首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Comparative Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Dominance in human (Homo sapiens) personality space and in hominoid phylogeny. 人类(智人)人格空间和类人猿系统发育中的支配地位。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000322
Alexander Weiss

Unlike nonhuman primates, individual differences between humans in dominance do not appear as broad personality factors. This may be attributable to differences between the questionnaires used to study human and nonhuman primate personality. Alternatively, this may reflect differences in the organization of personality in humans and nonhuman primates. To determine which of these possibilities was most likely, 1,147 participants were recruited and asked to rate their personality and/or that of somebody else on the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire (HPQ), which has been used to study nonhuman primate personality. A large subset of these participants (~80%) also completed self- and/or rater reports of one of three questionnaires used to measure human personality. Exploratory factor analyses of HPQ rater report data yielded five factors. These factors correlated mostly in expected ways with scales from questionnaires used to study human personality. Exploratory factor analyses of HPQ self-report data yielded no clear number of factors and no consistent evidence with respect to the presence of a dominance factor. Subsequent analyses compared HPQ scales that represented dominance factors in chimpanzees, bonobos, mountain gorillas, and orangutans to scales derived from the Revised NEO Personality Inventory, including Fearless Dominance, which combined Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Extraversion facets, Emotional Stability (the inverse of Neuroticism), and Extraversion's Assertiveness facet. Fearless Dominance and Assertiveness were most like the great ape dominance factors. The absence of human dominance factors, therefore, appears to reflect present or past social conditions of our species. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

与非人类灵长类动物不同,人类在支配地位方面的个体差异并不表现为广泛的人格因素。这可能是由于用于研究人类和非人类灵长类动物性格的问卷存在差异。或者,这可能反映了人类和非人类灵长类动物在人格组织上的差异。为了确定哪一种可能性最大,研究人员招募了1147名参与者,并要求他们在类人猿人格问卷(HPQ)上对自己和/或其他人的人格进行打分,该问卷已被用于研究非人类灵长类动物的人格。这些参与者中的很大一部分(约80%)还完成了用于测量人类性格的三份问卷中的一份自我和/或评分报告。对HPQ评分报告数据进行探索性因素分析,得出五个因素。这些因素与用于研究人类个性的问卷中的尺度主要以预期的方式相关。对HPQ自述数据的探索性因素分析没有得出明确的因素数量,也没有一致的证据表明存在优势因素。随后的分析将代表黑猩猩、倭黑猩猩、山地大猩猩和猩猩的优势因素的HPQ量表与来自修订NEO人格量表的量表进行了比较,其中包括无畏的优势,它结合了神经质、随和性、尽责性和外向性方面、情绪稳定性(与神经质相反)和外向性的自信方面。无所畏惧的优势和自信最接近类人猿的优势因素。因此,人类主导因素的缺失似乎反映了我们这个物种现在或过去的社会状况。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Dominance in human (Homo sapiens) personality space and in hominoid phylogeny.","authors":"Alexander Weiss","doi":"10.1037/com0000322","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000322","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Unlike nonhuman primates, individual differences between humans in dominance do not appear as broad personality factors. This may be attributable to differences between the questionnaires used to study human and nonhuman primate personality. Alternatively, this may reflect differences in the organization of personality in humans and nonhuman primates. To determine which of these possibilities was most likely, 1,147 participants were recruited and asked to rate their personality and/or that of somebody else on the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire (HPQ), which has been used to study nonhuman primate personality. A large subset of these participants (~80%) also completed self- and/or rater reports of one of three questionnaires used to measure human personality. Exploratory factor analyses of HPQ rater report data yielded five factors. These factors correlated mostly in expected ways with scales from questionnaires used to study human personality. Exploratory factor analyses of HPQ self-report data yielded no clear number of factors and no consistent evidence with respect to the presence of a dominance factor. Subsequent analyses compared HPQ scales that represented dominance factors in chimpanzees, bonobos, mountain gorillas, and orangutans to scales derived from the Revised NEO Personality Inventory, including Fearless Dominance, which combined Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Extraversion facets, Emotional Stability (the inverse of Neuroticism), and Extraversion's Assertiveness facet. Fearless Dominance and Assertiveness were most like the great ape dominance factors. The absence of human dominance factors, therefore, appears to reflect present or past social conditions of our species. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10531690","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
What is simple is actually quite complex: A critical note on terminology in the domain of language and communication. 简单的东西实际上很复杂:语言和交流领域术语的关键注释。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000328
Limor Raviv, Louise R Peckre, Cedric Boeckx

On the surface, the fields of animal communication and human linguistics have arrived at conflicting theories and conclusions with respect to the effect of social complexity on communicative complexity. For example, an increase in group size is argued to have opposite consequences on human versus animal communication systems: although an increase in human community size leads to some types of language simplification, an increase in animal group size leads to an increase in signal complexity. But do human and animal communication systems really show such a fundamental discrepancy? Our key message is that the tension between these two adjacent fields is the result of (a) a focus on different levels of analysis (namely, signal variation or grammar-like rules) and (b) an inconsistent use of terminology (namely, the terms "simple" and "complex"). By disentangling and clarifying these terms with respect to different measures of communicative complexity, we show that although animal and human communication systems indeed show some contradictory effects with respect to signal variability, they actually display essentially the same patterns with respect to grammar-like structure. This is despite the fact that the definitions of complexity and simplicity are actually aligned for signal variability, but diverge for grammatical structure. We conclude by advocating for the use of more objective and descriptive terms instead of terms such as "complexity," which can be applied uniformly for human and animal communication systems-leading to comparable descriptions of findings across species and promoting a more productive dialogue between fields. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

从表面上看,关于社会复杂性对交际复杂性的影响,动物交际领域和人类语言学领域已经得出了相互矛盾的理论和结论。例如,人们认为群体规模的增加会对人类和动物的交流系统产生相反的影响:尽管人类群体规模的增加会导致某些类型的语言简化,但动物群体规模的增加会导致信号复杂性的增加。但是,人类和动物的交流系统真的表现出如此根本的差异吗?我们的关键信息是,这两个相邻领域之间的紧张关系是(a)关注不同层次的分析(即信号变化或类似语法的规则)和(b)术语使用不一致(即术语“简单”和“复杂”)的结果。通过将这些术语从不同的交流复杂性度量中分离出来并加以澄清,我们表明,尽管动物和人类的交流系统在信号变异性方面确实表现出一些相互矛盾的影响,但它们实际上在类似语法的结构方面表现出本质上相同的模式。尽管事实上,复杂性和简单性的定义实际上是一致的信号变异性,但分歧的语法结构。最后,我们提倡使用更客观和描述性的术语,而不是像“复杂性”这样的术语,因为“复杂性”可以统一应用于人类和动物的交流系统,从而导致跨物种发现的可比描述,并促进领域之间更有成效的对话。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"What is simple is actually quite complex: A critical note on terminology in the domain of language and communication.","authors":"Limor Raviv,&nbsp;Louise R Peckre,&nbsp;Cedric Boeckx","doi":"10.1037/com0000328","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000328","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>On the surface, the fields of animal communication and human linguistics have arrived at conflicting theories and conclusions with respect to the effect of social complexity on communicative complexity. For example, an increase in group size is argued to have opposite consequences on human versus animal communication systems: although an increase in human community size leads to some types of language simplification, an increase in animal group size leads to an increase in signal complexity. But do human and animal communication systems really show such a fundamental discrepancy? Our key message is that the tension between these two adjacent fields is the result of (a) a focus on different levels of analysis (namely, signal variation or grammar-like rules) and (b) an inconsistent use of terminology (namely, the terms \"simple\" and \"complex\"). By disentangling and clarifying these terms with respect to different measures of communicative complexity, we show that although animal and human communication systems indeed show some contradictory effects with respect to signal variability, they actually display essentially the same patterns with respect to grammar-like structure. This is despite the fact that the definitions of complexity and simplicity are actually aligned for signal variability, but diverge for grammatical structure. We conclude by advocating for the use of more objective and descriptive terms instead of terms such as \"complexity,\" which can be applied uniformly for human and animal communication systems-leading to comparable descriptions of findings across species and promoting a more productive dialogue between fields. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10839707","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Supplemental Material for Same-Different Conceptualization in Dogs (Canis familiaris) 犬类同异概念化补充材料
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-10-24 DOI: 10.1037/com0000332.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Same-Different Conceptualization in Dogs (Canis familiaris)","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/com0000332.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000332.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"85108016","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rules and metarules: Adult cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) and 5-year-old children (Homo sapiens) can master both. 规则和基本规则:成年棉顶绢毛猴和5岁的儿童(智人)都能掌握这两种技能。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-08-01 Epub Date: 2022-06-30 DOI: 10.1037/com0000324
Dorothy Munkenbeck Fragaszy

Developmental psychologists have noted a similar timeline of change for children's use of different perspectives about the same objects or events, as in the use of different labels for the same object, an aspect of language, and in understanding other's knowledge or beliefs, an aspect of social cognition as reviewed in the study by Neiworth et al. Comparative psychologists are interested to know what cognitive flexibility looks like in other species and how such variation relates to life history, ecology, and phylogeny. The general pattern of results to date indicates that monkeys can master both intra- and interdimensional shifts, but intradimensional shifts are learned far more quickly than interdimensional shifts (reviewed in the study by Neiworth et al, 2022). Neiworth et al. report that they have conducted exactly this kind of comparative study: They examined cognitive flexibility in adult cotton-top tamarins and human children in three age groups as they participated in a modified version of the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS). Neiworth et al.'s study offers an example of careful consideration of one such possibility: that of using the experimenter's postural orientation to the cards as an inadvertent aid. Here the authors had the benefit of prior work showing that tamarins follow human-provided cues to make a spatially discriminated choice only if the experimenter's head, body, and eyes oriented to a particular location. Thus, in this study, the experimenter kept their head and body centered in the testing space between the two cards and looked at a point on the wall directly behind the midpoint of the testing tray. But the DCCS task, in abstract form, has potentially broader comparative value than to examine cognitive flexibility in primates alone. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

发展心理学家已经注意到,儿童对同一物体或事件使用不同视角的变化时间轴类似,例如对同一物体使用不同标签,这是语言的一个方面,以及在理解他人的知识或信仰,这是Neiworth等人的研究中回顾的社会认知的一个方面。比较心理学家有兴趣知道其他物种的认知灵活性是什么样的,以及这种变化与生活史、生态学和系统发育有什么关系。迄今为止的一般结果模式表明,猴子可以掌握维度内和维度间的变化,但维度内的变化比维度间的变化学习得快得多(Neiworth等人在2022年的研究中进行了回顾)。Neiworth等人报告说,他们已经进行了这种比较研究:他们检查了三个年龄组的成年棉顶狨猴和人类儿童的认知灵活性,因为他们参加了修改版本的维度变化卡片排序(DCCS)。Neiworth等人的研究提供了一个仔细考虑这种可能性的例子:使用实验者对卡片的姿势方向作为无意的辅助。在这里,作者得益于先前的研究表明,只有当实验者的头、身体和眼睛指向一个特定的位置时,狨猴才会根据人类提供的线索做出空间歧视的选择。因此,在这项研究中,实验者将他们的头和身体保持在两张卡片之间的测试空间的中心,并看着测试托盘中点后面的墙上的一个点。但是,抽象形式的DCCS任务可能比单独研究灵长类动物的认知灵活性具有更广泛的比较价值。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Rules and metarules: Adult cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) and 5-year-old children (Homo sapiens) can master both.","authors":"Dorothy Munkenbeck Fragaszy","doi":"10.1037/com0000324","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000324","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Developmental psychologists have noted a similar timeline of change for children's use of different perspectives about the same objects or events, as in the use of different labels for the same object, an aspect of language, and in understanding other's knowledge or beliefs, an aspect of social cognition as reviewed in the study by Neiworth et al. Comparative psychologists are interested to know what cognitive flexibility looks like in other species and how such variation relates to life history, ecology, and phylogeny. The general pattern of results to date indicates that monkeys can master both intra- and interdimensional shifts, but intradimensional shifts are learned far more quickly than interdimensional shifts (reviewed in the study by Neiworth et al, 2022). Neiworth et al. report that they have conducted exactly this kind of comparative study: They examined cognitive flexibility in adult cotton-top tamarins and human children in three age groups as they participated in a modified version of the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS). Neiworth et al.'s study offers an example of careful consideration of one such possibility: that of using the experimenter's postural orientation to the cards as an inadvertent aid. Here the authors had the benefit of prior work showing that tamarins follow human-provided cues to make a spatially discriminated choice only if the experimenter's head, body, and eyes oriented to a particular location. Thus, in this study, the experimenter kept their head and body centered in the testing space between the two cards and looked at a point on the wall directly behind the midpoint of the testing tray. But the DCCS task, in abstract form, has potentially broader comparative value than to examine cognitive flexibility in primates alone. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40411260","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Egg burial in the ringneck dove (Streptopelia risoria): A potential laboratory model system for egg-rejection research? 环颈鸽(链颈鸽)的卵埋藏:卵子排斥研究的潜在实验室模型系统?
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-08-01 Epub Date: 2022-06-30 DOI: 10.1037/com0000318
Melissa Burns-Cusato, Arpit Rana, Will Hawkins, Zach Young, Mark E Hauber

In avian brood parasitism, parasites lay their eggs in the nests of hosts, and many hosts in the wild respond by eliminating or abandoning foreign eggs in their clutch. However, a limitation upon the study of proximate, especially physiological and experience-dependent cognitive mechanisms of egg rejection, has been the lack of a suitable model system in captivity. Here, we tested whether laboratory-kept ringneck doves (Streptopelia risoria) respond to visually distinct egg types (through applying an ink treatment upon the doves' own eggs) by rejecting them. We found that in two of two experiments, brown eggs were more often rejected, through predominantly egg burial, relative to control eggs but were done so only by a subset of dove pairs. These results are supportive of ringneck doves to become a suitable captive model for the study of foreign-egg rejection, and open the way for future research on the integrative (e.g., genetic, endocrine, ontogenetic, and cognitive) study of egg-rejection responses in a tractable research system. However, the ecological validity and applicability of this model system for the analysis of host-parasite interactions in the wild remain narrowly limited. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

在鸟类的巢寄生中,寄生虫将卵产在寄主的巢中,而许多野生寄主的反应是消灭或抛弃它们的窝中的外来卵。然而,对卵子排斥的直接,特别是生理和经验依赖的认知机制的研究的限制是缺乏合适的圈养模型系统。在这里,我们测试了实验室饲养的环颈鸽(链颈鸽)是否对视觉上不同的卵类型(通过在鸽子自己的卵上涂上墨水处理)做出反应,拒绝它们。我们发现,在两个实验中的两个中,相对于对照蛋,棕色蛋更经常被拒绝,主要是通过鸡蛋埋葬,但只有一小部分鸽子对这样做。这些结果支持环颈鸽成为研究外来卵子排斥反应的合适圈养模型,并为未来在一个可处理的研究系统中对卵子排斥反应的综合研究(如遗传、内分泌、个体发生和认知)开辟了道路。然而,该模型系统在分析野生宿主-寄生虫相互作用方面的生态学有效性和适用性仍然有限。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Egg burial in the ringneck dove (Streptopelia risoria): A potential laboratory model system for egg-rejection research?","authors":"Melissa Burns-Cusato,&nbsp;Arpit Rana,&nbsp;Will Hawkins,&nbsp;Zach Young,&nbsp;Mark E Hauber","doi":"10.1037/com0000318","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000318","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In avian brood parasitism, parasites lay their eggs in the nests of hosts, and many hosts in the wild respond by eliminating or abandoning foreign eggs in their clutch. However, a limitation upon the study of proximate, especially physiological and experience-dependent cognitive mechanisms of egg rejection, has been the lack of a suitable model system in captivity. Here, we tested whether laboratory-kept ringneck doves <i>(Streptopelia risoria)</i> respond to visually distinct egg types (through applying an ink treatment upon the doves' own eggs) by rejecting them. We found that in two of two experiments, brown eggs were more often rejected, through predominantly egg burial, relative to control eggs but were done so only by a subset of dove pairs. These results are supportive of ringneck doves to become a suitable captive model for the study of foreign-egg rejection, and open the way for future research on the integrative (e.g., genetic, endocrine, ontogenetic, and cognitive) study of egg-rejection responses in a tractable research system. However, the ecological validity and applicability of this model system for the analysis of host-parasite interactions in the wild remain narrowly limited. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40411259","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The "avoid the empty cup" hypothesis does not explain great apes' (Gorilla gorilla, Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Pongo abelii) responses in two three-cup one-item inference by exclusion tasks. “避免空杯子”假说并不能解释类人猿(大猩猩、大猩猩、类人猿、类人猿)在排除任务中对两个三杯一项推理的反应。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-08-01 Epub Date: 2022-06-30 DOI: 10.1037/com0000321
Josep Call

In the two-cup one-item task, subjects are shown a food item, which is then hidden inside one of two cups. Several species spontaneously select the baited cup above chance levels if shown that the other cup is empty. Although this response may indicate inference by exclusion (if not A, then B), another possibility is that subjects simply avoid choosing the empty cup, not because they expect the food to be in the other cup but because they have seen that cup to be empty. I tested whether this hypothesis explains great apes' responses in a three-cup one-item task. Subjects saw three opaque cups on a platform, with two of them located behind a barrier during baiting. After baiting one of the cups behind the barrier, I revealed the identity of the empty cup that had been located behind the barrier (Experiment 1) or revealed the contents of the center cup (baited in half of the trials), but always removed it before the subjects' choice (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, subjects preferentially selected the baited cup even though one of the other two cups had not been shown to be empty. In Experiment 2, subjects' preference for the cup that had been located behind the barrier during baiting was modulated by the contents of the removed cup. These results suggest that expectations about the food's location, not just the sight of the empty cup, as postulated by the "avoid the empty cup" hypothesis, determine apes' responses in the three-cup one-item task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

在两个杯子一项的任务中,研究人员向受试者展示一种食物,然后将食物藏在两个杯子中的一个里。如果显示另一个杯子是空的,一些物种会自发地选择有诱饵的杯子。虽然这种反应可能表明排除推理(如果不是A,那么是B),但另一种可能性是,受试者只是避免选择空杯子,不是因为他们期望食物在另一个杯子里,而是因为他们看到那个杯子是空的。我测试了这一假设是否能解释类人猿在三杯一项任务中的反应。实验对象在一个平台上看到三个不透明的杯子,其中两个放在障碍物后面。在把其中一个杯子放在屏障后面后,我告诉他们屏障后面空杯子的身份(实验1),或者告诉他们中间杯子的内容(在一半的实验中是诱饵),但总是在被试做出选择之前把它拿掉(实验2)。在实验1中,被试优先选择被诱饵的杯子,即使其他两个杯子中的一个没有被证明是空的。在实验2中,受试者对诱饵放置在屏障后面的杯子的偏好受到被移走的杯子的内容的调节。这些结果表明,对食物位置的预期,而不仅仅是看到空杯子,正如“避免空杯子”假设所假设的那样,决定了猿在三杯一件任务中的反应。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"The \"avoid the empty cup\" hypothesis does not explain great apes' (Gorilla gorilla, Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Pongo abelii) responses in two three-cup one-item inference by exclusion tasks.","authors":"Josep Call","doi":"10.1037/com0000321","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000321","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the two-cup one-item task, subjects are shown a food item, which is then hidden inside one of two cups. Several species spontaneously select the baited cup above chance levels if shown that the other cup is empty. Although this response may indicate inference by exclusion (if not A, then B), another possibility is that subjects simply avoid choosing the empty cup, not because they expect the food to be in the other cup but because they have seen that cup to be empty. I tested whether this hypothesis explains great apes' responses in a three-cup one-item task. Subjects saw three opaque cups on a platform, with two of them located behind a barrier during baiting. After baiting one of the cups behind the barrier, I revealed the identity of the empty cup that had been located behind the barrier (Experiment 1) or revealed the contents of the center cup (baited in half of the trials), but always removed it before the subjects' choice (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, subjects preferentially selected the baited cup even though one of the other two cups had not been shown to be empty. In Experiment 2, subjects' preference for the cup that had been located behind the barrier during baiting was modulated by the contents of the removed cup. These results suggest that expectations about the food's location, not just the sight of the empty cup, as postulated by the \"avoid the empty cup\" hypothesis, determine apes' responses in the three-cup one-item task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40410915","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Investigation of mirror-self recognition in ravens (Corvus corax). 乌鸦镜像自我识别的研究。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-08-01 Epub Date: 2022-06-30 DOI: 10.1037/com0000319
Luigi Baciadonna, Georgia M Jerwood, Benjamin G Farrar, Nicola S Clayton, Nathan J Emery

Large-brained birds, such as corvids and parrots, tend to fail tests for self-recognition (mirror self-recognition [MSR]), but the limited positive evidence for MSR in these species has been questioned due to methodological limitations. In the present study, we aimed to investigate MSR in ravens by performing three mirror tests: a mirror exposure test, a mirror preference test, and a mark test. Across all three tests, the ravens' behavior was not consistent with MSR. Three out of six ravens infrequently interacted with the mirror and the nonmirror surfaces. Two birds explored the mirror and occasionally displayed contingent behaviors. Finally, the ravens made very few social displays toward the mirror, suggesting that at this stage they did not treat their reflection as a conspecific. These findings, along with the current evidence available, raise further questions on the validity of relying on one test to establish self-recognition and call for the development of methods beyond mirror tests to explore self-recognition in nonhuman animals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

大脑袋的鸟类,如鸦和鹦鹉,往往不能通过自我识别(镜像自我识别[MSR])的测试,但由于方法的限制,这些物种的镜像自我识别有限的积极证据受到质疑。在本研究中,我们旨在通过三种镜像测试来研究乌鸦的MSR:镜像暴露测试、镜像偏好测试和标记测试。在所有三个测试中,乌鸦的行为与MSR不一致。六只乌鸦中有三只很少与镜子和非镜子表面互动。两只鸟探索镜子,偶尔表现出偶然的行为。最后,渡鸦很少对着镜子进行社交展示,这表明在这个阶段,它们并不把自己的倒影当作同物。这些发现,连同现有的证据,对依赖一个测试来建立自我识别的有效性提出了进一步的问题,并呼吁开发超越镜像测试的方法来探索非人类动物的自我识别。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Investigation of mirror-self recognition in ravens (Corvus corax).","authors":"Luigi Baciadonna,&nbsp;Georgia M Jerwood,&nbsp;Benjamin G Farrar,&nbsp;Nicola S Clayton,&nbsp;Nathan J Emery","doi":"10.1037/com0000319","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000319","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Large-brained birds, such as corvids and parrots, tend to fail tests for self-recognition (mirror self-recognition [MSR]), but the limited positive evidence for MSR in these species has been questioned due to methodological limitations. In the present study, we aimed to investigate MSR in ravens by performing three mirror tests: a mirror exposure test, a mirror preference test, and a mark test. Across all three tests, the ravens' behavior was not consistent with MSR. Three out of six ravens infrequently interacted with the mirror and the nonmirror surfaces. Two birds explored the mirror and occasionally displayed contingent behaviors. Finally, the ravens made very few social displays toward the mirror, suggesting that at this stage they did not treat their reflection as a conspecific. These findings, along with the current evidence available, raise further questions on the validity of relying on one test to establish self-recognition and call for the development of methods beyond mirror tests to explore self-recognition in nonhuman animals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40411258","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A modified version of the dimensional change card sort task tests cognitive flexibility in children (Homo sapiens) and cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). 改良版的维度变化卡片分类任务测试了儿童(智人)和棉顶狨(Saguinus oedipus)的认知灵活性。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-08-01 Epub Date: 2022-03-21 DOI: 10.1037/com0000312
Julie J Neiworth, Marie T Balaban, Kate Wagner, Alexandria Carlsen, Sarah Min, Ye In Christopher Kwon, Isabelle Rieth

A modified Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) task was used to test cognitive flexibility in adult cotton-top tamarins and children aged 19 months to 60 months. Subjects had to infer a rule from the experience of selecting between two cards to earn a reward, and the pairs of stimuli defined the rule (e.g., pick blue ones, not red ones, or pick trucks, not boats). Two different tests measured subjects' ability to shift to a reversal of the rule (intradimensional shift) and to shift to a new rule defined by a dimension previously irrelevant (interdimensional shift). Both adult tamarins and children aged 49-60 months were able to learn the initial rule and switch to a reversal and to a rule based on a different dimension. In contrast, the two younger groups of children, aged 19-36 months and aged 37-48 months, could switch when a reversal was imposed but took significantly longer to learn a new rule on a former irrelevant dimension. Experiment 2 presented a wider set of novel stimuli which shared some features with the original set to further explore the basis of rule learning. The result was that tamarins and 52- to 60-month-old children both chose novel stimuli that fit the rule and had no a priori associative strength, suggesting a rule application not solely based on associative strength. Importantly, novel items introduced some risk for choice, and children showed themselves to be risk-averse, whereas tamarins were risk-prone within a novel context. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

研究人员采用改良的维度变化卡片排序(DCCS)任务来测试成年棉顶狨和 19 个月至 60 个月大儿童的认知灵活性。受试者必须从在两张牌中进行选择以获得奖励的经验中推断出一条规则,而成双成对的刺激则定义了规则(例如,选蓝色的,不选红色的;或选卡车,不选船)。两种不同的测试分别测量受试者转换到规则逆转(维度内转换)和转换到由以前无关的维度定义的新规则(维度间转换)的能力。成年狨猴和 49-60 个月大的儿童都能学会初始规则,并转换到逆转规则和基于不同维度的规则。相比之下,年龄分别为 19-36 个月和 37-48 个月的两组年龄较小的儿童,则可以在实施逆转时进行转换,但学习以前不相关维度的新规则所需的时间要长得多。实验 2 提供了一组更广泛的新刺激物,这些刺激物与原来的刺激物有一些共同的特征,以进一步探究规则学习的基础。结果发现,犭胥和 52 至 60 个月大的儿童都选择了符合规则且没有先验联想强度的新刺激物,这表明规则的应用并非完全基于联想强度。重要的是,新颖的刺激物会给儿童的选择带来一定的风险,儿童表现出规避风险的倾向,而犭胥则在新颖的情境中容易出现风险。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"A modified version of the dimensional change card sort task tests cognitive flexibility in children (Homo sapiens) and cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus).","authors":"Julie J Neiworth, Marie T Balaban, Kate Wagner, Alexandria Carlsen, Sarah Min, Ye In Christopher Kwon, Isabelle Rieth","doi":"10.1037/com0000312","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000312","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A modified Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) task was used to test cognitive flexibility in adult cotton-top tamarins and children aged 19 months to 60 months. Subjects had to infer a rule from the experience of selecting between two cards to earn a reward, and the pairs of stimuli defined the rule (e.g., pick blue ones, not red ones, or pick trucks, not boats). Two different tests measured subjects' ability to shift to a reversal of the rule (intradimensional shift) and to shift to a new rule defined by a dimension previously irrelevant (interdimensional shift). Both adult tamarins and children aged 49-60 months were able to learn the initial rule and switch to a reversal and to a rule based on a different dimension. In contrast, the two younger groups of children, aged 19-36 months and aged 37-48 months, could switch when a reversal was imposed but took significantly longer to learn a new rule on a former irrelevant dimension. Experiment 2 presented a wider set of novel stimuli which shared some features with the original set to further explore the basis of rule learning. The result was that tamarins and 52- to 60-month-old children both chose novel stimuli that fit the rule and had no a priori associative strength, suggesting a rule application not solely based on associative strength. Importantly, novel items introduced some risk for choice, and children showed themselves to be risk-averse, whereas tamarins were risk-prone within a novel context. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11131561/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40310360","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Investigation of Mirror-Self Recognition in Ravens (Corvus corax) 乌鸦(Corvus corax)镜像自我识别研究补充材料
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-06-27 DOI: 10.1037/com0000319.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Investigation of Mirror-Self Recognition in Ravens (Corvus corax)","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/com0000319.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000319.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-06-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87550901","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Inhibitory control and cue relevance modulate chimpanzees' (Pan troglodytes) performance in a spatial foraging task. 抑制控制和线索关联调节黑猩猩在空间觅食任务中的表现。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-05-01 Epub Date: 2022-03-24 DOI: 10.1037/com0000313
Christoph J Völter, Brandon Tinklenberg, Josep Call, Amanda M Seed

Inhibition tasks usually require subjects to exert control to act correctly when a competing action plan is prepotent. In comparative psychology, one concern about the existing inhibition tasks is that the relative contribution of inhibitory control to performance (as compared to learning or object knowledge) is rarely explicitly investigated. We addressed this problem by presenting chimpanzees with a spatial foraging task in which they could acquire food more efficiently by learning which objects were baited. In Experiment 1, we examined how objects that elicited a prepotent approach response, transparent cups containing food, affected their learning rates. Although showing an initial bias to approach these sealed cups with visible food, the chimpanzees learned to avoid them more quickly across sessions compared to a color discrimination. They also learned a color discrimination more quickly if the incorrect cups were sealed such that a piece of food could never be hidden inside them. In Experiment 2, visible food of 2 different types was sealed in the upper part of the cups: 1 type signaled the presence of food reward hidden underneath; the cups with the other type were sealed. The chimpanzees learned more quickly in a congruent condition (the to-be-chosen food cue matched the reward) than in an incongruent condition (the to-be-avoided food cue matched the reward). Together, these findings highlight that performance in inhibition tasks is affected by several other cognitive abilities such as object knowledge, memory, and learning, which need to be quantified before meaningful comparisons can be drawn. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

抑制任务通常要求受试者在竞争性行动计划具有优势时施加控制以正确行动。在比较心理学中,对现有抑制任务的一个关注是,抑制控制对表现的相对贡献(与学习或对象知识相比)很少得到明确的研究。我们通过给黑猩猩一个空间觅食任务来解决这个问题,在这个任务中,它们可以通过学习诱饵来更有效地获取食物。在实验1中,我们研究了那些能引起强势接近反应的物体,比如装有食物的透明杯子,是如何影响他们的学习速度的。尽管黑猩猩最初倾向于接近这些装有可见食物的密封杯子,但与颜色歧视相比,它们学会了在整个过程中更快地避开它们。如果把不正确的杯子密封起来,这样食物就永远不会藏在杯子里,它们还能更快地学会辨别颜色。在实验2中,两种不同类型的可见食物被密封在杯子的上部:一种类型表示隐藏在下面的食物奖励的存在;另一种杯子是密封的。黑猩猩在一致的条件下(待选择的食物线索与奖励相匹配)比在不一致的条件下(待避免的食物线索与奖励相匹配)学习得更快。总之,这些发现强调了抑制任务的表现受到其他几种认知能力的影响,如对象知识、记忆和学习,这些能力需要在进行有意义的比较之前进行量化。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Inhibitory control and cue relevance modulate chimpanzees' (Pan troglodytes) performance in a spatial foraging task.","authors":"Christoph J Völter,&nbsp;Brandon Tinklenberg,&nbsp;Josep Call,&nbsp;Amanda M Seed","doi":"10.1037/com0000313","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000313","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Inhibition tasks usually require subjects to exert control to act correctly when a competing action plan is prepotent. In comparative psychology, one concern about the existing inhibition tasks is that the relative contribution of inhibitory control to performance (as compared to learning or object knowledge) is rarely explicitly investigated. We addressed this problem by presenting chimpanzees with a spatial foraging task in which they could acquire food more efficiently by learning which objects were baited. In Experiment 1, we examined how objects that elicited a prepotent approach response, transparent cups containing food, affected their learning rates. Although showing an initial bias to approach these sealed cups with visible food, the chimpanzees learned to avoid them more quickly across sessions compared to a color discrimination. They also learned a color discrimination more quickly if the incorrect cups were sealed such that a piece of food could never be hidden inside them. In Experiment 2, visible food of 2 different types was sealed in the upper part of the cups: 1 type signaled the presence of food reward hidden underneath; the cups with the other type were sealed. The chimpanzees learned more quickly in a congruent condition (the to-be-chosen food cue matched the reward) than in an incongruent condition (the to-be-avoided food cue matched the reward). Together, these findings highlight that performance in inhibition tasks is affected by several other cognitive abilities such as object knowledge, memory, and learning, which need to be quantified before meaningful comparisons can be drawn. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40319621","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Journal of Comparative Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1