首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Comparative Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Is inferential reasoning a distinctly human cognitive feature? Testing reasoning in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). 推理是人类特有的认知特征吗?测试棉顶狨(Saguinus oedipus)的推理能力。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-12 DOI: 10.1037/com0000356
Julie J Neiworth, Ana D Knighten, Christopher Leppink-Shands

Logical inference is often assumed a human-unique ability, although many species of apes and monkeys have shown some facility within a two-cup task in which one cup is baited, the primate is shown the cup which is empty (an exclusion cue), and subsequently chooses the other baited cup. In published reports, New World monkey species show a limited ability to choose successfully, often with half or more of the subjects tested not showing the ability with auditory cues or with exclusion cues. In this study, five cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) were tested in a two-cup task with visual or auditory cues which revealed the presence or absence of bait, and in a second study, were tested with a four-cup array using a variety of walls to define the baiting space and a variety of visual cues including inclusion and exclusion. Tamarins demonstrated the ability to use either visual or auditory exclusion cues to find rewards in the two-cup study, although the visual cue required some exposure before accuracy was expressed. Experiment 2 revealed that two of three tamarins' first guesses to find rewards matched best a logic model. When they made errors, they typically chose cups adjacent to the cued location or made choices that seemed generated from avoiding empty cups. These results suggest that tamarins can deduce the location of food using reasoning, although the ability is only applied robustly to first guesses, while second guesses are motivated by approach/avoidance and proximity to cued locations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

逻辑推理通常被认为是人类独有的能力,尽管许多种类的猿猴在双杯任务中都表现出了一定的能力,在这个任务中,一个杯子是有诱饵的,灵长类动物看到的杯子是空的(排除线索),然后选择另一个有诱饵的杯子。在已发表的报告中,新世界猴类的成功选择能力有限,通常有一半或更多的受试者在听觉线索或排除线索下没有表现出这种能力。在这项研究中,五只棉顶狨(Saguinus oedipus)接受了一项双杯任务测试,测试中的视觉或听觉线索显示了诱饵的存在或不存在;在第二项研究中,五只棉顶狨接受了一项四杯阵列测试,测试中使用了各种墙壁来界定诱饵空间,并使用了包括包含和排除在内的各种视觉线索。在双杯研究中,狨猴表现出了利用视觉或听觉排除线索来寻找奖励的能力,尽管视觉线索需要接触一段时间才能表现出准确性。实验 2 显示,在三只狨猴中,有两只狨猴对寻找奖励的首次猜测与逻辑模型最为吻合。当它们出错时,通常会选择与提示位置相邻的杯子,或者做出似乎是为了避开空杯子的选择。这些结果表明,犭胥能利用推理推断出食物的位置,尽管这种能力只适用于第一次猜测,而第二次猜测是受接近/回避和接近提示位置的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Is inferential reasoning a distinctly human cognitive feature? Testing reasoning in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus).","authors":"Julie J Neiworth, Ana D Knighten, Christopher Leppink-Shands","doi":"10.1037/com0000356","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000356","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Logical inference is often assumed a human-unique ability, although many species of apes and monkeys have shown some facility within a two-cup task in which one cup is baited, the primate is shown the cup which is empty (an exclusion cue), and subsequently chooses the other baited cup. In published reports, New World monkey species show a limited ability to choose successfully, often with half or more of the subjects tested not showing the ability with auditory cues or with exclusion cues. In this study, five cotton-top tamarins (<i>Saguinus oedipus</i>) were tested in a two-cup task with visual or auditory cues which revealed the presence or absence of bait, and in a second study, were tested with a four-cup array using a variety of walls to define the baiting space and a variety of visual cues including inclusion and exclusion. Tamarins demonstrated the ability to use either visual or auditory exclusion cues to find rewards in the two-cup study, although the visual cue required some exposure before accuracy was expressed. Experiment 2 revealed that two of three tamarins' first guesses to find rewards matched best a logic model. When they made errors, they typically chose cups adjacent to the cued location or made choices that seemed generated from avoiding empty cups. These results suggest that tamarins can deduce the location of food using reasoning, although the ability is only applied robustly to first guesses, while second guesses are motivated by approach/avoidance and proximity to cued locations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11131473/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9620513","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Have we met? New insights into the role of head and body cues in the visual recognition of conspecifics in gray parrots (Psittacus erithacus). 我们见过面吗?灰鹦鹉(Psittacus erithacus)视觉识别同类时头部和身体线索作用的新见解。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000372
Alice Auersperg

Prikrylová et al. (see record 2023-79461-001) contribute a paper to this issue in which they tested two-dimensional individual recognition of familiar subjects in African gray parrots. They not only tested familiar individual recognition per se but also the effect of manipulating individual and combined features in the head and the body of their stimuli. Notably, instead of using discrimination tasks, which have previously been applied in bird studies (e.g., Hauser & Huber-Eicher, 2004; Nakamura et al., 2003; Watanabe & Ito, 1990), they went through great effort applying a matching-to-sample approach. This is particularly important as it allows for testing familiar conspecific recognition on a more conceptual level than a discrimination task. Moreover, it facilitates the comparison with previous work done on primates (e.g., Micheletta et al., 2015; Parr et al., 2000, 2006; Talbot et al., 2016). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

Prikrylová 等人(见记录 2023-79461-001)为本期撰写了一篇论文,他们测试了非洲灰鹦鹉对熟悉对象的二维个体识别能力。他们不仅测试了熟悉的个体识别本身,还测试了操纵刺激物头部和身体的单个和组合特征的效果。值得注意的是,他们没有使用以前在鸟类研究中使用过的辨别任务(例如,Hauser 和 Huber-Eicher, 2004; Nakamura 等人, 2003; Watanabe 和 Ito, 1990),而是花了很大力气应用了样本匹配方法。这一点尤为重要,因为与辨别任务相比,它可以在更概念化的层面上测试熟悉的同类识别能力。此外,这也便于与之前针对灵长类动物所做的研究进行比较(如 Micheletta 等人,2015 年;Parr 等人,2000 年、2006 年;Talbot 等人,2016 年)。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Have we met? New insights into the role of head and body cues in the visual recognition of conspecifics in gray parrots (Psittacus erithacus).","authors":"Alice Auersperg","doi":"10.1037/com0000372","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000372","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prikrylová et al. (see record 2023-79461-001) contribute a paper to this issue in which they tested two-dimensional individual recognition of familiar subjects in African gray parrots. They not only tested familiar individual recognition per se but also the effect of manipulating individual and combined features in the head and the body of their stimuli. Notably, instead of using discrimination tasks, which have previously been applied in bird studies (e.g., Hauser & Huber-Eicher, 2004; Nakamura et al., 2003; Watanabe & Ito, 1990), they went through great effort applying a matching-to-sample approach. This is particularly important as it allows for testing familiar conspecific recognition on a more conceptual level than a discrimination task. Moreover, it facilitates the comparison with previous work done on primates (e.g., Micheletta et al., 2015; Parr et al., 2000, 2006; Talbot et al., 2016). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138813216","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
First report of reindeer (Rangifer tarandus tarandus) response to human-given cues. 首次报告驯鹿(Rangifer tarandus tarandus)对人类提供的线索的反应。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-11 DOI: 10.1037/com0000353
Océane Liehrmann, Anne Ollila, Virpi Lummaa, Léa Lansade, Martin W Seltmann

Many argue that the animal understanding of human referential communication is a by-product of domestication. However, the domestication hypothesis is not unanimously supported as some nondomesticated species such as sea lions, dolphins, or African elephants perform well in the understanding of human pointing gesture. There is a need to study species with different levels of domestication across different taxa to understand the emerging communicative sociocognitive skills in animals that provide them with the ability to comprehend human-given cues. We conducted a pilot study to assess the performance of eight sledging reindeer following a commonly used human-given cue (a pointing gesture associated with gaze at the target and local enhancement) in a two-way choice task. Domestic reindeer are considered semicaptive, because of their husbandry system in free-ranging conditions, with limited control of their reproduction. We observed that the willingness of the reindeer to participate in the test was age-related, with the younger individuals which lack experience with humans being reluctant to approach the experimenters. Within the more experienced working sledging reindeer, two individuals showed excellent skills and followed the human-given cues 9 out of 10 times. Reindeer show great potential in following a human indication to locate hidden food with minimal training when properly tamed. The effect of previous experience with humans requires further investigation. This is the first demonstration in cervids of an ability to make use of experimenter-given cues in an object-choice task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

许多人认为,动物对人类指意交流的理解是驯化的副产品。然而,驯化假说并没有得到一致的支持,因为一些非驯化物种,如海狮、海豚或非洲象,在理解人类指向手势方面表现出色。有必要对不同类群中驯化程度不同的物种进行研究,以了解动物新出现的交际社会认知技能,这种技能使它们有能力理解人类给予的暗示。我们进行了一项试验性研究,以评估八只雪橇驯鹿在双向选择任务中根据人类给出的常用线索(与注视目标和局部增强相关的指向手势)的表现。家养驯鹿被认为是半适应性驯鹿,因为它们在自由放养条件下的饲养系统对其繁殖控制有限。我们观察到,驯鹿参加测试的意愿与年龄有关,年龄较小的驯鹿缺乏与人类接触的经验,不愿意接近实验者。在经验较丰富的工作雪橇驯鹿中,有两头驯鹿表现出了出色的技能,10 次中有 9 次听从了人类的提示。驯养得当的驯鹿显示出巨大的潜力,只需稍加训练就能按照人类的指示找到隐藏的食物。至于以前与人类相处的经验是否会产生影响,还需要进一步研究。这是首次在驯鹿中展示在选择对象任务中利用实验者给出的提示的能力。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"First report of reindeer (Rangifer tarandus tarandus) response to human-given cues.","authors":"Océane Liehrmann, Anne Ollila, Virpi Lummaa, Léa Lansade, Martin W Seltmann","doi":"10.1037/com0000353","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000353","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many argue that the animal understanding of human referential communication is a by-product of domestication. However, the domestication hypothesis is not unanimously supported as some nondomesticated species such as sea lions, dolphins, or African elephants perform well in the understanding of human pointing gesture. There is a need to study species with different levels of domestication across different taxa to understand the emerging communicative sociocognitive skills in animals that provide them with the ability to comprehend human-given cues. We conducted a pilot study to assess the performance of eight sledging reindeer following a commonly used human-given cue (a pointing gesture associated with gaze at the target and local enhancement) in a two-way choice task. Domestic reindeer are considered semicaptive, because of their husbandry system in free-ranging conditions, with limited control of their reproduction. We observed that the willingness of the reindeer to participate in the test was age-related, with the younger individuals which lack experience with humans being reluctant to approach the experimenters. Within the more experienced working sledging reindeer, two individuals showed excellent skills and followed the human-given cues 9 out of 10 times. Reindeer show great potential in following a human indication to locate hidden food with minimal training when properly tamed. The effect of previous experience with humans requires further investigation. This is the first demonstration in cervids of an ability to make use of experimenter-given cues in an object-choice task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9445296","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evidence of motor intentions in plants: A kinematical study. 植物运动意图的证据:一项运动学研究。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-11 DOI: 10.1037/com0000351
Bianca Bonato, Valentina Simonetti, Maria Bulgheroni, Qiuran Wang, Silvia Guerra, Silvia Quaggiotti, Benedetto Ruperti, Umberto Castiello

"How" an action is performed is not solely determined by biomechanical constraints, but it depends on the agent's intention, that is, "why" the action is performed. Recent findings suggest that intentions can be specified at a tangible and quantifiable level in the kinematics of movements; that is, different motor intentions translate into different kinematic patterns. In the present study, we used 3D kinematical analysis to investigate whether the organization of climbing plants' approach-to-grasp action is sensitive to the kind of intention driving their movement toward potential support, namely individual or social. For the individual condition, a plant in isolation acted upon the support. For the social condition, two plants were located in the same pot opposite to each other with a support in the middle. Results indicate differences in kinematics depending on the context within which the plant is acting. In the presence of neighbors, climbing plants are able to modify their behaviors to maximize their long-term gains, including the grasping of a potential support. Overall, these data suggest that the organization of climbing plants' kinematics is sensitive to the "intention" driving their movement toward a potential support. To discuss this phenomenon, we capitalize on the concept of motor intentionality in plants and on available theories concerned to motor cognition. We suggest how they could be revisited to explain the intentionality component inherent in plant life and other brainless organisms. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

“如何”执行一个动作不仅取决于生物力学约束,还取决于agent的意图,即“为什么”执行该动作。最近的研究结果表明,在运动的运动学中,意图可以在有形和可量化的水平上指定;也就是说,不同的运动意图转化为不同的运动模式。在本研究中,我们使用三维运动学分析来研究攀爬植物接近抓取动作的组织是否对驱动它们向潜在支持(即个体或社会)移动的意图敏感。对于个别情况,孤立的植物作用于支撑物。在社会条件下,两株植物被放置在同一个花盆中,彼此相对,中间有一个支撑。结果表明,运动学的差异取决于工厂所处的环境。在邻居存在的情况下,攀缘植物能够改变自己的行为,以最大化自己的长期收益,包括抓住潜在的支持。总的来说,这些数据表明,攀爬植物的运动学组织对驱动它们向潜在支撑物移动的“意图”很敏感。为了讨论这一现象,我们利用了植物的运动意向性概念和有关运动认知的现有理论。我们建议如何重新审视它们,以解释植物生命和其他无脑生物固有的意向性成分。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Evidence of motor intentions in plants: A kinematical study.","authors":"Bianca Bonato, Valentina Simonetti, Maria Bulgheroni, Qiuran Wang, Silvia Guerra, Silvia Quaggiotti, Benedetto Ruperti, Umberto Castiello","doi":"10.1037/com0000351","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000351","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>\"How\" an action is performed is not solely determined by biomechanical constraints, but it depends on the agent's intention, that is, \"why\" the action is performed. Recent findings suggest that intentions can be specified at a tangible and quantifiable level in the kinematics of movements; that is, different motor intentions translate into different kinematic patterns. In the present study, we used 3D kinematical analysis to investigate whether the organization of climbing plants' approach-to-grasp action is sensitive to the kind of intention driving their movement toward potential support, namely individual or social. For the individual condition, a plant in isolation acted upon the support. For the social condition, two plants were located in the same pot opposite to each other with a support in the middle. Results indicate differences in kinematics depending on the context within which the plant is acting. In the presence of neighbors, climbing plants are able to modify their behaviors to maximize their long-term gains, including the grasping of a potential support. Overall, these data suggest that the organization of climbing plants' kinematics is sensitive to the \"intention\" driving their movement toward a potential support. To discuss this phenomenon, we capitalize on the concept of motor intentionality in plants and on available theories concerned to motor cognition. We suggest how they could be revisited to explain the intentionality component inherent in plant life and other brainless organisms. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9449331","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Pitch Affects Human (Homo sapiens) Perception of Emotional Arousal From Diverse Animal Calls 音调补充材料影响人类(智人)对各种动物叫声的情绪唤醒的感知
4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-10-16 DOI: 10.1037/com0000366.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Pitch Affects Human (Homo sapiens) Perception of Emotional Arousal From Diverse Animal Calls","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/com0000366.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000366.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136114519","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Humans’ (Homo sapiens), Capuchin Monkeys’ (Sapajus [Cebus] apella), and Rhesus Macaques’ (Macaca mulatta) Size Judgments Shift When Stimuli Change in Frequency 人类(Homo sapiens)、卷尾猴(Sapajus [Cebus] apella)和恒河猴(Macaca mulatta)大小判断随刺激频率变化而变化的补充材料
4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-10-16 DOI: 10.1037/com0000365.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Humans’ (Homo sapiens), Capuchin Monkeys’ (Sapajus [Cebus] apella), and Rhesus Macaques’ (Macaca mulatta) Size Judgments Shift When Stimuli Change in Frequency","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/com0000365.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000365.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136113158","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for The Effects of Goal–Landmark Distance on Overshadowing: A Replication in Humans (Homo sapiens) of Goodyear and Kamil (2004) 目标-地标距离对遮蔽的影响:Goodyear和Kamil(2004)在人类(智人)中的复制
4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-09-21 DOI: 10.1037/com0000362.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for The Effects of Goal–Landmark Distance on Overshadowing: A Replication in Humans (Homo sapiens) of Goodyear and Kamil (2004)","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/com0000362.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000362.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136129640","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for A Study of Executive Function in Grey Parrots (Psittacus erithacus): Experience Can Affect Delay of Gratification 灰鹦鹉(Psittacus erithacus)执行功能研究补充材料:经验可以影响延迟满足
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-08-07 DOI: 10.1037/com0000361.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for A Study of Executive Function in Grey Parrots (Psittacus erithacus): Experience Can Affect Delay of Gratification","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/com0000361.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000361.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-08-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"82022296","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Why do distractions sometimes aid self-control? Pigeons (Columba livia) highlight possible mechanisms underlying the distraction effect. 为什么分心有时有助于自我控制?鸽子(Columba livia)强调了分心效应的可能机制。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000363
Michael J Beran

In this essay, the author explores the question of why distractions sometimes aid self-control. In a study with chimpanzees, Evans and Beran (2007) used two conditions with toys to address the possibility raised by Mueller et al. (2023) about toys as distractors. In the first, the accumulating rewards were within reach, and so chimpanzees had to inhibit taking rewards if more were to accumulate. The second condition was essential to this issue, as in that case toys also were available, but the delayed reward was out of reach (i.e., the chimpanzees were forced to wait to get the delayed reward). Because these trial types were intermixed, an explanation of the toys' effectiveness through conditioned association with the delayed reward would predict that the chimpanzees should have engaged the toys equally often in both conditions. However, three of four chimpanzees engaged the toys more when the accumulating reward was within reach compared to when it was not. Evans and Beran suggested that engagement with distractors in that study occurred when it was functionally effective in aiding delayed gratification, not solely as a result of toys being associated with delayed rewards, and thus was a cognitively controlled decision by the chimpanzees. The results of Evans and Beran (2007) have yet to be replicated in any other primate species. But, Mueller et al. (2023) noted that a study with a parrot (Koepke et al., 2015) showed that the parrot would use distractors and even move the smaller, sooner reward away from itself, perhaps matching the self-distraction techniques of children who hide their faces or talk to themselves. Thus, it remains to be determined whether those behaviors also can be explained by learned associations with reward or whether they reflect attention-based explanations. Mueller et al. (2023) have provided a creative experiment and a compelling argument that more careful analysis is needed of exactly what happens when an animal (or child) engages distractors and shows improved self-control, so that we can understand what role past reinforcement history may play and what possible attentional strategies or other cognitive strategies are at work in different self-control tasks. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

在这篇文章中,作者探讨了为什么分散注意力有时有助于自我控制的问题。在对黑猩猩的研究中,埃文斯和贝兰(2007 年)使用了两种玩具条件来解决穆勒等人(2023 年)提出的玩具作为分心物的可能性。在第一种情况下,不断累积的奖励触手可及,因此黑猩猩必须在更多奖励累积时抑制拿走奖励。第二种情况对这一问题至关重要,因为在这种情况下,玩具也是可用的,但延迟奖励却遥不可及(即黑猩猩被迫等待获得延迟奖励)。由于这些试验类型是混合在一起的,如果通过延迟奖励的条件联想来解释玩具的有效性,那么黑猩猩在这两种情况下使用玩具的频率应该是相同的。然而,四只黑猩猩中有三只在累积奖赏触手可及时比触及不到时更多地使用玩具。埃文斯和贝兰认为,在该研究中,当玩具在功能上有效地帮助延迟满足时,黑猩猩才会使用分散注意力的玩具,而不仅仅是因为玩具与延迟奖励相关联,因此这是黑猩猩在认知控制下做出的决定。埃文斯和贝兰(2007 年)的研究结果尚未在其他灵长类动物身上得到验证。但是,Mueller 等人(2023 年)指出,一项对鹦鹉的研究(Koepke 等人,2015 年)表明,鹦鹉会使用分散注意力的方法,甚至会将较小的、较快的奖励从自己身边移开,这可能与隐藏自己的脸或自言自语的儿童的自我分散技术相吻合。因此,这些行为是否也可以用学习到的奖励联想来解释,或者它们是否反映了基于注意力的解释,还有待确定。穆勒等人(2023 年)提供了一个创造性的实验和一个令人信服的论据,即我们需要更仔细地分析当动物(或儿童)与分心物接触并表现出更好的自我控制能力时到底会发生什么,这样我们才能了解过去的强化历史可能起什么作用,以及在不同的自我控制任务中可能有哪些注意策略或其他认知策略在起作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Why do distractions sometimes aid self-control? Pigeons (Columba livia) highlight possible mechanisms underlying the distraction effect.","authors":"Michael J Beran","doi":"10.1037/com0000363","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000363","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this essay, the author explores the question of why distractions sometimes aid self-control. In a study with chimpanzees, Evans and Beran (2007) used two conditions with toys to address the possibility raised by Mueller et al. (2023) about toys as distractors. In the first, the accumulating rewards were within reach, and so chimpanzees had to inhibit taking rewards if more were to accumulate. The second condition was essential to this issue, as in that case toys also were available, but the delayed reward was out of reach (i.e., the chimpanzees were forced to wait to get the delayed reward). Because these trial types were intermixed, an explanation of the toys' effectiveness through conditioned association with the delayed reward would predict that the chimpanzees should have engaged the toys equally often in both conditions. However, three of four chimpanzees engaged the toys more when the accumulating reward was within reach compared to when it was not. Evans and Beran suggested that engagement with distractors in that study occurred when it was functionally effective in aiding delayed gratification, not solely as a result of toys being associated with delayed rewards, and thus was a cognitively controlled decision by the chimpanzees. The results of Evans and Beran (2007) have yet to be replicated in any other primate species. But, Mueller et al. (2023) noted that a study with a parrot (Koepke et al., 2015) showed that the parrot would use distractors and even move the smaller, sooner reward away from itself, perhaps matching the self-distraction techniques of children who hide their faces or talk to themselves. Thus, it remains to be determined whether those behaviors also can be explained by learned associations with reward or whether they reflect attention-based explanations. Mueller et al. (2023) have provided a creative experiment and a compelling argument that more careful analysis is needed of exactly what happens when an animal (or child) engages distractors and shows improved self-control, so that we can understand what role past reinforcement history may play and what possible attentional strategies or other cognitive strategies are at work in different self-control tasks. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10111174","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What enables "distraction" to reduce delay discounting for pigeons (Columba livia). 是什么使“分心”能够减少鸽子的延迟折扣(Columba livia)。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000337
Peyton M Mueller, Daniel N Peng, Thomas R Zentall

In a successive delay-discounting task, a small reward can be obtained immediately but a larger reward can be obtained if one waits. There is evidence that the larger reward can be obtained more easily if one is "distracted" from obtaining the small reward. It is proposed here that a distractor stimulus may function as a Pavlovian conditioned stimulus (sign tracking) because orienting to it may be directly associated with the larger reinforcer. In the present study with pigeons, we examined two successive procedures: (a) a peck to a red light resulted in one pellet of food, and waiting for the red light to turn off resulted in five pellets (Red-Only). (b) If the pigeon pecked a red light, it received one pellet of food, and if it waited for the red light to turn to green, a peck to the green light resulted in five pellets of food (Red-Green). For both groups, on some trials, a concurrent (distractor) stimulus appeared with the red light but responses to it had no programed consequence. Results indicated that the pigeons in both groups waited for the larger reward more often when the distractor was present than when it was absent and that pigeons in the Red-Only group waited longer than those in the Red-Green group. The results are consistent with the hypothesis that the concurrent stimulus served as a conditioned stimulus for the Red-Only group and as a higher order conditioned stimulus for the Red-Green group. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

在连续延迟贴现任务中,可以立即获得较小的奖励,但如果等待则可以获得较大的奖励。有证据表明,如果一个人在获得小奖励时“分心”,那么获得大奖励就会更容易。本文提出,干扰物刺激可能具有巴甫洛夫条件刺激(符号跟踪)的功能,因为对它的定向可能与较大的强化物直接相关。在目前对鸽子的研究中,我们检查了两个连续的过程:(a)啄红灯导致一个食物颗粒,等待红灯关闭导致五个颗粒(仅限红色)。(b)如果鸽子啄红灯,它会得到一粒食物,如果它等待红灯变成绿灯,啄绿灯会得到五粒食物(红绿)。对两组来说,在一些试验中,一个同时出现的(干扰物)刺激与红灯同时出现,但对它的反应没有程序化的后果。结果表明,两组鸽子在有干扰物时比没有干扰物时等待更大奖励的次数要多,而且只有红色干扰物组的鸽子等待的时间比红绿干扰物组的鸽子要长。结果与同时刺激在红绿组和红绿组中分别作为条件刺激和高阶条件刺激的假设一致。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"What enables \"distraction\" to reduce delay discounting for pigeons (Columba livia).","authors":"Peyton M Mueller,&nbsp;Daniel N Peng,&nbsp;Thomas R Zentall","doi":"10.1037/com0000337","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000337","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In a successive delay-discounting task, a small reward can be obtained immediately but a larger reward can be obtained if one waits. There is evidence that the larger reward can be obtained more easily if one is \"distracted\" from obtaining the small reward. It is proposed here that a distractor stimulus may function as a Pavlovian conditioned stimulus (sign tracking) because orienting to it may be directly associated with the larger reinforcer. In the present study with pigeons, we examined two successive procedures: (a) a peck to a red light resulted in one pellet of food, and waiting for the red light to turn off resulted in five pellets (Red-Only). (b) If the pigeon pecked a red light, it received one pellet of food, and if it waited for the red light to turn to green, a peck to the green light resulted in five pellets of food (Red-Green). For both groups, on some trials, a concurrent (distractor) stimulus appeared with the red light but responses to it had no programed consequence. Results indicated that the pigeons in both groups waited for the larger reward more often when the distractor was present than when it was absent and that pigeons in the Red-Only group waited longer than those in the Red-Green group. The results are consistent with the hypothesis that the concurrent stimulus served as a conditioned stimulus for the Red-Only group and as a higher order conditioned stimulus for the Red-Green group. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10111173","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Journal of Comparative Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1