Ana Lucía Arbaiza-Bayona, Colleen M Schaffner, Germán Gutiérrez, Filippo Aureli
We studied mother-infant relationships and infant independence in wild Geoffroy's spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) during the first 3 years of infant life. We used 15-min focal sampling to collect data on mother-infant interactions and infant behavior in 12 mother-infant dyads in the Yucatan peninsula, Mexico. Data were analyzed using generalized linear mixed models. Newborns spent almost all their time in proximity and in contact with their mothers. The time infants spent within one-arm reach from the mother decreased with age, and the infant was primarily responsible for maintaining proximity. The time infants spent farther than 8 m from the mother, in independent locomotion, exploration, and proximity with group members other than the mother increased with age. We identified two developmental periods associated with critical milestones of infant independence: the first at 8 to 10 months when independent locomotion and exploration began, and the proportion of time in proximity with group members other than the mother increased and the second at 19 to 21 months when the mother's rejection started, maternal carrying ended and mother's help, in the form of bridging canopy gaps, peaked. Compared with other primate species of similar size, Ateles geoffroyi have an extended dependence period, which could be related to the cognitive and developmental challenges imposed by their socioecological characteristics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
我们研究了野生Geoffroy蜘蛛猴(Ateles geoffroyi)在婴儿生命的前3年里的母子关系和婴儿独立性。我们采用15分钟的焦点抽样方法收集了墨西哥尤卡坦半岛12对母子的母婴互动和婴儿行为数据。数据分析采用广义线性混合模型。新生儿几乎所有的时间都和母亲在一起。婴儿在母亲单臂可及范围内的时间随着年龄的增长而减少,婴儿主要负责保持与母亲的接近。随着年龄的增长,婴儿在距离母亲超过8米的地方进行独立运动、探索和接近母亲以外的群体成员的时间增加。我们确定了两个与婴儿独立的关键里程碑相关的发育时期:第一个是在8到10个月时,当母亲开始独立运动和探索时,与母亲以外的群体成员接近的时间比例增加;第二个是在19到21个月时,母亲开始排斥,母亲的携带结束,母亲的帮助(以弥合树冠间隙的形式)达到顶峰。与其他类似大小的灵长类物种相比,geoffroyi的依赖期较长,这可能与其社会生态特征带来的认知和发育挑战有关。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Mother-infant relationships and infant independence in wild Geoffroy's spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi).","authors":"Ana Lucía Arbaiza-Bayona, Colleen M Schaffner, Germán Gutiérrez, Filippo Aureli","doi":"10.1037/com0000329","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000329","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We studied mother-infant relationships and infant independence in wild Geoffroy's spider monkeys (<i>Ateles geoffroyi</i>) during the first 3 years of infant life. We used 15-min focal sampling to collect data on mother-infant interactions and infant behavior in 12 mother-infant dyads in the Yucatan peninsula, Mexico. Data were analyzed using generalized linear mixed models. Newborns spent almost all their time in proximity and in contact with their mothers. The time infants spent within one-arm reach from the mother decreased with age, and the infant was primarily responsible for maintaining proximity. The time infants spent farther than 8 m from the mother, in independent locomotion, exploration, and proximity with group members other than the mother increased with age. We identified two developmental periods associated with critical milestones of infant independence: the first at 8 to 10 months when independent locomotion and exploration began, and the proportion of time in proximity with group members other than the mother increased and the second at 19 to 21 months when the mother's rejection started, maternal carrying ended and mother's help, in the form of bridging canopy gaps, peaked. Compared with other primate species of similar size, <i>Ateles geoffroyi</i> have an extended dependence period, which could be related to the cognitive and developmental challenges imposed by their socioecological characteristics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"136 4","pages":"221-235"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10474444","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Unlike nonhuman primates, individual differences between humans in dominance do not appear as broad personality factors. This may be attributable to differences between the questionnaires used to study human and nonhuman primate personality. Alternatively, this may reflect differences in the organization of personality in humans and nonhuman primates. To determine which of these possibilities was most likely, 1,147 participants were recruited and asked to rate their personality and/or that of somebody else on the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire (HPQ), which has been used to study nonhuman primate personality. A large subset of these participants (~80%) also completed self- and/or rater reports of one of three questionnaires used to measure human personality. Exploratory factor analyses of HPQ rater report data yielded five factors. These factors correlated mostly in expected ways with scales from questionnaires used to study human personality. Exploratory factor analyses of HPQ self-report data yielded no clear number of factors and no consistent evidence with respect to the presence of a dominance factor. Subsequent analyses compared HPQ scales that represented dominance factors in chimpanzees, bonobos, mountain gorillas, and orangutans to scales derived from the Revised NEO Personality Inventory, including Fearless Dominance, which combined Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Extraversion facets, Emotional Stability (the inverse of Neuroticism), and Extraversion's Assertiveness facet. Fearless Dominance and Assertiveness were most like the great ape dominance factors. The absence of human dominance factors, therefore, appears to reflect present or past social conditions of our species. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
与非人类灵长类动物不同,人类在支配地位方面的个体差异并不表现为广泛的人格因素。这可能是由于用于研究人类和非人类灵长类动物性格的问卷存在差异。或者,这可能反映了人类和非人类灵长类动物在人格组织上的差异。为了确定哪一种可能性最大,研究人员招募了1147名参与者,并要求他们在类人猿人格问卷(HPQ)上对自己和/或其他人的人格进行打分,该问卷已被用于研究非人类灵长类动物的人格。这些参与者中的很大一部分(约80%)还完成了用于测量人类性格的三份问卷中的一份自我和/或评分报告。对HPQ评分报告数据进行探索性因素分析,得出五个因素。这些因素与用于研究人类个性的问卷中的尺度主要以预期的方式相关。对HPQ自述数据的探索性因素分析没有得出明确的因素数量,也没有一致的证据表明存在优势因素。随后的分析将代表黑猩猩、倭黑猩猩、山地大猩猩和猩猩的优势因素的HPQ量表与来自修订NEO人格量表的量表进行了比较,其中包括无畏的优势,它结合了神经质、随和性、尽责性和外向性方面、情绪稳定性(与神经质相反)和外向性的自信方面。无所畏惧的优势和自信最接近类人猿的优势因素。因此,人类主导因素的缺失似乎反映了我们这个物种现在或过去的社会状况。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Dominance in human (Homo sapiens) personality space and in hominoid phylogeny.","authors":"Alexander Weiss","doi":"10.1037/com0000322","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000322","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Unlike nonhuman primates, individual differences between humans in dominance do not appear as broad personality factors. This may be attributable to differences between the questionnaires used to study human and nonhuman primate personality. Alternatively, this may reflect differences in the organization of personality in humans and nonhuman primates. To determine which of these possibilities was most likely, 1,147 participants were recruited and asked to rate their personality and/or that of somebody else on the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire (HPQ), which has been used to study nonhuman primate personality. A large subset of these participants (~80%) also completed self- and/or rater reports of one of three questionnaires used to measure human personality. Exploratory factor analyses of HPQ rater report data yielded five factors. These factors correlated mostly in expected ways with scales from questionnaires used to study human personality. Exploratory factor analyses of HPQ self-report data yielded no clear number of factors and no consistent evidence with respect to the presence of a dominance factor. Subsequent analyses compared HPQ scales that represented dominance factors in chimpanzees, bonobos, mountain gorillas, and orangutans to scales derived from the Revised NEO Personality Inventory, including Fearless Dominance, which combined Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Extraversion facets, Emotional Stability (the inverse of Neuroticism), and Extraversion's Assertiveness facet. Fearless Dominance and Assertiveness were most like the great ape dominance factors. The absence of human dominance factors, therefore, appears to reflect present or past social conditions of our species. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"136 4","pages":"236-254"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10531690","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
On the surface, the fields of animal communication and human linguistics have arrived at conflicting theories and conclusions with respect to the effect of social complexity on communicative complexity. For example, an increase in group size is argued to have opposite consequences on human versus animal communication systems: although an increase in human community size leads to some types of language simplification, an increase in animal group size leads to an increase in signal complexity. But do human and animal communication systems really show such a fundamental discrepancy? Our key message is that the tension between these two adjacent fields is the result of (a) a focus on different levels of analysis (namely, signal variation or grammar-like rules) and (b) an inconsistent use of terminology (namely, the terms "simple" and "complex"). By disentangling and clarifying these terms with respect to different measures of communicative complexity, we show that although animal and human communication systems indeed show some contradictory effects with respect to signal variability, they actually display essentially the same patterns with respect to grammar-like structure. This is despite the fact that the definitions of complexity and simplicity are actually aligned for signal variability, but diverge for grammatical structure. We conclude by advocating for the use of more objective and descriptive terms instead of terms such as "complexity," which can be applied uniformly for human and animal communication systems-leading to comparable descriptions of findings across species and promoting a more productive dialogue between fields. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
从表面上看,关于社会复杂性对交际复杂性的影响,动物交际领域和人类语言学领域已经得出了相互矛盾的理论和结论。例如,人们认为群体规模的增加会对人类和动物的交流系统产生相反的影响:尽管人类群体规模的增加会导致某些类型的语言简化,但动物群体规模的增加会导致信号复杂性的增加。但是,人类和动物的交流系统真的表现出如此根本的差异吗?我们的关键信息是,这两个相邻领域之间的紧张关系是(a)关注不同层次的分析(即信号变化或类似语法的规则)和(b)术语使用不一致(即术语“简单”和“复杂”)的结果。通过将这些术语从不同的交流复杂性度量中分离出来并加以澄清,我们表明,尽管动物和人类的交流系统在信号变异性方面确实表现出一些相互矛盾的影响,但它们实际上在类似语法的结构方面表现出本质上相同的模式。尽管事实上,复杂性和简单性的定义实际上是一致的信号变异性,但分歧的语法结构。最后,我们提倡使用更客观和描述性的术语,而不是像“复杂性”这样的术语,因为“复杂性”可以统一应用于人类和动物的交流系统,从而导致跨物种发现的可比描述,并促进领域之间更有成效的对话。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"What is simple is actually quite complex: A critical note on terminology in the domain of language and communication.","authors":"Limor Raviv, Louise R Peckre, Cedric Boeckx","doi":"10.1037/com0000328","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000328","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>On the surface, the fields of animal communication and human linguistics have arrived at conflicting theories and conclusions with respect to the effect of social complexity on communicative complexity. For example, an increase in group size is argued to have opposite consequences on human versus animal communication systems: although an increase in human community size leads to some types of language simplification, an increase in animal group size leads to an increase in signal complexity. But do human and animal communication systems really show such a fundamental discrepancy? Our key message is that the tension between these two adjacent fields is the result of (a) a focus on different levels of analysis (namely, signal variation or grammar-like rules) and (b) an inconsistent use of terminology (namely, the terms \"simple\" and \"complex\"). By disentangling and clarifying these terms with respect to different measures of communicative complexity, we show that although animal and human communication systems indeed show some contradictory effects with respect to signal variability, they actually display essentially the same patterns with respect to grammar-like structure. This is despite the fact that the definitions of complexity and simplicity are actually aligned for signal variability, but diverge for grammatical structure. We conclude by advocating for the use of more objective and descriptive terms instead of terms such as \"complexity,\" which can be applied uniformly for human and animal communication systems-leading to comparable descriptions of findings across species and promoting a more productive dialogue between fields. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"136 4","pages":"215-220"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10839707","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Same-Different Conceptualization in Dogs (Canis familiaris)","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/com0000332.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000332.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"10 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"85108016","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Investigation of Mirror-Self Recognition in Ravens (Corvus corax)","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/com0000319.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000319.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"22 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-06-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87550901","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Complex social life is considered important to the evolution of cognition in primates. One key aspect of primate social interactions concerns the degree of competition that individuals face in their social group. To examine how social tolerance versus competition shapes social cognition, we experimentally assessed capacities for flexible gaze-following in more tolerant Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) and compared to previous data from despotic rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Monkeys experienced one of two possible conditions. In the barrier condition, they observed an actor look upwards into an overheard barrier, so they could not directly see the target of the actor's gaze without reorienting. In the no barrier condition, they observed an actor look upwards without a barrier blocking her line-of-sight, so they could observe the target of the actor's gaze by also looking upwards. Both species (N = 58 Barbary macaques, 64 rhesus macaques) could flexibly modulate their gaze responses to account for the demonstrator's line of sight, looking up more often when no barrier was present, and this flexible modulation declined with age in both species. However, neither species preferentially approached to look inside the barrier when their view of the target location was obscured, although rhesus macaques approached more overall. This pattern suggests that both tolerant and despotic macaques exhibit similar capacities to track other's line of sight and do not preferentially reorient their bodies to observe what an actor looks at in this situation. This contrasts with other work indicating that competitive primates are especially adept at some aspects of theory of mind. Thus, it is important to understand both the similarities and differences in the social-cognitive abilities of primates with different social styles. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
复杂的社会生活被认为对灵长类动物的认知进化很重要。灵长类动物社会互动的一个关键方面是个体在其社会群体中面临的竞争程度。为了研究社会宽容与竞争是如何塑造社会认知的,我们通过实验评估了更具宽容的巴巴里猕猴(Macaca sylvanus)灵活注视跟随的能力,并与之前来自专制恒河猕猴(Macaca mulatta)的数据进行了比较。猴子经历了两种可能的情况之一。在屏障条件下,他们观察到一个演员向上看一个偷听的屏障,所以他们不能直接看到演员的目光的目标,而不改变方向。在无障碍条件下,他们观察到演员向上看,没有障碍挡住她的视线,所以他们可以通过向上看来观察演员凝视的目标。这两个物种(N = 58只巴巴里猕猴,64只恒河猴)都可以灵活地调节它们的凝视反应,以解释演示者的视线,在没有障碍物的情况下更频繁地抬头,这种灵活的调节在这两个物种中都随着年龄的增长而下降。然而,当目标位置被遮挡时,这两个物种都不会优先接近障碍物内部,尽管恒河猴更全面地接近。这种模式表明,宽容和专制的猕猴都表现出类似的追踪他人视线的能力,在这种情况下,它们不会优先调整自己的身体来观察演员在看什么。这与其他表明竞争性灵长类动物特别擅长心理理论某些方面的研究形成了对比。因此,了解不同社会类型的灵长类动物社会认知能力的异同是十分重要的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Sensitivity to line-of-sight in tolerant versus despotic macaques (Macaca sylvanus and Macaca mulatta).","authors":"Rosemary Bettle, Alexandra G Rosati","doi":"10.1037/com0000309","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000309","url":null,"abstract":"Complex social life is considered important to the evolution of cognition in primates. One key aspect of primate social interactions concerns the degree of competition that individuals face in their social group. To examine how social tolerance versus competition shapes social cognition, we experimentally assessed capacities for flexible gaze-following in more tolerant Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) and compared to previous data from despotic rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Monkeys experienced one of two possible conditions. In the barrier condition, they observed an actor look upwards into an overheard barrier, so they could not directly see the target of the actor's gaze without reorienting. In the no barrier condition, they observed an actor look upwards without a barrier blocking her line-of-sight, so they could observe the target of the actor's gaze by also looking upwards. Both species (N = 58 Barbary macaques, 64 rhesus macaques) could flexibly modulate their gaze responses to account for the demonstrator's line of sight, looking up more often when no barrier was present, and this flexible modulation declined with age in both species. However, neither species preferentially approached to look inside the barrier when their view of the target location was obscured, although rhesus macaques approached more overall. This pattern suggests that both tolerant and despotic macaques exhibit similar capacities to track other's line of sight and do not preferentially reorient their bodies to observe what an actor looks at in this situation. This contrasts with other work indicating that competitive primates are especially adept at some aspects of theory of mind. Thus, it is important to understand both the similarities and differences in the social-cognitive abilities of primates with different social styles. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"136 2","pages":"93-104"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9851130/pdf/nihms-1862451.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9110397","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Comments on an article by R. Bettle and A. G. Rosati (see record 2022-45647-001). The testing of subjects' abilities to follow human eye gaze has been particularly well studied in nonhuman primates, and this is the question addressed by the Featured Article for this issue by Bettle and Rosati. As described in Bettle and Rosati, he competition hypothesis, stemming from the Machiavellian Intelligence Hypothesis, predicts that species that are more competitive and aggressive will exhibit greater ability to use cues like eye gaze. The tolerance hypothesis, conversely, predicts that species that are more tolerant and affiliative will exhibit greater ability to use these cues. Bettle and Rosati tested Barbary macaques with identical methods. Compared to rhesus macaques, Barbary macaques are relatively tolerant and highly affiliative. Importantly, the authors coded the video-recorded data blind and with high interobserver reliability. The authors found that Barbary macaques were also able to follow human eye gaze: Although roughly half the individuals looked up and followed human's eye gaze in the no barrier condition, only a third looked up in the barrier condition where they could not see what the human was looking at by doing so. These results were quite comparable to the earlier study conducted with rhesus macaques, suggesting that tolerant and less competitive species actually show similar skills. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
对R. Bettle和A. G. Rosati文章的评论(见记录2022-45647-001)。在非人类灵长类动物中,对受试者跟随人类目光的能力的测试已经得到了很好的研究,这是Bettle和Rosati在本期专题文章中提到的问题。正如Bettle和Rosati所描述的那样,来自马基雅维利智力假说的竞争假说预测,更具竞争力和侵略性的物种将表现出更强的使用眼神等线索的能力。与此相反,耐受性假说预测,耐受性和亲和性更强的物种将表现出更强的利用这些线索的能力。贝特尔和罗萨蒂用同样的方法对巴巴里猕猴进行了测试。与恒河猴相比,巴巴里恒河猴具有相对的忍耐力和高度的亲和力。重要的是,作者对视频记录的数据进行了盲编码,具有较高的观察者间可靠性。研究人员发现,巴巴里猕猴也能够跟随人类的目光:尽管在没有障碍的情况下,大约有一半的猕猴向上看并跟随人类的目光,但在有障碍的情况下,只有三分之一的猕猴向上看,在这种情况下,它们看不到人类在看什么。这些结果与之前对恒河猴进行的研究相当,表明宽容和不那么竞争的物种实际上表现出相似的技能。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Testing the role of macaque social tolerance in ability to follow human eye gaze.","authors":"T. Freeberg","doi":"10.1037/com0000317","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000317","url":null,"abstract":"Comments on an article by R. Bettle and A. G. Rosati (see record 2022-45647-001). The testing of subjects' abilities to follow human eye gaze has been particularly well studied in nonhuman primates, and this is the question addressed by the Featured Article for this issue by Bettle and Rosati. As described in Bettle and Rosati, he competition hypothesis, stemming from the Machiavellian Intelligence Hypothesis, predicts that species that are more competitive and aggressive will exhibit greater ability to use cues like eye gaze. The tolerance hypothesis, conversely, predicts that species that are more tolerant and affiliative will exhibit greater ability to use these cues. Bettle and Rosati tested Barbary macaques with identical methods. Compared to rhesus macaques, Barbary macaques are relatively tolerant and highly affiliative. Importantly, the authors coded the video-recorded data blind and with high interobserver reliability. The authors found that Barbary macaques were also able to follow human eye gaze: Although roughly half the individuals looked up and followed human's eye gaze in the no barrier condition, only a third looked up in the barrier condition where they could not see what the human was looking at by doing so. These results were quite comparable to the earlier study conducted with rhesus macaques, suggesting that tolerant and less competitive species actually show similar skills. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"39 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-04-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72644720","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Anthony Roig, H. Meunier, Eva Poulingue, Angélique Marty, R. Thouvarecq, J. Rivière
Economic risk proneness is displayed by human children and some nonhuman primate species. To explore the role of attraction toward the unknown and the unexpected in economic choices, 2.5-year-old children and capuchin monkeys were presented in Experiment 1 with a gambling task in which participants had to choose between 2 options, a secure option and a risky option characterized by an unexpected event. In contrast to capuchins, toddlers showed a strong preference for the risky option over the safe option. In Experiment 2, toddlers maintained their risky choices despite the increased salience of the safe option. In contrast to toddlers, capuchins preferentially chose the safe option in this second experiment. We argue that capuchins' risk aversion reflects an exploitation strategy of known and safe options. In human children, the attractiveness of uncertain reward appears to be linked to their novelty seeking. We argue that toddlers' risk proneness in the gain domain reflects an exploratory search strategy. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
人类儿童和一些非人灵长类动物也表现出经济风险倾向。为了探索未知和意外的吸引力在经济选择中的作用,实验1向2.5岁的儿童和卷尾猴提供了一个赌博任务,参与者必须在两个选项中做出选择,一个是安全选项,另一个是以意外事件为特征的风险选项。与卷尾猴相比,幼儿表现出对风险选项的强烈偏好,而不是安全选项。在实验2中,尽管安全选项越来越明显,但幼儿仍然坚持他们的风险选择。与幼儿相比,卷尾猴在第二个实验中优先选择了安全的选项。我们认为,卷尾猴的风险厌恶反映了一种已知和安全选项的开发策略。在人类儿童中,不确定奖励的吸引力似乎与他们对新奇事物的追求有关。我们认为,幼儿在获得领域的风险倾向反映了一种探索性搜索策略。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Is economic risk proneness in young children (Homo sapiens) driven by exploratory behavior? A comparison with capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella).","authors":"Anthony Roig, H. Meunier, Eva Poulingue, Angélique Marty, R. Thouvarecq, J. Rivière","doi":"10.1037/com0000314","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000314","url":null,"abstract":"Economic risk proneness is displayed by human children and some nonhuman primate species. To explore the role of attraction toward the unknown and the unexpected in economic choices, 2.5-year-old children and capuchin monkeys were presented in Experiment 1 with a gambling task in which participants had to choose between 2 options, a secure option and a risky option characterized by an unexpected event. In contrast to capuchins, toddlers showed a strong preference for the risky option over the safe option. In Experiment 2, toddlers maintained their risky choices despite the increased salience of the safe option. In contrast to toddlers, capuchins preferentially chose the safe option in this second experiment. We argue that capuchins' risk aversion reflects an exploitation strategy of known and safe options. In human children, the attractiveness of uncertain reward appears to be linked to their novelty seeking. We argue that toddlers' risk proneness in the gain domain reflects an exploratory search strategy. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"25 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-04-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"73994503","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Human joint action is generally facilitated by the tendency to represent not only one's own task and behavior but also the partner's. Yet, under some conditions, such as in the joint Simon task, corepresentation can cause interference and hampers, rather than facilitates, joint performance. A competent cooperator should thus also be able to flexibly inhibit corepresentation if that is conducive to cooperation success. To investigate the evolutionary origin of corepresentation, as well as the cooperative flexibility to inhibit it when necessary, we tested brown capuchins and Tonkean macaques in the joint Simon task and compared them with the previously tested marmosets. Corepresentation was present in all 3 species, but its strength and the cooperation success varied substantially. The cooperatively breeding marmosets showed the weakest corepresentation effect and, therefore, highest cooperation success, and they were the only ones to use mutual gaze when coordination with the partner was necessary. Cooperative flexibility was therefore not correlated with brain size but with the prevalence of cooperation in nature. This conclusion was corroborated by species differences in gazing patterns and suggests that the drivers of cooperative flexibility in humans were not solely cognitive. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
人类的联合行动通常是由不仅代表自己的任务和行为而且代表同伴的倾向而促成的。然而,在某些情况下,例如在联合Simon任务中,共同表示可能会造成干扰和阻碍,而不是促进联合执行。因此,一个称职的合作者也应该能够灵活地抑制共同代表,如果这有利于合作的成功。为了研究共同表征的进化起源,以及必要时抑制共同表征的合作灵活性,我们在联合Simon任务中测试了棕色卷尾猴和东京猕猴,并将它们与之前测试的狨猴进行了比较。3个物种均存在共代表,但其强度和合作成功率差异较大。合作繁殖的狨猴表现出最弱的共同表征效应,因此合作成功率最高,当需要与同伴协调时,它们是唯一使用相互凝视的狨猴。因此,合作灵活性与大脑大小无关,而与自然界中合作的普遍程度有关。这一结论被注视模式的物种差异所证实,并表明人类合作灵活性的驱动因素不仅仅是认知。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Primate origins of corepresentation and cooperative flexibility: A comparative study with common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus), brown capuchins (Sapajus apella), and Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana).","authors":"F. Miss, H. Meunier, Judith M Burkart","doi":"10.1037/com0000315","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000315","url":null,"abstract":"Human joint action is generally facilitated by the tendency to represent not only one's own task and behavior but also the partner's. Yet, under some conditions, such as in the joint Simon task, corepresentation can cause interference and hampers, rather than facilitates, joint performance. A competent cooperator should thus also be able to flexibly inhibit corepresentation if that is conducive to cooperation success. To investigate the evolutionary origin of corepresentation, as well as the cooperative flexibility to inhibit it when necessary, we tested brown capuchins and Tonkean macaques in the joint Simon task and compared them with the previously tested marmosets. Corepresentation was present in all 3 species, but its strength and the cooperation success varied substantially. The cooperatively breeding marmosets showed the weakest corepresentation effect and, therefore, highest cooperation success, and they were the only ones to use mutual gaze when coordination with the partner was necessary. Cooperative flexibility was therefore not correlated with brain size but with the prevalence of cooperation in nature. This conclusion was corroborated by species differences in gazing patterns and suggests that the drivers of cooperative flexibility in humans were not solely cognitive. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"313 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-04-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77512412","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Primate Origins of Corepresentation and Cooperative Flexibility: A Comparative Study With Common Marmosets (Callithrix jacchus), Brown Capuchins (Sapajus apella), and Tonkean Macaques (Macaca tonkeana)","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/com0000315.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000315.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"6 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-04-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86824677","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}