as the spinal cord. The period after 1988 revealed that pain (nociceptive) stimuli can induce a lasting sensitization of spinal cord circuits, laying down a kind of memory mediated by signal pathways analogous to those implicated in brain dependent learning and memory. Evidence suggests that the spinal cord is sensitive to instrumental response-outcome (R-O) relations, that learning can induce a peripheral modification (muscle memory) that helps maintain the learned response, and that learning can promote adaptive plasticity (a form of metaplasticity). Conversely, exposure to uncontrollable stimulation disables the capacity to learn. Spinal cord neurons can also abstract that stimuli occur in a regular (predictable) manner, a capacity that appears linked to a neural oscillator (central pattern generator). Disrupting communication with the brain has been shown to transform how GABA affects neuronal function (an example of ionic plasticity), releasing a brake that enables plasticity. We conclude by presenting a framework for understanding these findings and the implications for the broader study of learning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10391333/pdf/nihms-1902123.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10286253","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
On the importance of feedback for categorization: Revisiting category learning experiments using an adaptive filter model. 论反馈对分类的重要性:用自适应滤波模型回顾类别学习实验。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-10-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000339
Nicolás Marchant, Sergio E Chaigneau

Associative accounts of category learning have been, for the most part, abandoned in favor of cognitive explanations (e.g., similarity, explicit rules). In the current work, we implement an Adaptive Linear Filter (ALF) closely related to the Rescorla and Wagner learning rule, and use it to tackle three learning tasks that pose challenges to an associative view of category learning. Across three computational simulations, we show that the ALF is in fact able to make the predictions that seemed problematic. Notably, in our simulations we use exactly the same model and specifications, attesting to the generality of our account. We discuss the consequences of our findings for the category learning literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

在很大程度上,类别学习的联想解释已经被抛弃,而倾向于认知解释(例如,相似性,明确规则)。在当前的工作中,我们实现了一个与Rescorla和Wagner学习规则密切相关的自适应线性滤波器(ALF),并使用它来解决对类别学习的联想观点构成挑战的三个学习任务。通过三个计算模拟,我们表明ALF实际上能够做出看起来有问题的预测。值得注意的是,在我们的模拟中,我们使用了完全相同的模型和规格,证明了我们的描述的普遍性。我们讨论了我们的发现对类别学习文献的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"On the importance of feedback for categorization: Revisiting category learning experiments using an adaptive filter model.","authors":"Nicolás Marchant,&nbsp;Sergio E Chaigneau","doi":"10.1037/xan0000339","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000339","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Associative accounts of category learning have been, for the most part, abandoned in favor of cognitive explanations (e.g., similarity, explicit rules). In the current work, we implement an Adaptive Linear Filter (ALF) closely related to the Rescorla and Wagner learning rule, and use it to tackle three learning tasks that pose challenges to an associative view of category learning. Across three computational simulations, we show that the ALF is in fact able to make the predictions that seemed problematic. Notably, in our simulations we use exactly the same model and specifications, attesting to the generality of our account. We discuss the consequences of our findings for the category learning literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40657695","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Inhibition in discriminated operant learning: Tests of response-specificity after feature-negative and extinction learning. 辨别操作性学习中的抑制:特征阴性和消退学习后的反应特异性测试。
IF 1.2 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-10-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000337
Michael R Steinfeld, Mark E Bouton

Six experiments with rats examined the nature of inhibition learned in an operant feature-negative (FN) discrimination. The results of prior experiments that examined instrumental extinction rather than FN learning suggest that inhibition can be very specific to the inhibited response. In Experiment 1, we trained lever-press and chain-pull responses in separate but parallel FN discriminations (AR1+, ABR1-, CR2+, and CDR2-) and then tested both inhibitors (B and D) with both responses. Of primary interest was the extent to which the inhibitors suppressed the response they were trained with (same-response inhibition) versus the other response (cross-response inhibition). We found that cross-response inhibition was robust and essentially equal to same-response inhibition. Experiment 2 replicated this result and confirmed stronger inhibition after FN learning than after a differential inhibition procedure (AR1+, BR1-, CR2+, and DR2-). There was also little evidence that cross-response inhibition was due to a demonstrable competing response. Experiment 3 found that cross-response inhibition did not depend on having the two responses reinforced by a common outcome. Experiments 4 and 5 then found that cross-response inhibition depended substantially (though not completely) on the transfer target response having been trained in its own FN discrimination. However, Experiment 6 found that inhibition after instrumental extinction (as opposed to FN learning) was still highly response-specific when the transferred-to response had been trained in an FN discrimination. The overall results suggest that the characteristics of inhibition in instrumental extinction and FN learning differ and that transfer of FN inhibition across responses depends at least partly on previous "inhibitability" of the target response. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

用大鼠进行的六个实验检查了在操作特征阴性(FN)辨别中学习到的抑制的性质。先前研究仪器消光而非FN学习的实验结果表明,抑制可能对被抑制的反应非常特异。在实验1中,我们在单独但平行的FN辨别(AR1+、ABR1-、CR2+和CDR2-)中训练杠杆按压和链拉动反应,然后用这两种反应测试两种抑制剂(B和D)。主要感兴趣的是抑制剂抑制他们训练的反应(相同反应抑制)与其他反应(交叉反应抑制)的程度。我们发现交叉反应抑制是稳健的,基本上等于相同反应抑制。实验2复制了这一结果,并证实了FN学习后比差异抑制程序(AR1+、BR1-、CR2+和DR2-)后更强的抑制。也几乎没有证据表明交叉反应抑制是由于明显的竞争反应。实验3发现,交叉反应抑制并不取决于两种反应是否被共同的结果所加强。实验4和5随后发现,交叉反应抑制基本上(尽管不是完全)取决于已经在其自身的FN辨别中训练的转移目标反应。然而,实验6发现,当在FN辨别中训练转移到反应时,仪器消光后的抑制(与FN学习相反)仍然是高度反应特异性的。总体结果表明,仪器消光和FN学习中的抑制特征不同,FN抑制在反应之间的转移至少部分取决于靶反应先前的“抑制性”。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2022 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Inhibition in discriminated operant learning: Tests of response-specificity after feature-negative and extinction learning.","authors":"Michael R Steinfeld, Mark E Bouton","doi":"10.1037/xan0000337","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xan0000337","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Six experiments with rats examined the nature of inhibition learned in an operant feature-negative (FN) discrimination. The results of prior experiments that examined instrumental extinction rather than FN learning suggest that inhibition can be very specific to the inhibited response. In Experiment 1, we trained lever-press and chain-pull responses in separate but parallel FN discriminations (AR1+, ABR1-, CR2+, and CDR2-) and then tested both inhibitors (B and D) with both responses. Of primary interest was the extent to which the inhibitors suppressed the response they were trained with (same-response inhibition) versus the other response (cross-response inhibition). We found that cross-response inhibition was robust and essentially equal to same-response inhibition. Experiment 2 replicated this result and confirmed stronger inhibition after FN learning than after a differential inhibition procedure (AR1+, BR1-, CR2+, and DR2-). There was also little evidence that cross-response inhibition was due to a demonstrable competing response. Experiment 3 found that cross-response inhibition did not depend on having the two responses reinforced by a common outcome. Experiments 4 and 5 then found that cross-response inhibition depended substantially (though not completely) on the transfer target response having been trained in its own FN discrimination. However, Experiment 6 found that inhibition after instrumental extinction (as opposed to FN learning) was still highly response-specific when the transferred-to response had been trained in an FN discrimination. The overall results suggest that the characteristics of inhibition in instrumental extinction and FN learning differ and that transfer of FN inhibition across responses depends at least partly on previous \"inhibitability\" of the target response. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2022-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10215030/pdf/nihms-1896940.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9882713","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Response-independent outcome presentations weaken the instrumental response-outcome association. 反应独立的结果陈述削弱了工具性反应-结果的关联。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-10-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000340
Byron Crimmins, Thomas J Burton, Molly McNulty, Vincent Laurent, Genevra Hart, Bernard W Balleine

The present article explored the fate of previously formed response-outcome associations when the relation between R and O was disrupted by arranging for O to occur independently of R. In each of three experiments response independent outcome delivery selectively reduced the R earning that O. Nevertheless, in Experiments 1 and 2, the R continued to show sensitivity to outcome devaluation, suggesting that the strength of the R-O association was undiminished by this treatment. These experiments used a two-lever, two-outcome design introducing the possibility that devaluation reflected the influence of specific Pavlovian lever-outcome associations. In an attempt to nullify the influence of these incidental Pavlovian cues Experiment 3 used a single bidirectional vertical lever that rats could press left or right for different outcomes. Again, response-independent outcome presentations selectively depressed the performance of the R that delivered the response-independent O. However, in this situation, the response independent O also reduced the sensitivity of R to outcome devaluation; whereas the nondegraded R was sensitive to devaluation, the degraded R was not. We conclude that selective degradation of the instrumental contingency can weaken a specific R-O association while leaving other R-O associations intact. Furthermore, the use of a bidirectional vertical lever in Experiment 3 revealed that unidirectional and spatially separated instrumental manipulanda, such as levers or chains, may produce Pavlovian cues capable of forming incidental associations with the instrumental outcome that can obscure the relative influence of R-O associations after various manipulations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

本文探讨了当R和O之间的关系因安排O独立于R发生而被破坏时,先前形成的反应-结果关联的命运。在三个实验中的每个实验中,反应独立的结果传递都选择性地降低了R的收益O。然而,在实验1和2中,R继续表现出对结果贬值的敏感性,这表明这种处理并未减弱R-O关联的强度。这些实验采用双杠杆、双结果设计,引入贬值反映特定巴甫洛夫杠杆-结果关联影响的可能性。为了消除这些偶然的巴甫洛夫线索的影响,实验3使用了一个单一的双向垂直杠杆,老鼠可以向左或向右按压不同的结果。同样,反应独立的结果呈现选择性地抑制了传递反应独立O的R的表现。然而,在这种情况下,反应独立的O也降低了R对结果贬值的敏感性;未退化的R对货币贬值敏感,而退化的R对货币贬值不敏感。我们的结论是,工具偶然性的选择性降解可以削弱特定的R-O关联,同时保持其他R-O关联完整。此外,实验3中双向垂直杠杆的使用表明,单向和空间分离的工具操作(如杠杆或链条)可能产生巴甫洛夫线索,能够与工具结果形成附带关联,从而模糊各种操作后R-O关联的相对影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Response-independent outcome presentations weaken the instrumental response-outcome association.","authors":"Byron Crimmins,&nbsp;Thomas J Burton,&nbsp;Molly McNulty,&nbsp;Vincent Laurent,&nbsp;Genevra Hart,&nbsp;Bernard W Balleine","doi":"10.1037/xan0000340","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000340","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present article explored the fate of previously formed response-outcome associations when the relation between R and O was disrupted by arranging for O to occur independently of R. In each of three experiments response independent outcome delivery selectively reduced the R earning that O. Nevertheless, in Experiments 1 and 2, the R continued to show sensitivity to outcome devaluation, suggesting that the strength of the R-O association was undiminished by this treatment. These experiments used a two-lever, two-outcome design introducing the possibility that devaluation reflected the influence of specific Pavlovian lever-outcome associations. In an attempt to nullify the influence of these incidental Pavlovian cues Experiment 3 used a single bidirectional vertical lever that rats could press left or right for different outcomes. Again, response-independent outcome presentations selectively depressed the performance of the R that delivered the response-independent O. However, in this situation, the response independent O also reduced the sensitivity of R to outcome devaluation; whereas the nondegraded R was sensitive to devaluation, the degraded R was not. We conclude that selective degradation of the instrumental contingency can weaken a specific R-O association while leaving other R-O associations intact. Furthermore, the use of a bidirectional vertical lever in Experiment 3 revealed that unidirectional and spatially separated instrumental manipulanda, such as levers or chains, may produce Pavlovian cues capable of forming incidental associations with the instrumental outcome that can obscure the relative influence of R-O associations after various manipulations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40657698","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to influence decision criterion in a target detection paradigm. 利用经颅直流电刺激(tDCS)影响目标检测范式中的决策准则。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-10-01 Epub Date: 2022-06-23 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000329
Ciro Civile, Rossy McLaren, Anna Cooke, I P L McLaren

In this article we investigate how a psychological theory used to model perceptual learning and face recognition can be used to predict that anodal tDCS delivered over the DLPFC at Fp3 site (for 10 mins duration at 1.5 mA intensity) modulates the decision criterion, C, (and not d-prime [d']) in a target detection task. In two between-subjects and double-blind experiments (n = 112) we examined the tDCS effects on C when subjects were engaged in a target detection task, in the first instance involving artificial checkerboard stimuli (Experiment 1a), and subsequently face stimuli (Experiment 1b). The results from both experiments revealed that in the sham/control groups a significantly higher C was used when detecting a target pattern (Experiment 1a) or face (Experiment 1b) presented on a familiar rather than a random background. Importantly, anodal tDCS significantly reduced/reversed this difference between C adopted for familiar and random backgrounds in both Experiment 1a and 1b without affecting d'. These results contribute to advance our understanding of the tDCS-induced effects on stimulus representation and to the literature regarding the modulation of C. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

在本文中,我们研究了用于模拟感知学习和人脸识别的心理学理论如何用于预测在Fp3位点(在1.5 mA强度下持续10分钟)通过DLPFC传递的节点tDCS调节目标检测任务中的决策标准C(而不是d'])。在两个受试者间实验和双盲实验(n = 112)中,我们研究了受试者在进行目标检测任务时tDCS对C的影响,第一个实验涉及人工棋盘刺激(实验1a),随后是面部刺激(实验1b)。两个实验的结果都表明,在假组/对照组中,当检测在熟悉背景而不是随机背景上呈现的目标图案(实验1a)或人脸(实验1b)时,使用的C值明显更高。重要的是,在实验1a和1b中,在不影响d'的情况下,阳极tDCS显著减少/逆转了在熟悉背景和随机背景下采用的C之间的差异。这些结果有助于我们进一步了解tdcs诱导的刺激表征效应,以及有关c .调制的文献(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to influence decision criterion in a target detection paradigm.","authors":"Ciro Civile,&nbsp;Rossy McLaren,&nbsp;Anna Cooke,&nbsp;I P L McLaren","doi":"10.1037/xan0000329","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000329","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this article we investigate how a psychological theory used to model perceptual learning and face recognition can be used to predict that anodal tDCS delivered over the DLPFC at Fp3 site (for 10 mins duration at 1.5 mA intensity) modulates the decision criterion, <i>C</i>, (and not <i>d</i>-prime [<i>d</i>']) in a target detection task. In two between-subjects and double-blind experiments (<i>n</i> = 112) we examined the tDCS effects on C when subjects were engaged in a target detection task, in the first instance involving artificial checkerboard stimuli (Experiment 1a), and subsequently face stimuli (Experiment 1b). The results from both experiments revealed that in the sham/control groups a significantly higher <i>C</i> was used when detecting a target pattern (Experiment 1a) or face (Experiment 1b) presented on a familiar rather than a random background. Importantly, anodal tDCS significantly reduced/reversed this difference between <i>C</i> adopted for familiar and random backgrounds in both Experiment 1a and 1b without affecting <i>d</i>'. These results contribute to advance our understanding of the tDCS-induced effects on stimulus representation and to the literature regarding the modulation of <i>C</i>. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40279817","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
The learning curve, revisited. 重新审视学习曲线。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-10-01 Epub Date: 2022-06-30 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000331
Justin A Harris

The nature of the operations that support learning should be evident in the form or shape of the learning curve. For example, models that describe learning as an iterative error-correction process expect that the amount learned on each trial follows a decelerating (negatively inflected) function. That prediction is broadly consistent with the shape of the acquisition and extinction curves derived from mean measures of response strength. However, such evidence can be flawed because group means may not accurately portray the response curves of individual subjects in a conditioning experiment. Moreover, such evidence relies on strong assumptions about the way that what has been learned is expressed in responding. The current work presents a new analytical approach to reveal the rate of change in responding across the course of conditioning in individual animals. When applied to the conditioning and extinction data from a large sample of rats, this analysis confirms that responses are acquired and extinguish gradually and, in both cases, follow a decelerating learning curve. That is, changes in responding are largest at the start of conditioning or extinction and get progressively smaller as responding approaches an asymptote. However, rather than conforming to the specific shape predicted by an error-correction process, the results suggest that the amount learned increases according to a logarithmic function such that responding during conditioning and extinction is proportional to the log of the number of trials. The implications of these findings for models of associative learning are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

支持学习的操作的性质应该在学习曲线的形式或形状中很明显。例如,将学习描述为迭代纠错过程的模型期望每次尝试的学习量遵循减速(负弯曲)函数。这一预测与从响应强度的平均测量中得出的获取和消退曲线的形状大致一致。然而,这样的证据可能是有缺陷的,因为群体方法可能不能准确地描绘条件反射实验中个体受试者的反应曲线。此外,这些证据依赖于对所学知识在回应中表达方式的强烈假设。目前的工作提出了一种新的分析方法来揭示在整个过程中,在个别动物的条件反射反应的变化率。当应用于来自大鼠样本的条件反射和消退数据时,该分析证实了反应是逐渐获得和消退的,并且在这两种情况下都遵循减速学习曲线。也就是说,响应的变化在条件反射或消退开始时最大,并在响应接近渐近线时逐渐变小。然而,结果表明,学习的数量根据对数函数增加,而不是符合错误纠正过程预测的特定形状,这样在条件反射和消除期间的响应与试验次数的对数成正比。讨论了这些发现对联想学习模型的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"The learning curve, revisited.","authors":"Justin A Harris","doi":"10.1037/xan0000331","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000331","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The nature of the operations that support learning should be evident in the form or shape of the learning curve. For example, models that describe learning as an iterative error-correction process expect that the amount learned on each trial follows a decelerating (negatively inflected) function. That prediction is broadly consistent with the shape of the acquisition and extinction curves derived from mean measures of response strength. However, such evidence can be flawed because group means may not accurately portray the response curves of individual subjects in a conditioning experiment. Moreover, such evidence relies on strong assumptions about the way that what has been learned is expressed in responding. The current work presents a new analytical approach to reveal the rate of change in responding across the course of conditioning in individual animals. When applied to the conditioning and extinction data from a large sample of rats, this analysis confirms that responses are acquired and extinguish gradually and, in both cases, follow a decelerating learning curve. That is, changes in responding are largest at the start of conditioning or extinction and get progressively smaller as responding approaches an asymptote. However, rather than conforming to the specific shape predicted by an error-correction process, the results suggest that the amount learned increases according to a logarithmic function such that responding during conditioning and extinction is proportional to the log of the number of trials. The implications of these findings for models of associative learning are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40411352","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Signal detection analysis of contingency assessment: Associative interference and nonreinforcement impact cue-outcome contingency sensitivity, whereas cue density affects bias. 偶然性评估的信号检测分析:联想干扰和非强化影响线索-结果偶然性敏感性,而线索密度影响偏差。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-07-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000334
Jérémie Jozefowiez, Gonzalo P Urcelay, Ralph R Miller

In a signal detection theory approach to associative learning, the perceived (i.e., subjective) contingency between a cue and an outcome is a random variable drawn from a Gaussian distribution. At the end of the sequence, participants report a positive cue-outcome contingency provided the subjective contingency is above some threshold. Some researchers have suggested that the mean of the subjective contingency distributions and the threshold are controlled by different variables. The present data provide empirical support for this claim. In three experiments, participants were exposed to rapid streams of trials at the end of which they had to indicate whether a target outcome O1 was more likely following a target cue X. Interfering treatments were incorporated in some streams to impend participants' ability to identify the objective X-O1 contingency: interference trials (X was paired with an irrelevant outcome O2), nonreinforced trials (X was presented alone), plus control trials (an irrelevant cue W was paired with O2). Overall, both interference and nonreinforced trials impaired participants' sensitivity to the contingencies as measured by signal detection theory's d', but they also enhanced detection of positive contingencies through a cue density effect, with nonreinforced trials being more susceptible to this effect than interference trials. These results are explicable if one assumes interference and nonreinforced trials impact the mean of the associative strength distribution, while the cue density influences the threshold. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

在联合学习的信号检测理论方法中,线索和结果之间的感知(即主观)偶然性是从高斯分布中抽取的随机变量。在序列的最后,参与者报告了一个积极的线索-结果偶然性,前提是主观偶然性高于某个阈值。一些研究者认为主观偶然性分布的均值和阈值是由不同的变量控制的。目前的数据为这一说法提供了实证支持。在三个实验中,参与者被暴露在快速的实验流中,最后他们必须指出目标结果O1是否更有可能跟随目标线索X。干扰处理被纳入一些流中,以影响参与者识别客观X-O1偶然性的能力:干扰试验(X与不相关的结果O2配对),非强化试验(X单独呈现),加上对照试验(不相关的线索W与O2配对)。总体而言,干扰试验和非强化试验都削弱了参与者对偶然事件的敏感性,这是用信号检测理论的d'来衡量的,但它们也通过线索密度效应增强了对积极偶然事件的检测,非强化试验比干扰试验更容易受到这种效应的影响。如果假设干扰和非强化试验影响联想强度分布的平均值,而线索密度影响阈值,那么这些结果是可以解释的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Signal detection analysis of contingency assessment: Associative interference and nonreinforcement impact cue-outcome contingency sensitivity, whereas cue density affects bias.","authors":"Jérémie Jozefowiez,&nbsp;Gonzalo P Urcelay,&nbsp;Ralph R Miller","doi":"10.1037/xan0000334","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000334","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In a signal detection theory approach to associative learning, the perceived (i.e., subjective) contingency between a cue and an outcome is a random variable drawn from a Gaussian distribution. At the end of the sequence, participants report a positive cue-outcome contingency provided the subjective contingency is above some threshold. Some researchers have suggested that the mean of the subjective contingency distributions and the threshold are controlled by different variables. The present data provide empirical support for this claim. In three experiments, participants were exposed to rapid streams of trials at the end of which they had to indicate whether a target outcome O1 was more likely following a target cue X. Interfering treatments were incorporated in some streams to impend participants' ability to identify the objective X-O1 contingency: interference trials (X was paired with an irrelevant outcome O2), nonreinforced trials (X was presented alone), plus control trials (an irrelevant cue W was paired with O2). Overall, both interference and nonreinforced trials impaired participants' sensitivity to the contingencies as measured by signal detection theory's d', but they also enhanced detection of positive contingencies through a cue density effect, with nonreinforced trials being more susceptible to this effect than interference trials. These results are explicable if one assumes interference and nonreinforced trials impact the mean of the associative strength distribution, while the cue density influences the threshold. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10232211/pdf/nihms-1899390.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9731928","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceptual learning after rapidly alternating exposure to taste compounds: Assessment with different indices of generalization. 快速交替暴露于味觉化合物后的知觉学习:不同泛化指数的评估。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-07-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000333
Jesús Sánchez, Dominic M Dwyer, Robert C Honey, Isabel de Brugada

Exposure to two similar stimuli (AX and BX; e.g., two tastes) reduces the extent to which a conditioned response later established to BX generalizes to AX. This example of perceptual learning is more evident when AX and BX are exposed in an alternating manner (AX, BX, AX, BX,…) than when AX and BX occur in separate blocks (e.g., AX, AX,…BX, BX,…). We examined in male rats (N = 126) the impact of rapid alternation to AX and BX on generalization of a taste aversion from BX to AX. Experiment 1 showed that such alternating presentations (with 5-min intervals between AX and BX) reduced generalization relative to blocked exposure; but only as assessed by consumption levels and not by lick cluster size (an index of hedonic reactions). Experiment 1 also showed that the nature of exposure did not affect how A influenced performance to a novel conditioned taste, Y. Experiment 2 replicated the pattern of results involving the different influences of rapidly alternating and blocked exposure on generalization from BX to AX, and showed that this effect was only evident when rats received access to water during the 5-min intervals between AX and BX. These results reinforce parallels between perceptual learning effects in rats and humans, both at empirical and theoretical levels. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

暴露于两种相似的刺激(AX和BX;例如,两种口味)降低了后来建立为BX的条件反应推广到AX的程度。当AX和BX以交替的方式暴露(AX, BX, AX, BX,…)时,这个感知学习的例子比当AX和BX出现在单独的块(例如,AX, AX,…BX, BX,…)时更为明显。我们在雄性大鼠(N = 126)中研究了快速切换到AX和BX对从BX到AX的味觉厌恶的泛化的影响。实验1表明,相对于封闭暴露,这种交替呈现(AX和BX之间间隔5分钟)降低了泛化;但这只是通过消费水平来评估的,而不是通过舔的簇大小(一种享乐反应的指数)来评估的。实验1还表明,暴露的性质并不影响A对新的条件味道y的影响。实验2重复了快速交替暴露和阻断暴露对从BX到AX的泛化的不同影响的结果模式,并表明这种影响仅在大鼠在AX和BX之间的5分钟间隔期间获得水时才明显。这些结果在经验和理论层面上强化了大鼠和人类感知学习效应之间的相似之处。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Perceptual learning after rapidly alternating exposure to taste compounds: Assessment with different indices of generalization.","authors":"Jesús Sánchez,&nbsp;Dominic M Dwyer,&nbsp;Robert C Honey,&nbsp;Isabel de Brugada","doi":"10.1037/xan0000333","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000333","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Exposure to two similar stimuli (AX and BX; e.g., two tastes) reduces the extent to which a conditioned response later established to BX generalizes to AX. This example of perceptual learning is more evident when AX and BX are exposed in an alternating manner (AX, BX, AX, BX,…) than when AX and BX occur in separate blocks (e.g., AX, AX,…BX, BX,…). We examined in male rats (N = 126) the impact of rapid alternation to AX and BX on generalization of a taste aversion from BX to AX. Experiment 1 showed that such alternating presentations (with 5-min intervals between AX and BX) reduced generalization relative to blocked exposure; but only as assessed by consumption levels and not by lick cluster size (an index of hedonic reactions). Experiment 1 also showed that the nature of exposure did not affect how A influenced performance to a novel conditioned taste, Y. Experiment 2 replicated the pattern of results involving the different influences of rapidly alternating and blocked exposure on generalization from BX to AX, and showed that this effect was only evident when rats received access to water during the 5-min intervals between AX and BX. These results reinforce parallels between perceptual learning effects in rats and humans, both at empirical and theoretical levels. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40624473","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Generalization following symmetrical intradimensional discrimination training. 对称内维辨别训练后的泛化。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-07-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000327
David W Ng, Jessica C Lee, Brett K Hayes, Peter F Lovibond

A challenge for generalization models is to specify how excitation generated from a CS+ (i.e., positive evidence) should interact with inhibition from a CS- (i.e., negative evidence) to produce generalized responding. Empirically, many generalization phenomena are consistent with the monotonicity principle, which states that additional positive evidence should increase generalized responding, whereas additional negative evidence should decrease responding. However, a recent study (Lee et al.,, 2019) demonstrated that additional negative evidence can sometimes increase generalization, in direct contrast to animal data and associative accounts of generalization. The current study investigated whether a similar effect could be found in a symmetrical intradimensional discrimination procedure with two sources of negative evidence (CS-s) located on each side of a CS+. In three experiments, we compared generalization along a green-blue dimension between one group of participants who learned that an aqua-colored shape (CS+) predicted an outcome (Single Positive group) with another group who also learned that both a slightly greener and a slightly bluer shape led to no outcome (Double Negative group). Experiments 1A and 1B showed no effect of the additional negative evidence in increasing generalization around the CS+. However, changing a stimulus feature at test (shape) resulted in a higher gradient peak in the Double Negative group relative to the Single Positive group in Experiment 2. Although this result violates the monotonicity principle, an extended version of Blough's (1975) model applying cue competition to multiple stimulus dimensions (i.e., shape and color) successfully replicated the group differences. Our results suggest that associative mechanisms can account for some instances in which negative evidence increases generalization. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

泛化模型面临的一个挑战是,具体说明由CS+(即积极证据)产生的激励如何与CS-(即消极证据)产生的抑制相互作用,以产生泛化反应。从经验上看,许多泛化现象符合单调性原则,即额外的积极证据应该增加泛化反应,而额外的消极证据应该减少反应。然而,最近的一项研究(Lee et al., 2019)表明,额外的负面证据有时会增加泛化,这与动物数据和泛化的相关描述直接相反。目前的研究调查了是否可以在对称的内维歧视过程中发现类似的效应,其中两个负面证据来源(CS-s)位于CS+的每一侧。在三个实验中,我们比较了一组参与者在绿-蓝维度上的泛化,一组参与者学习了水色形状(CS+)预测结果(单一积极组),另一组参与者也学习了稍微绿色和稍微蓝色的形状导致没有结果(双重消极组)。实验1A和1B没有发现额外的负面证据对CS+周围的泛化增加有影响。然而,在实验2中,在测试中改变刺激特征(形状)导致双负组相对于单正组的梯度峰值更高。尽管这一结果违反了单调性原则,但Blough(1975)模型的扩展版本将线索竞争应用于多个刺激维度(即形状和颜色),成功地复制了群体差异。我们的研究结果表明,联想机制可以解释某些情况下,负面证据增加概括。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Generalization following symmetrical intradimensional discrimination training.","authors":"David W Ng,&nbsp;Jessica C Lee,&nbsp;Brett K Hayes,&nbsp;Peter F Lovibond","doi":"10.1037/xan0000327","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000327","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A challenge for generalization models is to specify how excitation generated from a CS+ (i.e., positive evidence) should interact with inhibition from a CS- (i.e., negative evidence) to produce generalized responding. Empirically, many generalization phenomena are consistent with the monotonicity principle, which states that additional positive evidence should increase generalized responding, whereas additional negative evidence should decrease responding. However, a recent study (Lee et al.,, 2019) demonstrated that additional negative evidence can sometimes increase generalization, in direct contrast to animal data and associative accounts of generalization. The current study investigated whether a similar effect could be found in a symmetrical intradimensional discrimination procedure with two sources of negative evidence (CS-s) located on each side of a CS+. In three experiments, we compared generalization along a green-blue dimension between one group of participants who learned that an aqua-colored shape (CS+) predicted an outcome (Single Positive group) with another group who also learned that both a slightly greener and a slightly bluer shape led to no outcome (Double Negative group). Experiments 1A and 1B showed no effect of the additional negative evidence in increasing generalization around the CS+. However, changing a stimulus feature at test (shape) resulted in a higher gradient peak in the Double Negative group relative to the Single Positive group in Experiment 2. Although this result violates the monotonicity principle, an extended version of Blough's (1975) model applying cue competition to multiple stimulus dimensions (i.e., shape and color) successfully replicated the group differences. Our results suggest that associative mechanisms can account for some instances in which negative evidence increases generalization. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40624474","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A response function that maps associative strengths to probabilities. 将联想强度映射到概率的响应函数。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-07-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000322
Stefano Ghirlanda

Bridging associative and normative theories of animal learning, I show that an associative system can behave as if performing probabilistic inference by using the function f(V) = 1 - e-cV to transform associative strengths (V) into response probabilities. For example, using this function, an associative system can respond normatively to a compound stimulus AB, given previous separate experiences with the components A and B. The CR probability formulae that result from the proposed function have a normative interpretation in terms of statistical decision theory. The formulae also suggest a normative interpretation of stimulus generalization as a heuristic to infer whether different stimuli are likely to convey redundant or independent information about reinforcement. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

通过连接动物学习的关联理论和规范理论,我展示了一个关联系统可以表现得就像通过使用函数f(V) = 1 - e-cV将关联强度(V)转换为响应概率来进行概率推理一样。例如,使用这个函数,一个关联系统可以对复合刺激AB做出规范的响应,给定之前对组件a和b的单独经验。从所提出的函数中得出的CR概率公式在统计决策理论方面具有规范的解释。该公式还提出了刺激泛化的规范解释,作为一种启发式方法来推断不同的刺激是否可能传达有关强化的冗余或独立信息。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"A response function that maps associative strengths to probabilities.","authors":"Stefano Ghirlanda","doi":"10.1037/xan0000322","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000322","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Bridging associative and normative theories of animal learning, I show that an associative system can behave as if performing probabilistic inference by using the function <i>f</i>(<i>V</i>) = 1 - <i>e<sup>-cV</sup></i> to transform associative strengths (<i>V</i>) into response probabilities. For example, using this function, an associative system can respond normatively to a compound stimulus <i>AB</i>, given previous separate experiences with the components <i>A</i> and <i>B</i>. The CR probability formulae that result from the proposed function have a normative interpretation in terms of statistical decision theory. The formulae also suggest a normative interpretation of stimulus generalization as a heuristic to infer whether different stimuli are likely to convey redundant or independent information about reinforcement. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54259,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9324335","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1