首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Comparative Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
The roles of social information, asocial information, and initial bias in nest-building decisions. 社会信息、非社会信息和初始偏差在筑巢决策中的作用。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1037/com0000374
Benjamin A Whittaker, Gopika Balasubramanian, Andrés Camacho-Alpízar, Connor T Lambert, Lauren M Guillette

Animals can use asocial (e.g., environmental cues) or social (e.g., conspecific behavior) information when making decisions. We investigated decisions made by zebra finches when asocial and social sources conveyed agreeing or conflicting information, and assessed the influence of initial bias on decision making. Finches completed an initial preference test ranking preference for three colors of nest-building material. Birds in the agree group (n = 14) then observed demonstrators build nests using nonpreferred color material (social information) that matched the environment color (asocial information). Birds in the conflict group (n = 15) observed demonstrators build nests with nonpreferred color material that did not match the cage environment (another nonpreferred color). A final preference test assessed any changes in color preference. The agree group reduced average preference for their initially preferred color, but did not significantly increase average preference for the asocial/social colors. The conflict group also reduced average preference for the initially preferred color and also increased preference for the socially demonstrated color. Observers with stronger initial bias were less likely to choose the socially demonstrated color than observers with weaker initial bias. This shows that social information informs nest-building decisions, even when in conflict with asocial information. However, bias influences social information use and adds nuance to how different individuals use information when making decisions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

动物在做决定时可以使用非社会信息(如环境线索)或社会信息(如同种动物的行为)。我们研究了斑马雀在非社会信息源和社会信息源传递一致或冲突信息时做出的决策,并评估了初始偏差对决策的影响。斑马雀完成了一项初始偏好测试,对三种颜色的筑巢材料进行了偏好排序。然后,同意组的鸟类(n = 14)观察示范者使用与环境颜色(非社会信息)相匹配的非偏好颜色材料(社会信息)筑巢。冲突组(n = 15)的鸟儿观察示范者使用与笼子环境不匹配的非偏好颜色材料(另一种非偏好颜色)筑巢。最后的偏好测试评估了颜色偏好的变化。同意组降低了对其最初偏好颜色的平均偏好,但对非社交/社交颜色的平均偏好并没有显著增加。冲突组也降低了对最初偏好颜色的平均偏好,但也增加了对社会表现颜色的偏好。与初始偏好较弱的观察者相比,初始偏好较强的观察者不太可能选择社会展示的颜色。这表明,即使社会信息与非社会信息相冲突,社会信息也会影响筑巢决策。然而,偏见会影响社会信息的使用,并使不同个体在决策时使用信息的方式更加微妙。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The roles of social information, asocial information, and initial bias in nest-building decisions.","authors":"Benjamin A Whittaker, Gopika Balasubramanian, Andrés Camacho-Alpízar, Connor T Lambert, Lauren M Guillette","doi":"10.1037/com0000374","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000374","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Animals can use asocial (e.g., environmental cues) or social (e.g., conspecific behavior) information when making decisions. We investigated decisions made by zebra finches when asocial and social sources conveyed agreeing or conflicting information, and assessed the influence of initial bias on decision making. Finches completed an initial preference test ranking preference for three colors of nest-building material. Birds in the agree group (<i>n</i> = 14) then observed demonstrators build nests using nonpreferred color material (social information) that matched the environment color (asocial information). Birds in the conflict group (<i>n</i> = 15) observed demonstrators build nests with nonpreferred color material that did not match the cage environment (another nonpreferred color). A final preference test assessed any changes in color preference. The agree group reduced average preference for their initially preferred color, but did not significantly increase average preference for the asocial/social colors. The conflict group also reduced average preference for the initially preferred color and also increased preference for the socially demonstrated color. Observers with stronger initial bias were less likely to choose the socially demonstrated color than observers with weaker initial bias. This shows that social information informs nest-building decisions, even when in conflict with asocial information. However, bias influences social information use and adds nuance to how different individuals use information when making decisions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"190-202"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139708579","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Still face in pet dogs (Canis familiaris). 宠物狗(Canis familiaris)的静止面部。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-15 DOI: 10.1037/com0000371
Molly Byrne, Kayla Sawyer, Angie Johnston

Dogs are able to cooperate in reciprocal exchange with humans but little is known about the extent of these abilities (Range & Virányi, 2015). In the Still Face paradigm, infants reply to a sudden nonreciprocal facial expression with gaze aversion and an increase in re-engagement and distress behaviors (E. Tronick et al., 1978). We directly adapted this method; the dog's owner talked to the dog, then abruptly switched to a still, neutral face, maintaining eye contact. In Study 1 (N = 20), we found that dogs showed a significant decrease in the amount of looking at the owner in the Still Face phase, paralleling the results found in gaze aversion in infants, and they performed fewer pawing and vocalizations toward the person in the Still Face phase. In Study 2 (N = 60), we included one condition of continuous physical contact, and one condition that was a direct replication of the initial study without physical contact. Similar to human infants, we found a significant decrease in looking from the Interaction phase to the Still Face phase. However, in contrast to human infants, re-engagement and stress behaviors were higher in the Interaction phase than the Still Face phase. Looking and re-engagement behaviors differed based on the condition, with a smaller difference between phases in the Petting condition. These results suggest that dogs are capable of perceiving these small changes in human affect. However, unlike human infants, dogs seem to have greater expectations about physical interactions than verbal interactions, as they reacted more strongly to an Interaction phase without physical contact than the Still Face. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

狗能够与人类进行互惠交流合作,但人们对这种能力的程度知之甚少(Range & Virányi, 2015)。在 "静止的脸 "范式中,婴儿会对突然出现的非互惠面部表情做出反应,表现为凝视厌恶以及重新参与和苦恼行为的增加(E. Tronick 等人,1978 年)。我们直接采用了这种方法;狗的主人与狗交谈,然后突然切换到静止的中性面孔,并保持目光接触。在研究 1(N = 20)中,我们发现狗在静止脸部阶段看主人的次数明显减少,这与在婴儿凝视厌恶中发现的结果类似,而且它们在静止脸部阶段对人的爪子和发声次数也减少了。在研究 2(N = 60)中,我们加入了一个持续身体接触的条件和一个直接复制初始研究的无身体接触条件。与人类婴儿类似,我们发现从 "互动 "阶段到 "静止脸部 "阶段,婴儿看人的次数明显减少。然而,与人类婴儿不同的是,互动阶段的重新参与和压力行为高于静止阶段。注视和重新参与行为因条件而异,在抚摸条件下,不同阶段之间的差异较小。这些结果表明,狗能够感知人类情感的这些微小变化。然而,与人类婴儿不同的是,狗对肢体互动的期望似乎大于语言互动,因为它们对没有肢体接触的互动阶段的反应比静止的脸部反应更强烈。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Still face in pet dogs (Canis familiaris).","authors":"Molly Byrne, Kayla Sawyer, Angie Johnston","doi":"10.1037/com0000371","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000371","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Dogs are able to cooperate in reciprocal exchange with humans but little is known about the extent of these abilities (Range & Virányi, 2015). In the Still Face paradigm, infants reply to a sudden nonreciprocal facial expression with gaze aversion and an increase in re-engagement and distress behaviors (E. Tronick et al., 1978). We directly adapted this method; the dog's owner talked to the dog, then abruptly switched to a still, neutral face, maintaining eye contact. In Study 1 (<i>N</i> = 20), we found that dogs showed a significant decrease in the amount of looking at the owner in the Still Face phase, paralleling the results found in gaze aversion in infants, and they performed fewer pawing and vocalizations toward the person in the Still Face phase. In Study 2 (<i>N</i> = 60), we included one condition of continuous physical contact, and one condition that was a direct replication of the initial study without physical contact. Similar to human infants, we found a significant decrease in looking from the Interaction phase to the Still Face phase. However, in contrast to human infants, re-engagement and stress behaviors were higher in the Interaction phase than the Still Face phase. Looking and re-engagement behaviors differed based on the condition, with a smaller difference between phases in the Petting condition. These results suggest that dogs are capable of perceiving these small changes in human affect. However, unlike human infants, dogs seem to have greater expectations about physical interactions than verbal interactions, as they reacted more strongly to an Interaction phase without physical contact than the Still Face. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"157-169"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139736789","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) with better task-based delay of gratification skills are rated as less impulsive, more agreeable, and smarter. 任务型延迟满足技能较好的黑猩猩(Pan troglodytes)被评为不那么冲动、更合群、更聪明。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-26 DOI: 10.1037/com0000373
William D Hopkins, Chelsea M Cox, Robert D Latzman, Michael J Beran

Delay of gratification and inhibitory control are generally considered measures of self-control. In humans, individual differences in measures of self-control are associated with a host of behavioral, neurological, cognitive, and health-related outcomes. Self-control is not unique to humans and has been demonstrated in a variety of nonhuman species using a variety of paradigms. In this study, the effect of sex and age on delay of gratification performance, as measured by the hybrid delay task, was tested in a sample of 88 chimpanzees. Additionally, whether individual differences in hybrid delay task performance were associated with different aspects of personality was examined in this study. Contrary to reports in human subjects, geriatric male chimpanzees were found to perform more efficiently than adult males, while no age differences were found between geriatric and adult females. Indeed, delay of gratification efficiency was positively associated with age in males and negatively associated with age in females. Chimpanzees that performed more efficiently on the hybrid delay task were also found to be rated as more intelligent, more extraverted, and less impulsive. These findings suggest that objective measures of efficiency in delay of gratification tasks are associated with different dimensions of personality, which have some overlapping construct validity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

延迟满足和抑制控制通常被认为是自我控制的衡量标准。在人类中,自我控制能力的个体差异与一系列行为、神经、认知和健康相关结果有关。自我控制并非人类所独有,它已在多种非人类物种中通过各种范式得到证实。本研究以 88 只黑猩猩为样本,通过混合延迟任务测试了性别和年龄对延迟满足表现的影响。此外,本研究还考察了混合延迟任务表现的个体差异是否与不同的性格有关。与人类受试者的报告相反,研究发现老年雄性黑猩猩比成年雄性黑猩猩的表现更有效率,而老年雌性黑猩猩和成年雌性黑猩猩之间没有年龄差异。事实上,延迟满足的效率与雄性黑猩猩的年龄呈正相关,而与雌性黑猩猩的年龄呈负相关。在混合延迟任务中表现更有效率的黑猩猩还被认为更聪明、更外向和更不冲动。这些研究结果表明,对延迟满足任务效率的客观测量与人格的不同维度相关,而这些维度具有一些重叠的建构效度。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) with better task-based delay of gratification skills are rated as less impulsive, more agreeable, and smarter.","authors":"William D Hopkins, Chelsea M Cox, Robert D Latzman, Michael J Beran","doi":"10.1037/com0000373","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000373","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Delay of gratification and inhibitory control are generally considered measures of self-control. In humans, individual differences in measures of self-control are associated with a host of behavioral, neurological, cognitive, and health-related outcomes. Self-control is not unique to humans and has been demonstrated in a variety of nonhuman species using a variety of paradigms. In this study, the effect of sex and age on delay of gratification performance, as measured by the hybrid delay task, was tested in a sample of 88 chimpanzees. Additionally, whether individual differences in hybrid delay task performance were associated with different aspects of personality was examined in this study. Contrary to reports in human subjects, geriatric male chimpanzees were found to perform more efficiently than adult males, while no age differences were found between geriatric and adult females. Indeed, delay of gratification efficiency was positively associated with age in males and negatively associated with age in females. Chimpanzees that performed more efficiently on the hybrid delay task were also found to be rated as more intelligent, more extraverted, and less impulsive. These findings suggest that objective measures of efficiency in delay of gratification tasks are associated with different dimensions of personality, which have some overlapping construct validity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"203-210"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139974688","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual and acoustic exploratory behaviors toward novel stimuli in Antillean manatees (Trichechus manatus manatus) under human care. 人类照料下的安的列斯海牛(Trichechus manatus manatus)对新刺激的视觉和听觉探索行为。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-14 DOI: 10.1037/com0000360
Aviva Charles, Yann Henaut, Michel Saint-Jalme, Baptiste Mulot, Alexis Lecu, Fabienne Delfour

Exploratory behaviors describe the actions performed by an animal to obtain information on an object, environment, or individual by using its different senses. Exploration is described in some marine mammals, but not yet in manatees. Our study investigated behavioral and acoustic responses of two groups of Antillean manatees (N = 12 and N = 4) housed in zoological parks toward various stimuli involving three sensory modalities: visual, tactile, and auditory. Simultaneous audio and video recordings were collected during three periods of time (i.e., before, during, and after the presentation of all stimuli). Behaviors related to interest, social behaviors, the number and type of calls produced, and their frequency and duration were recorded and analyzed. Manatees reacted more to submerged stimuli than to out-of-water and sound stimuli, with an increase in approach, social contacts, and number of vocalizations. The proportion of squeaks and squeals call types also varied according to stimuli, and call entropy and F0 range varied according to periods. Our results suggest that manatees display sensory preferences when exploring stimuli, with more interest in manipulable stimuli, supporting the importance of their somatic perception. We highlight the need for particular enrichment programs (i.e., involving submerged objects) in zoological facilities. By displaying social contacts and by producing vocalizations, manatees communicate information such as their motivational state. The increase in call rate, harsh calls, and entropy values could be valid indicators of heightened arousal. We encourage further studies to associate acoustic recordings with ethological data collection to increase the understanding of manatees' behaviors andperception. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

探索行为描述的是动物通过不同的感官获取物体、环境或个体信息的行为。一些海洋哺乳动物有探索行为,但海牛还没有。我们的研究调查了饲养在动物园的两组安的列斯海牛(12 头和 4 头)对涉及视觉、触觉和听觉三种感官模式的各种刺激的行为和声音反应。在三个时间段(即所有刺激物出现之前、期间和之后)同时采集音频和视频记录。记录并分析了与兴趣、社交行为、叫声的数量和类型、频率和持续时间有关的行为。与出水和声音刺激相比,海牛对水下刺激的反应更大,接近、社交接触和发声次数都有所增加。吱吱声和尖叫声叫声类型的比例也因刺激而异,叫声熵和 F0 范围也因时期而异。我们的研究结果表明,海牛在探索刺激物时表现出感官偏好,对可操作的刺激物更感兴趣,这支持了其躯体感知的重要性。我们强调了在动物园设施中开展特定强化项目(即涉及水下物体)的必要性。海牛通过显示社会接触和发出声音来交流信息,如它们的动机状态。叫声频率的增加、刺耳的叫声和熵值都可能是海牛亢奋的有效指标。我们鼓励进一步的研究,将声学记录与人种学数据收集结合起来,以增加对海牛行为和感知的了解。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Visual and acoustic exploratory behaviors toward novel stimuli in Antillean manatees (Trichechus manatus manatus) under human care.","authors":"Aviva Charles, Yann Henaut, Michel Saint-Jalme, Baptiste Mulot, Alexis Lecu, Fabienne Delfour","doi":"10.1037/com0000360","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000360","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Exploratory behaviors describe the actions performed by an animal to obtain information on an object, environment, or individual by using its different senses. Exploration is described in some marine mammals, but not yet in manatees. Our study investigated behavioral and acoustic responses of two groups of Antillean manatees (<i>N</i> = 12 and <i>N</i> = 4) housed in zoological parks toward various stimuli involving three sensory modalities: visual, tactile, and auditory. Simultaneous audio and video recordings were collected during three periods of time (i.e., before, during, and after the presentation of all stimuli). Behaviors related to interest, social behaviors, the number and type of calls produced, and their frequency and duration were recorded and analyzed. Manatees reacted more to submerged stimuli than to out-of-water and sound stimuli, with an increase in approach, social contacts, and number of vocalizations. The proportion of squeaks and squeals call types also varied according to stimuli, and call entropy and F0 range varied according to periods. Our results suggest that manatees display sensory preferences when exploring stimuli, with more interest in manipulable stimuli, supporting the importance of their somatic perception. We highlight the need for particular enrichment programs (i.e., involving submerged objects) in zoological facilities. By displaying social contacts and by producing vocalizations, manatees communicate information such as their motivational state. The increase in call rate, harsh calls, and entropy values could be valid indicators of heightened arousal. We encourage further studies to associate acoustic recordings with ethological data collection to increase the understanding of manatees' behaviors andperception. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"118-129"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138813215","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Humans' (Homo sapiens), capuchin monkeys' (Sapajus [Cebus] apella), and rhesus macaques' (Macaca mulatta) size judgments shift when stimuli change in frequency. 当刺激频率发生变化时,人类(智人)、卷尾猴(Sapajus[Cebus]apella)和恒河猴(Macaca mulatta)对体型的判断会发生变化。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-23 DOI: 10.1037/com0000365
Sierra M V Simmons, Sarah F Brosnan

When making decisions, humans often strive to uphold objective, absolute standards, such as about what is small versus large, blue versus purple, or unfair versus fair, suggesting that our judgments should not be swayed by extraneous factors such as the sequence or frequency of events to be judged. Yet in previous research, when some items (e.g., threatening faces) became less frequent, humans responded by expanding their concept (of "threatening") to include more ambiguous stimuli. We assessed the origins of this perceptual frequency bias by testing 25 capuchins, seven rhesus monkeys, and 102 humans on a computer task in which they had to classify one circle at a time (pulled from a continuum of 50 circle sizes) as either small or large. Small and large circles initially appeared with equal probability but over time small circles either became less frequent, more frequent, or did not change in frequency. All three species showed changes in judgment, but contrary to predictions, they contracted, rather than expanded, their size judgments of the less frequent category. In other words, when small circles became rare, participants were more likely to judge ambiguous circles sizes as large (and vice versa). This may have been due to the immediate explicit feedback, as has recently been found in humans, and we consider possible mechanisms driving our participants' responses. These results suggest that humans' difficulties in maintaining absolute standards are shared with other animals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在做出决定时,人类通常努力维护客观、绝对的标准,例如关于什么是小与大、蓝色与紫色、不公平与公平,这表明我们的判断不应受到外部因素的影响,例如要判断的事件的顺序或频率。然而,在之前的研究中,当一些项目(例如威胁性面孔)变得不那么频繁时,人类的反应是扩大他们的概念(“威胁”),包括更模糊的刺激。我们通过在一项计算机任务中测试25只卷尾猴、7只恒河猴和102个人来评估这种感知频率偏差的起源,在这项任务中,他们必须一次将一个圆圈(从50个圆圈大小的连续体中提取)分为小圆圈或大圆圈。小圆圈和大圆圈最初出现的概率相等,但随着时间的推移,小圆圈要么变得不那么频繁,要么变得更频繁,要么频率没有变化。这三个物种的判断都发生了变化,但与预测相反,它们缩小而不是扩大了对不太常见类别的大小判断。换句话说,当小圆圈变得罕见时,参与者更有可能将模糊的圆圈大小判断为大(反之亦然)。这可能是由于直接的明确反馈,正如最近在人类身上发现的那样,我们考虑了驱动参与者反应的可能机制。这些结果表明,人类在保持绝对标准方面的困难与其他动物一样。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Humans' (Homo sapiens), capuchin monkeys' (Sapajus [Cebus] apella), and rhesus macaques' (Macaca mulatta) size judgments shift when stimuli change in frequency.","authors":"Sierra M V Simmons, Sarah F Brosnan","doi":"10.1037/com0000365","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000365","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When making decisions, humans often strive to uphold objective, absolute standards, such as about what is small versus large, blue versus purple, or unfair versus fair, suggesting that our judgments should not be swayed by extraneous factors such as the sequence or frequency of events to be judged. Yet in previous research, when some items (e.g., threatening faces) became less frequent, humans responded by expanding their concept (of \"threatening\") to include more ambiguous stimuli. We assessed the origins of this perceptual frequency bias by testing 25 capuchins, seven rhesus monkeys, and 102 humans on a computer task in which they had to classify one circle at a time (pulled from a continuum of 50 circle sizes) as either small or large. Small and large circles initially appeared with equal probability but over time small circles either became less frequent, more frequent, or did not change in frequency. All three species showed changes in judgment, but contrary to predictions, they contracted, rather than expanded, their size judgments of the less frequent category. In other words, when small circles became rare, participants were more likely to judge ambiguous circles sizes as large (and vice versa). This may have been due to the immediate explicit feedback, as has recently been found in humans, and we consider possible mechanisms driving our participants' responses. These results suggest that humans' difficulties in maintaining absolute standards are shared with other animals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"99-107"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49694100","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pitch affects human (Homo sapiens) perception of emotional arousal from diverse animal calls. 音高会影响人类(智人)对不同动物叫声的情感唤起的感知。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-23 DOI: 10.1037/com0000366
Jay W Schwartz, Kayleigh H Pierson, Alexander K Reece

A growing body of research demonstrates that humans can accurately perceive the emotional states of animals solely by listening to their calls, highlighting shared evolutionary ancestry. Yet, the cognitive and perceptual mechanisms underlying heterospecific emotion perception have remained open to investigation. One hypothesis is that humans rely on simple acoustic heuristics to make such judgments, for example, perceiving higher-pitched calls as reflecting heightened emotional arousal (the "pitch rule"). This could lead to accurate judgments of emotion since in most mammals, as in humans, vocal fundamental frequency (the acoustic determinant of the pitch percept) does objectively correlate with emotional arousal. In the present study, we used digital pitch manipulation to create pairs of animal calls that were perceptually identical except for pitch, and we measured human perceptions of the caller's emotional arousal using an online survey. Calls of six phylogenetically diverse species were included as stimuli. Participants attributed slightly but statistically significantly higher arousal to higher-pitched versions of the same calls. Variation in application of the pitch rule across species was not well explained by familiarity, and prior experience with cats did not significantly predict sensitivity to pitch in cat vocalizations. Cross-species variation also did not align with phylogenetic distance from humans, or the hypothetical usefulness of pitch for making accurate judgments. Thus, the pitch rule may be a "mammalomorphic" heuristic leading to accurate emotion judgments in some taxa and call types and erroneous judgments in others, depending in part on phylogenetic distance and the mechanisms of call production. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

越来越多的研究表明,人类仅通过倾听动物的叫声就能准确感知动物的情绪状态,这突出了共同的进化祖先。然而,异性情感感知背后的认知和感知机制仍有待研究。一种假设是,人类依靠简单的声学启发式来做出这样的判断,例如,将音调更高的叫声感知为反映了情绪唤醒的增强(“音调规则”)。这可能会导致对情绪的准确判断,因为在大多数哺乳动物中,就像在人类中一样,发声基频(音高感知的声学决定因素)确实与情绪唤醒客观相关。在本研究中,我们使用数字音高操纵来创建除音高外在感知上完全相同的动物叫声对,并通过在线调查测量了人类对来电者情绪唤醒的感知。六个系统发育不同物种的叫声被包括在内作为刺激。参与者将轻微但统计上显著较高的唤醒归因于相同叫声的高音版本。不同物种之间音高规则应用的变化并不能很好地用熟悉度来解释,之前与猫相处的经验也不能显著预测猫发声对音高的敏感性。跨物种变异也不符合与人类的系统发育距离,也不符合音高对做出准确判断的假设有用性。因此,音高规则可能是一种“长毛象”启发式,导致某些分类群和叫声类型的情绪判断准确,而另一些分类群和声调类型的判断错误,这在一定程度上取决于系统发育距离和叫声产生的机制。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Pitch affects human (Homo sapiens) perception of emotional arousal from diverse animal calls.","authors":"Jay W Schwartz, Kayleigh H Pierson, Alexander K Reece","doi":"10.1037/com0000366","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000366","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A growing body of research demonstrates that humans can accurately perceive the emotional states of animals solely by listening to their calls, highlighting shared evolutionary ancestry. Yet, the cognitive and perceptual mechanisms underlying heterospecific emotion perception have remained open to investigation. One hypothesis is that humans rely on simple acoustic heuristics to make such judgments, for example, perceiving higher-pitched calls as reflecting heightened emotional arousal (the \"pitch rule\"). This could lead to accurate judgments of emotion since in most mammals, as in humans, vocal fundamental frequency (the acoustic determinant of the pitch percept) does objectively correlate with emotional arousal. In the present study, we used digital pitch manipulation to create pairs of animal calls that were perceptually identical except for pitch, and we measured human perceptions of the caller's emotional arousal using an online survey. Calls of six phylogenetically diverse species were included as stimuli. Participants attributed slightly but statistically significantly higher arousal to higher-pitched versions of the same calls. Variation in application of the pitch rule across species was not well explained by familiarity, and prior experience with cats did not significantly predict sensitivity to pitch in cat vocalizations. Cross-species variation also did not align with phylogenetic distance from humans, or the hypothetical usefulness of pitch for making accurate judgments. Thus, the pitch rule may be a \"mammalomorphic\" heuristic leading to accurate emotion judgments in some taxa and call types and erroneous judgments in others, depending in part on phylogenetic distance and the mechanisms of call production. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"80-87"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49694101","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of goal-landmark distance on overshadowing: A replication in humans (Homo sapiens) of Goodyear and Kamil (2004). 目标里程碑距离对遮蔽的影响:Goodyear和Kamil(2004)在人类(智人)中的复制。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-21 DOI: 10.1037/com0000362
Estibaliz Herrera, Joe M Austen, Gonzalo P Urcelay

Goodyear and Kamil (2004) assessed the ability of Clark's nutcrackers to find buried food based on a cross-shaped array of landmarks at different distances from the goal. Their findings suggested that proximal landmarks overshadowed learning about distal landmarks, and this was attenuated when assessing the effect of distal landmarks on learning about proximal landmarks. In this study, we aimed to replicate their findings in human spatial navigation by using a virtual environment. Three groups of participants were trained in an open environment featuring orientation cues, and they had to find a hidden goal with reference to four landmarks that were arranged in the shape of a cross and placed at different distances from the goal. Two of the four landmark distances were common across all three groups to allow a comparison of the extent of overshadowing under comparable conditions. Following training, all participants were tested with each of the four landmarks individually. Consistent with the results in birds, we observed better performance in the groups with more distal landmarks, suggesting that overshadowing was greater in the groups with closer landmarks and thus dependent on the spatial distance between the landmarks and the goal. Landmarks near the goal more effectively overshadowed landmarks far from the goal. A second experiment, in which landmarks and orientation cues were misaligned in order to prevent the use of a straightforward solution to the task, replicated the results. The results are discussed in terms of a modification of Pearce's configural model. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

Goodyear和Kamil(2004)根据距离球门不同距离的十字形地标阵列,评估了克拉克胡桃夹子寻找埋藏食物的能力。他们的研究结果表明,近端标志物盖过了对远端标志物的学习,在评估远端标志物对近端标志性学习的影响时,这一点有所减弱。在这项研究中,我们旨在通过使用虚拟环境在人类空间导航中复制他们的发现。三组参与者在一个以方位线索为特征的开放环境中接受训练,他们必须参照四个排列成十字架形状并与目标相距不同距离的地标找到一个隐藏的目标。四个标志距离中的两个在所有三组中都是常见的,以便比较在可比较条件下的遮蔽程度。培训结束后,所有参与者分别接受了四个里程碑中的每一个的测试。与鸟类的研究结果一致,我们观察到具有更多远端标志的组表现更好,这表明具有较近标志的组阴影更大,因此取决于标志和目标之间的空间距离。球门附近的地标更有效地盖过了远离球门的地标。第二个实验复制了结果,在该实验中,为了防止使用简单的解决方案来完成任务,地标和方向线索没有对齐。对皮尔斯的构形模型进行了修正,对结果进行了讨论。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The effects of goal-landmark distance on overshadowing: A replication in humans (Homo sapiens) of Goodyear and Kamil (2004).","authors":"Estibaliz Herrera, Joe M Austen, Gonzalo P Urcelay","doi":"10.1037/com0000362","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000362","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Goodyear and Kamil (2004) assessed the ability of Clark's nutcrackers to find buried food based on a cross-shaped array of landmarks at different distances from the goal. Their findings suggested that proximal landmarks overshadowed learning about distal landmarks, and this was attenuated when assessing the effect of distal landmarks on learning about proximal landmarks. In this study, we aimed to replicate their findings in human spatial navigation by using a virtual environment. Three groups of participants were trained in an open environment featuring orientation cues, and they had to find a hidden goal with reference to four landmarks that were arranged in the shape of a cross and placed at different distances from the goal. Two of the four landmark distances were common across all three groups to allow a comparison of the extent of overshadowing under comparable conditions. Following training, all participants were tested with each of the four landmarks individually. Consistent with the results in birds, we observed better performance in the groups with more distal landmarks, suggesting that overshadowing was greater in the groups with closer landmarks and thus dependent on the spatial distance between the landmarks and the goal. Landmarks near the goal more effectively overshadowed landmarks far from the goal. A second experiment, in which landmarks and orientation cues were misaligned in order to prevent the use of a straightforward solution to the task, replicated the results. The results are discussed in terms of a modification of Pearce's configural model. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"108-117"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41179420","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do standard behavioral assays predict foraging behavior of individual Black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) in response to a predator model or calls? 标准的行为分析是否可以预测个体黑帽山雀(Poecile atricapillo)对捕食者模型或叫声的觅食行为?
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/com0000364
Jodilyn R Jenkins, Ian G Pope, Madeline A Dykstra, Jennifer J Jenkins, Cheryl R Dykstra, Kelly A Williams

Black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) and other species that feed at bird feeders balance the benefit of easy foraging with the added risk of predation. Individual birds respond differently to risky situations, and these differences have been attributed to the birds' personalities, which researchers commonly assess with an "open-field" behavioral assay. However, these behavioral assays in birds have not been compared to behavior in the wild in the context of foraging in the presence of a predator (i.e., risk-taking behavior). We color-banded chickadees in a wild population and conducted behavioral assays in the field. We later used foraging trials to investigate these color-banded individuals' responses to a predator (Cooper's hawk, Accipiter cooperii) model or a series of Cooper's hawk calls. We found that foraging black-capped chickadees responded more strongly to the presence of a predator model than to predator calls. Individual birds differed in their responses, and the behavioral assays (activity and exploration) predicted individual behavior in the wild during the foraging experiments. Activity and exploration assay scores were only weakly related, suggesting these two assays represent different traits. Both highly active birds and fast explorers exhibited some reluctance to visit the feeder (either reduced number of visits or greater latency to visit) when the predator model was present, a relationship that was somewhat unexpected. Our results suggest that standard behavioral assays predict behavior in the wild, but care should be taken when generalizing among species and studies. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

黑帽山雀(Poecile atricapillo)和其他在鸟类饲养器中觅食的物种平衡了容易觅食的好处和增加的捕食风险。鸟类个体对危险情况的反应不同,这些差异被归因于鸟类的性格,研究人员通常通过“开放领域”行为分析来评估这些性格。然而,鸟类的这些行为测定尚未与捕食者存在下觅食的野外行为(即冒险行为)进行比较。我们对野生种群中的山雀进行了染色,并在野外进行了行为测定。后来,我们使用觅食试验来调查这些色带个体对捕食者(库伯鹰,Accipiter-cooperii)模型或一系列库伯鹰叫声的反应。我们发现,觅食的黑帽山雀对捕食者模型的存在比对捕食者的叫声反应更强烈。鸟类个体的反应各不相同,行为分析(活动和探索)预测了觅食实验期间野外的个体行为。活性测定和探索测定得分仅呈弱相关,这表明这两种测定代表了不同的特征。当捕食者模型存在时,高度活跃的鸟类和快速探索者都表现出不愿意访问喂食器(要么访问次数减少,要么访问延迟更长),这种关系有些出乎意料。我们的研究结果表明,标准的行为测定可以预测野生动物的行为,但在物种和研究之间进行推广时应注意。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Do standard behavioral assays predict foraging behavior of individual Black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) in response to a predator model or calls?","authors":"Jodilyn R Jenkins, Ian G Pope, Madeline A Dykstra, Jennifer J Jenkins, Cheryl R Dykstra, Kelly A Williams","doi":"10.1037/com0000364","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000364","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Black-capped chickadees (<i>Poecile atricapillus</i>) and other species that feed at bird feeders balance the benefit of easy foraging with the added risk of predation. Individual birds respond differently to risky situations, and these differences have been attributed to the birds' personalities, which researchers commonly assess with an \"open-field\" behavioral assay. However, these behavioral assays in birds have not been compared to behavior in the wild in the context of foraging in the presence of a predator (i.e., risk-taking behavior). We color-banded chickadees in a wild population and conducted behavioral assays in the field. We later used foraging trials to investigate these color-banded individuals' responses to a predator (Cooper's hawk, <i>Accipiter cooperii</i>) model or a series of Cooper's hawk calls. We found that foraging black-capped chickadees responded more strongly to the presence of a predator model than to predator calls. Individual birds differed in their responses, and the behavioral assays (activity and exploration) predicted individual behavior in the wild during the foraging experiments. Activity and exploration assay scores were only weakly related, suggesting these two assays represent different traits. Both highly active birds and fast explorers exhibited some reluctance to visit the feeder (either reduced number of visits or greater latency to visit) when the predator model was present, a relationship that was somewhat unexpected. Our results suggest that standard behavioral assays predict behavior in the wild, but care should be taken when generalizing among species and studies. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"130-146"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71429298","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evaluation of decision-making behavior under uncertainty in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella) and humans (Homo sapiens) using a modified Balloon Analogue Risk Task. 使用改良气球模拟风险任务评估卷尾猴(Sapajus apella)和人类(Homo sapiens)在不确定性下的决策行为。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-14 DOI: 10.1037/com0000368
Olivia T Reilly, Sarah F Brosnan

Many animals, including humans, must make decisions when outcomes involve risk and/or ambiguity. To explore the evolutionary roots of decision making when outcomes are unknown, we modified the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) for use with tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus [Cebus] apella), creating the Primate Analogue Risk Task (PART). Using both the BART and the PART, we first compared human performance across the two tasks using analogous parameters. Humans' performance on the two tasks was positively correlated. Next, we tested capuchin monkeys' performance on the PART to assess their decision-making strategies in the context of ambiguity. Secondarily, although it was not the main goal of the study, this allowed us to look at species differences between capuchins' and humans' performance. Finally, we investigated the influence of prior experience on human and capuchin decision-making behavior. Neither capuchins nor humans behaved differently following an unsuccessful trial compared to a successful trial. We found individual differences in capuchin monkeys' choice behavior, though as a whole they demonstrated a pattern of reward maximization over time. Finally, as a species, capuchins had lower PART risk scores than humans. This paradigm presents a useful way to assess behavior in a context with uncertain outcomes using a comparative approach. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

包括人类在内的许多动物都必须在结果具有风险和/或模糊性的情况下做出决策。为了探索未知结果下决策制定的进化根源,我们对气球模拟风险任务(BART)进行了修改,将其用于簇毛卷尾猴(Sapajus [Cebus] apella),创建了灵长类模拟风险任务(PART)。利用 BART 和 PART,我们首先使用类比参数比较了人类在这两项任务中的表现。人类在这两项任务中的表现呈正相关。接下来,我们测试了卷尾猴在 PART 任务中的表现,以评估它们在模棱两可情况下的决策策略。其次,虽然这并不是研究的主要目的,但这让我们得以观察卷尾猴和人类在表现上的物种差异。最后,我们研究了先前经验对人类和卷尾猴决策行为的影响。与成功的试验相比,卷尾猴和人类在试验失败后的行为都没有什么不同。我们发现卷尾猴的选择行为存在个体差异,但整体而言,它们表现出一种随时间推移奖励最大化的模式。最后,作为一个物种,卷尾猴的 PART 风险得分低于人类。这种范例提供了一种有用的方法,可以在结果不确定的情况下使用比较法来评估行为。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Evaluation of decision-making behavior under uncertainty in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella) and humans (Homo sapiens) using a modified Balloon Analogue Risk Task.","authors":"Olivia T Reilly, Sarah F Brosnan","doi":"10.1037/com0000368","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000368","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many animals, including humans, must make decisions when outcomes involve risk and/or ambiguity. To explore the evolutionary roots of decision making when outcomes are unknown, we modified the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) for use with tufted capuchin monkeys (<i>Sapajus [Cebus] apella</i>), creating the Primate Analogue Risk Task (PART). Using both the BART and the PART, we first compared human performance across the two tasks using analogous parameters. Humans' performance on the two tasks was positively correlated. Next, we tested capuchin monkeys' performance on the PART to assess their decision-making strategies in the context of ambiguity. Secondarily, although it was not the main goal of the study, this allowed us to look at species differences between capuchins' and humans' performance. Finally, we investigated the influence of prior experience on human and capuchin decision-making behavior. Neither capuchins nor humans behaved differently following an unsuccessful trial compared to a successful trial. We found individual differences in capuchin monkeys' choice behavior, though as a whole they demonstrated a pattern of reward maximization over time. Finally, as a species, capuchins had lower PART risk scores than humans. This paradigm presents a useful way to assess behavior in a context with uncertain outcomes using a comparative approach. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"88-98"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138813214","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Within the sound of trouble: Do humans use pitch to correctly assess emotional arousal across species? 在麻烦的声音中:人类是否会利用音调来正确评估不同物种的情绪唤醒?
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000389
Alice Auersperg

Comments on an article by Jay W. Schwartz , Kayleigh H. Pierson, and Alexander K. Reece (see record 2024-19488-001). In this issue, Schwartz et al. (2024) tackle the pitch rule in humans by testing to what extent we use pitch alone to judge emotional arousal across closely and distantly related animal species. The findings of Schwartz et al. open a number of intriguing possibilities for future research: Notably important additional steps would include to further investigate the accuracy of the pitch rule across closely and distantly related species. Upon this, in order to study the evolutionary ancestry of the pitch rule, it will be necessary to study its applicability across nonhumans. Particularly interesting would be the inclusion of subject species that have been found to eavesdrop on heterospecific alarm calls. Previous research (see Hoeschele, 2017 for a review) as well as present findings on human ratings of macaque versus cricket calls also suggest that we should additionally focus on sound features that compliment emotional arousal rating beyond pitch such as spectral information. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

对 Jay W. Schwartz、Kayleigh H. Pierson 和 Alexander K. Reece 的文章(见记录 2024-19488-001)的评论。在本期中,Schwartz 等人(2024 年)通过测试我们在多大程度上仅使用音调来判断近亲和远亲动物物种的情绪唤醒,解决了人类的音调规则问题。Schwartz 等人的发现为今后的研究提供了许多引人入胜的可能性:值得注意的是,进一步研究音调规则在近亲和远亲物种中的准确性将是重要的补充步骤。在此基础上,为了研究音高规则的进化祖先,有必要研究它在非人类中的适用性。尤其有趣的是,研究对象物种也会被发现偷听异种警报声。以前的研究(见 Hoeschele, 2017 年综述)以及目前人类对猕猴和蟋蟀叫声的评分结果也表明,我们还应该关注除音调以外的其他声音特征,如频谱信息,以补充情绪唤醒评分。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Within the sound of trouble: Do humans use pitch to correctly assess emotional arousal across species?","authors":"Alice Auersperg","doi":"10.1037/com0000389","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000389","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Comments on an article by Jay W. Schwartz , Kayleigh H. Pierson, and Alexander K. Reece (see record 2024-19488-001). In this issue, Schwartz et al. (2024) tackle the pitch rule in humans by testing to what extent we use pitch alone to judge emotional arousal across closely and distantly related animal species. The findings of Schwartz et al. open a number of intriguing possibilities for future research: Notably important additional steps would include to further investigate the accuracy of the pitch rule across closely and distantly related species. Upon this, in order to study the evolutionary ancestry of the pitch rule, it will be necessary to study its applicability across nonhumans. Particularly interesting would be the inclusion of subject species that have been found to eavesdrop on heterospecific alarm calls. Previous research (see Hoeschele, 2017 for a review) as well as present findings on human ratings of macaque versus cricket calls also suggest that we should additionally focus on sound features that compliment emotional arousal rating beyond pitch such as spectral information. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"138 2","pages":"77-79"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141201519","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Comparative Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1