首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Comparative Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Editorial. 社论
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000370
Michael J Beran

The Journal of Comparative Psychology (JCP) is the flagship APA journal dedicated to understanding psychological processes from a comparative perspective. Traditionally, "comparative" has meant comparison across species. However, "comparative" means more than just assessing as many species as possible or relating species to each other. I also think of the importance of a "comparative psychology" perspective in two other ways that should be reflected in the journal's publications. I would like to outline a few important points about how I view the mission of JCP, and how my term as chief editor will address some of the major issues that exist for the journal and for the field of comparative psychology more broadly. Preregistration, replication, and the review process are highlighted. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

比较心理学杂志》(JCP)是美国心理学会的旗舰杂志,致力于从比较的角度理解心理过程。传统上,"比较 "意味着跨物种比较。然而,"比较 "不仅仅意味着尽可能多地评估物种或将物种相互联系起来。我还认为,"比较心理学 "视角的重要性还体现在其他两个方面,这一点应该在期刊的出版物中得到体现。我想就我如何看待 JCP 的使命,以及我在担任主编期间将如何解决该期刊和更广泛的比较心理学领域存在的一些主要问题,概述几个要点。其中特别强调了预注册、复制和审稿流程。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Editorial.","authors":"Michael J Beran","doi":"10.1037/com0000370","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000370","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The <i>Journal of Comparative Psychology</i> (JCP) is the flagship APA journal dedicated to understanding psychological processes from a comparative perspective. Traditionally, \"comparative\" has meant comparison across species. However, \"comparative\" means more than just assessing as many species as possible or relating species to each other. I also think of the importance of a \"comparative psychology\" perspective in two other ways that should be reflected in the journal's publications. I would like to outline a few important points about how I view the mission of JCP, and how my term as chief editor will address some of the major issues that exist for the journal and for the field of comparative psychology more broadly. Preregistration, replication, and the review process are highlighted. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"138 1","pages":"1-4"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140307908","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A study of executive function in grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus): Experience can affect delay of gratification. 灰鹦鹉(Psittacus erithacus)的执行功能研究:经验会影响延迟满足感
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1037/com0000361
Irene M Pepperberg, Leigh Ann Hartsfield

Executive function (EF) involves several abilities often correlated with success in various aspects of human life. Similar skills could also be advantageous to nonhumans, but few studies have effectively examined the extent of their EF abilities. Studies have also examined what experiences might strengthen/weaken human EF; might specific experiences also affect nonhuman EF? One type of EF often tested in both humans and nonhumans involves a delay of gratification-the ability to forgo an immediate reward to gain one either better in quality or quantity. We compared how Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) with somewhat different experimental histories performed when required to wait for more food-a difficult task for avian species. One laboratory-raised parrot, Griffin, had previously succeeded when asked to wait for a better reward and on other tasks purportedly involving some level of EF skills but failed to wait for a larger reward. After succeeding on a task designed to improve impulse control, he consistently waited for a larger reward, more nuts, for up to 15 min-far longer than most avian species tested. Two other parrots, Pepper and Franco, companion animals, had had no experience with delayed gratification tasks, but were as successful as Griffin on other EF-related studies in which they participated. These birds, with different histories than Griffin, also waited for more food for longer periods than most other birds, though not as consistently as Griffin. We suggest that specific types of experiences may strengthen EF in Grey parrots. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

执行功能(EF)涉及多种能力,通常与人类生活各方面的成功息息相关。类似的技能对非人类也可能是有利的,但很少有研究有效地考察了非人类的执行功能。研究还探讨了哪些经历可能会增强/削弱人类的 EF;特定的经历是否也会影响非人类的 EF?在人类和非人类中经常测试的一种 EF 包括延迟满足--放弃即时奖励以获得质量或数量更好的奖励的能力。我们比较了具有不同实验经历的灰鹦鹉(Psittacus erithacus)在被要求等待更多食物时的表现--这对鸟类来说是一项艰巨的任务。其中一只在实验室饲养的鹦鹉格里芬曾在被要求等待更好的奖励和其他据称涉及某种程度的 EF 技能的任务中取得成功,但在等待更大的奖励时却失败了。在成功完成一项旨在提高冲动控制能力的任务后,它一直在等待更大的奖励、更多的坚果,等待时间长达15分钟,远远超过了大多数被测试的鸟类。另外两只鹦鹉佩珀(Pepper)和弗朗科(Franco)是伴侣动物,它们都没有完成延迟满足任务的经验,但在它们参与的其他与EF相关的研究中,它们和格里芬一样成功。这两只鸟的经历与格里芬不同,它们等待食物的时间也比其他大多数鸟类更长,但不如格里芬那么持久。我们认为,特定类型的经历可能会增强灰鹦鹉的 EF。 (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"A study of executive function in grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus): Experience can affect delay of gratification.","authors":"Irene M Pepperberg, Leigh Ann Hartsfield","doi":"10.1037/com0000361","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000361","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Executive function (EF) involves several abilities often correlated with success in various aspects of human life. Similar skills could also be advantageous to nonhumans, but few studies have effectively examined the extent of their EF abilities. Studies have also examined what experiences might strengthen/weaken human EF; might specific experiences also affect nonhuman EF? One type of EF often tested in both humans and nonhumans involves a delay of gratification-the ability to forgo an immediate reward to gain one either better in quality or quantity. We compared how Grey parrots <i>(Psittacus erithacus)</i> with somewhat different experimental histories performed when required to wait for more food-a difficult task for avian species. One laboratory-raised parrot, Griffin, had previously succeeded when asked to wait for a better reward and on other tasks purportedly involving some level of EF skills but failed to wait for a larger reward. After succeeding on a task designed to improve impulse control, he consistently waited for a larger reward, more nuts, for up to 15 min-far longer than most avian species tested. Two other parrots, Pepper and Franco, companion animals, had had no experience with delayed gratification tasks, but were as successful as Griffin on other EF-related studies in which they participated. These birds, with different histories than Griffin, also waited for more food for longer periods than most other birds, though not as consistently as Griffin. We suggest that specific types of experiences may strengthen EF in Grey parrots. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"8-19"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10321004","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On parrots, delay of gratification, executive function, and how sometimes we do the best we can. 关于鹦鹉、延迟满足、执行功能,以及有时我们如何尽力而为。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000378
Michael J Beran

Engaging executive functions provides an individual with the means to engage in cognitive control by adjusting to the environment and processing information in a way that leads to optimal outcomes. There are some claims that explicit training on certain executive functioning abilities provides benefits beyond the training tasks, but other studies indicate that this may not be true or may be limited based on age and other factors. This same mixed pattern has been reported with nonhuman species, where training or even experience in one specific area, like inhibition, sometimes leads to positive transfer in new but similar tasks that presumably also require executive functions. Pepperberg and Hartsfield (2024) sought to determine whether experience in previous tasks that required different executive functions impacted how well three African grey parrots: Griffin, Pepper, and Franco could perform in a new assessment of delayed gratification. Griffin showed a clear and consistent capacity to wait through a delay for a quantitatively better reward. This suggested that the previous experience with the tokens aided improvement in the quantitative delay of gratification task with food items as the options to choose between. The other two parrots, Pepper and Franco, never completed the intended sequence of phases in their study. Unfortunately, the testing conditions dictated by COVID restrictions were such that these two subjects appeared to exhibit stress in doing the task, and so no further testing was conducted with them. This article is an example of what can happen when two intelligent species (people and parrots) are put in difficult circumstances (a global pandemic unlike anything any of us has ever been through), and yet both species attempted to continue to engage in science. The effects of COVID-19 will remain an integral factor in comparative psychology for some time to come, and I suspect there are many other half-completed experiments that suffered because of the pandemic. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

执行功能为个人提供了一种手段,通过适应环境和处理信息来进行认知控制,从而获得最佳结果。有人声称,对某些执行功能能力的明确训练会带来训练任务之外的益处,但其他研究表明,这种说法可能并不正确,或可能因年龄和其他因素而受到限制。在非人类物种中也有这种混合模式的报道,在某一特定领域(如抑制)的训练甚至经验有时会导致在新的类似任务中的积极迁移,而这些任务可能也需要执行功能。佩珀伯格和哈茨菲尔德(2024 年)试图确定,以往需要不同执行功能的任务经验是否会影响三只非洲灰鹦鹉的表现:格里芬、佩珀和弗兰科在一项新的延迟满足评估中的表现如何。格里芬表现出明显而持续的能力,能够通过延迟等待获得数量上更好的奖励。这表明,以前使用代币的经验有助于提高在以食物为选项的定量延迟满足任务中的表现。另外两只鹦鹉佩珀和弗兰科在研究中从未完成预定的阶段顺序。不幸的是,受 COVID 限制的测试条件使这两只受试者在完成任务时似乎表现出了压力,因此没有对它们进行进一步的测试。这篇文章举例说明了当两种聪明的物种(人和鹦鹉)被置于困难的环境中(我们从未经历过的全球大流行),但这两种物种都试图继续从事科学研究时会发生什么。在未来的一段时间里,COVID-19 的影响仍将是比较心理学中不可或缺的因素,我猜想还有许多其他半途而废的实验也会因为这场大流行病而受到影响。(PsycInfo 数据库记录 (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"On parrots, delay of gratification, executive function, and how sometimes we do the best we can.","authors":"Michael J Beran","doi":"10.1037/com0000378","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000378","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Engaging executive functions provides an individual with the means to engage in cognitive control by adjusting to the environment and processing information in a way that leads to optimal outcomes. There are some claims that explicit training on certain executive functioning abilities provides benefits beyond the training tasks, but other studies indicate that this may not be true or may be limited based on age and other factors. This same mixed pattern has been reported with nonhuman species, where training or even experience in one specific area, like inhibition, sometimes leads to positive transfer in new but similar tasks that presumably also require executive functions. Pepperberg and Hartsfield (2024) sought to determine whether experience in previous tasks that required different executive functions impacted how well three African grey parrots: Griffin, Pepper, and Franco could perform in a new assessment of delayed gratification. Griffin showed a clear and consistent capacity to wait through a delay for a quantitatively better reward. This suggested that the previous experience with the tokens aided improvement in the quantitative delay of gratification task with food items as the options to choose between. The other two parrots, Pepper and Franco, never completed the intended sequence of phases in their study. Unfortunately, the testing conditions dictated by COVID restrictions were such that these two subjects appeared to exhibit stress in doing the task, and so no further testing was conducted with them. This article is an example of what can happen when two intelligent species (people and parrots) are put in difficult circumstances (a global pandemic unlike anything any of us has ever been through), and yet both species attempted to continue to engage in science. The effects of COVID-19 will remain an integral factor in comparative psychology for some time to come, and I suspect there are many other half-completed experiments that suffered because of the pandemic. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"138 1","pages":"5-7"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140307909","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Primate socio-ecology shapes the evolution of distinctive facial repertoires. 灵长类动物的社会生态塑造了独特面部动作的进化。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-11 DOI: 10.1037/com0000350
Brittany N Florkiewicz, Linda S Oña, Leonardo Oña, Matthew W Campbell

Primate facial musculature enables a wide variety of movements during bouts of communication, but how these movements contribute to signal construction and repertoire size is unclear. The facial mobility hypothesis suggests that morphological constraints shape the evolution of facial repertoires: species with higher facial mobility will produce larger and more complex repertoires. In contrast, the socio-ecological complexity hypothesis suggests that social needs shape the evolution of facial repertoires: as social complexity increases, so does communicative repertoire size. We tested these two hypotheses by comparing chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and gibbons (family Hylobatidae), two distantly related apes who vary in their facial mobility and social organization. While gibbons have higher facial mobility than chimpanzees, chimpanzees live in more complex social groups than gibbons. We compared the morphology and complexity of facial repertoires for both apes using Facial Action Coding Systems designed for chimpanzees and gibbons. Our comparisons were made at the level of individual muscle movements (action units [AUs]) and the level of muscle movement combinations (AU combinations). Our results show that the chimpanzee facial signaling repertoire was larger and more complex than gibbons, consistent with the socio-ecological complexity hypothesis. On average, chimpanzees produced AU combinations consisting of more morphologically distinct AUs than gibbons. Moreover, chimpanzees also produced more morphologically distinct AU combinations than gibbons, even when focusing exclusively on AUs present in both apes. Therefore, our results suggest that socio-ecological factors were more important than anatomical ones to the evolution of facial signaling repertoires in chimpanzees and gibbons. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

灵长类动物的面部肌肉在交流过程中可以做出多种多样的动作,但这些动作如何有助于信号的构建和语汇的大小尚不清楚。面部活动性假说认为,形态上的限制决定了面部动作集的进化:面部活动性较高的物种会产生更大、更复杂的动作集。与此相反,社会生态复杂性假说认为,社会需求决定了面部信号的进化:随着社会复杂性的增加,交流信号的规模也会增加。我们通过比较黑猩猩(Pan troglodytes)和长臂猿(Hylobatidae科)来验证这两种假说。长臂猿的面部活动度比黑猩猩高,但黑猩猩生活在比长臂猿更复杂的社会群体中。我们使用专为黑猩猩和长臂猿设计的面部动作编码系统,比较了这两种猿类面部动作的形态和复杂程度。我们从单个肌肉动作(动作单元 [AU])和肌肉动作组合(AU 组合)两个层面进行了比较。我们的结果表明,黑猩猩的面部信号库比长臂猿更大、更复杂,这与社会生态复杂性假说一致。平均而言,黑猩猩比长臂猿能做出更多形态各异的 AU 组合。此外,黑猩猩也比长臂猿产生了更多形态上不同的AU组合,即使只关注两种猿类都存在的AU也是如此。因此,我们的研究结果表明,在黑猩猩和长臂猿面部信号系统的进化过程中,社会生态因素比解剖学因素更为重要。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Primate socio-ecology shapes the evolution of distinctive facial repertoires.","authors":"Brittany N Florkiewicz, Linda S Oña, Leonardo Oña, Matthew W Campbell","doi":"10.1037/com0000350","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000350","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Primate facial musculature enables a wide variety of movements during bouts of communication, but how these movements contribute to signal construction and repertoire size is unclear. The <i>facial mobility hypothesis</i> suggests that morphological constraints shape the evolution of facial repertoires: species with higher facial mobility will produce larger and more complex repertoires. In contrast, the <i>socio-ecological complexity hypothesis</i> suggests that social needs shape the evolution of facial repertoires: as social complexity increases, so does communicative repertoire size. We tested these two hypotheses by comparing chimpanzees (<i>Pan troglodytes</i>) and gibbons (family <i>Hylobatidae</i>), two distantly related apes who vary in their facial mobility and social organization. While gibbons have higher facial mobility than chimpanzees, chimpanzees live in more complex social groups than gibbons. We compared the morphology and complexity of facial repertoires for both apes using Facial Action Coding Systems designed for chimpanzees and gibbons. Our comparisons were made at the level of individual muscle movements (action units [AUs]) and the level of muscle movement combinations (AU combinations). Our results show that the chimpanzee facial signaling repertoire was larger and more complex than gibbons, consistent with the <i>socio-ecological complexity hypothesis.</i> On average, chimpanzees produced AU combinations consisting of more morphologically distinct AUs than gibbons. Moreover, chimpanzees also produced more morphologically distinct AU combinations than gibbons, even when focusing exclusively on AUs present in both apes. Therefore, our results suggest that socio-ecological factors were more important than anatomical ones to the evolution of facial signaling repertoires in chimpanzees and gibbons. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"32-44"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9449335","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are chimpanzees futurists? Effects of motion lines and motion blur on the judgments of global motion direction in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). 黑猩猩是未来主义者吗?运动线和运动模糊对黑猩猩(Pan troglodytes)全局运动方向判断的影响。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-14 DOI: 10.1037/com0000348
Masaki Tomonaga, Tomoko Imura

Based on the invention and development of photography and movie in the 19th century, schools of contemporary art, such as Futurism, have emerged that express the dynamism of motion in painting. Painting techniques such as multiple stroboscopic images, motion blur, and motion lines are culturally based, but the biological basis of their perception has also been intensively investigated recently. Then what are the evolutionary origins of such pictorial representations of motion? Do nonhuman animals also have sensitivity to such representations? To address this question, we examined the effects of motion blur and motion lines on the judgments of global motion directions in chimpanzees. The results showed that the motion lines biased the chimpanzees' judgments toward the direction of motion implied by them, whereas the effect of the motion blur was either absent or weak (Experiment 1). In Experiment 2, we manipulated the length and number of motion lines to examine the effect of "speed" and "distance" in addition to the motion direction implied by the motion lines. The results showed that the effect of motion lines became stronger as the length and the number of lines increased within a specific range. These results indicate that the motion lines also imply the direction of motion in chimpanzees and provide a clue to the evolutionary basis for the pictorial representations of motion. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在 19 世纪摄影和电影发明和发展的基础上,出现了以未来主义为代表的当代艺术流派,这些流派在绘画中表现了运动的活力。多重频闪图像、运动模糊和运动线条等绘画技巧具有文化基础,但其感知的生物学基础近年来也得到了深入研究。那么,这种运动图像表现的进化起源是什么?非人类动物是否也对此类表征具有敏感性?为了解决这个问题,我们研究了运动模糊和运动线对黑猩猩全局运动方向判断的影响。结果显示,运动线使黑猩猩的判断偏向于运动线所暗示的运动方向,而运动模糊的影响要么不存在,要么很弱(实验 1)。在实验 2 中,我们改变了运动线的长度和数量,以考察除运动线暗示的运动方向之外,"速度 "和 "距离 "的影响。结果表明,在特定范围内,随着运动线长度和数量的增加,运动线的效果会越来越强。这些结果表明,黑猩猩的运动线也暗示着运动的方向,并为运动图像表征的进化基础提供了线索。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Are chimpanzees futurists? Effects of motion lines and motion blur on the judgments of global motion direction in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).","authors":"Masaki Tomonaga, Tomoko Imura","doi":"10.1037/com0000348","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000348","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Based on the invention and development of photography and movie in the 19th century, schools of contemporary art, such as Futurism, have emerged that express the dynamism of motion in painting. Painting techniques such as multiple stroboscopic images, motion blur, and motion lines are culturally based, but the biological basis of their perception has also been intensively investigated recently. Then what are the evolutionary origins of such pictorial representations of motion? Do nonhuman animals also have sensitivity to such representations? To address this question, we examined the effects of motion blur and motion lines on the judgments of global motion directions in chimpanzees. The results showed that the motion lines biased the chimpanzees' judgments toward the direction of motion implied by them, whereas the effect of the motion blur was either absent or weak (Experiment 1). In Experiment 2, we manipulated the length and number of motion lines to examine the effect of \"speed\" and \"distance\" in addition to the motion direction implied by the motion lines. The results showed that the effect of motion lines became stronger as the length and the number of lines increased within a specific range. These results indicate that the motion lines also imply the direction of motion in chimpanzees and provide a clue to the evolutionary basis for the pictorial representations of motion. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"45-55"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140133273","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reach-to-grasp kinematic signatures in Colombian spider monkeys (Ateles fusciceps rufiventris). 哥伦比亚蜘蛛猴(Ateles fusciceps rufiventris)从伸手到抓握的运动特征。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1037/com0000355
Eliza L Nelson, Megan A Taylor, Armando Del Valle, Narciso Pavon

A defining feature of most primates is a hand with five fingers. Spider monkeys are an exception because they have four fingers and no thumb. Despite the prevalence of reach-to-grasp research in primates, it is not known how the lack of a thumb affects reaching and grasping in spider monkeys. Drawing on patterns that have been well described in human adults, human infants, and other nonhuman primates, this study characterized prehension in Colombian spider monkeys (Ateles fusciceps rufiventris). Monkeys reached for two differently sized food objects and reaches were digitized offline for two-dimensional kinematic analysis. Grasp strategy was coded from video as preshaped when the hand was adjusted to grasp the food before contact, or not preshaped when the hand was adjusted to grasp the food after contact. Monkeys exhibited variability in reach smoothness that contrasted with the typical pattern seen in other adult primates and instead resembled the pattern observed in human infants. Monkeys anticipated the object to be grasped approximately half of the time. Reaches where the hand was preshaped to the object were smoother than reaches where the hand was adjusted to grasp after object contact. For the small object, reaches with preshaping were straighter than reaches without preshaping. Results are the first evidence of kinematic signatures for reach-to-grasp actions in spider monkeys. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

大多数灵长类动物的一个显著特点是手有五个手指。蜘蛛猴是一个例外,因为它们只有四个手指,没有拇指。尽管对灵长类动物的伸手抓握研究非常普遍,但人们并不清楚没有拇指对蜘蛛猴的伸手抓握有何影响。这项研究借鉴了在人类成年人、人类婴儿和其他非人灵长类动物身上已经得到充分描述的模式,对哥伦比亚蜘蛛猴(Ateles fusciceps rufiventris)的抓握能力进行了描述。猴子伸手抓取两种不同大小的食物,并对其进行离线数字化,以进行二维运动学分析。当猴子的手在接触食物之前调整为抓取食物时,抓取策略被编码为 "预成型";当猴子的手在接触食物之后调整为抓取食物时,抓取策略被编码为 "非预成型"。猴子在伸手平稳性方面表现出的变化与其他成年灵长类动物的典型模式不同,而是类似于在人类婴儿身上观察到的模式。猴子大约有一半的时间预期到物体会被抓住。与接触物体后调整手的抓取方式相比,手与物体预成形状的抓取方式更为顺畅。对于小物体,有预设形状的伸手比没有预设形状的伸手更直。这些结果首次证明了蜘蛛猴伸手抓握动作的运动学特征。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Reach-to-grasp kinematic signatures in Colombian spider monkeys (Ateles fusciceps rufiventris).","authors":"Eliza L Nelson, Megan A Taylor, Armando Del Valle, Narciso Pavon","doi":"10.1037/com0000355","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000355","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A defining feature of most primates is a hand with five fingers. Spider monkeys are an exception because they have four fingers and no thumb. Despite the prevalence of reach-to-grasp research in primates, it is not known how the lack of a thumb affects reaching and grasping in spider monkeys. Drawing on patterns that have been well described in human adults, human infants, and other nonhuman primates, this study characterized prehension in Colombian spider monkeys (<i>Ateles fusciceps rufiventris</i>). Monkeys reached for two differently sized food objects and reaches were digitized offline for two-dimensional kinematic analysis. Grasp strategy was coded from video as preshaped when the hand was adjusted to grasp the food before contact, or not preshaped when the hand was adjusted to grasp the food after contact. Monkeys exhibited variability in reach smoothness that contrasted with the typical pattern seen in other adult primates and instead resembled the pattern observed in human infants. Monkeys anticipated the object to be grasped approximately half of the time. Reaches where the hand was preshaped to the object were smoother than reaches where the hand was adjusted to grasp after object contact. For the small object, reaches with preshaping were straighter than reaches without preshaping. Results are the first evidence of kinematic signatures for reach-to-grasp actions in spider monkeys. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"56-67"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10021117","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Impulsivity as a trait in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): A systematic review and meta-analysis. 家犬(Canis familiaris)的冲动特质:系统回顾与荟萃分析。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-03 DOI: 10.1037/com0000352
Jessica Barela, Yasmin Worth, Jeffrey R Stevens

Impulsivity is a critical component of dog (Canis familiaris) behavior that owners often want to curtail. Though studies of dog impulsivity have examined their inability to wait and to inhibit inappropriate behaviors, it is not clear whether impulsivity is a behavioral trait with consistent characteristics across contexts. For this project, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis to investigate whether impulsivity exists as a behavioral trait in domestic dogs. Under a preregistered protocol, we processed over 10,000 bibliographic database records to uncover 13 articles with multiple impulsivity tasks assessed in the same subjects. Across 31 pairs of impulsivity tasks, 28 failed to detect a correlation in performance between tasks and three detected a correlation. For 15 correlations of impulsivity tasks with the owner's perception of their dog's impulsivity, 10 were not correlated, while five were correlated. A formal meta-analysis on one pair of tasks (A-not-B task and cylinder task) tested across seven different studies showed no overall correlation between the tasks. Our systematic review and meta-analysis found little indication of consistent relationships between impulsivity levels across tasks for dogs. Therefore, at the moment, we do not have good evidence of impulsivity as a behavioral trait that transfers across contexts, suggesting that perhaps we should focus on the context-specific nature of impulsivity in dogs. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

冲动是狗(Canis familiaris)行为的一个重要组成部分,狗的主人通常都想遏制这种行为。虽然对狗冲动性的研究已经考察了它们无法等待和抑制不当行为的能力,但还不清楚冲动性是否是一种在不同情况下具有一致特征的行为特征。在本项目中,我们进行了系统回顾和荟萃分析,以研究冲动是否是家犬的一种行为特征。根据预先登记的协议,我们处理了 10,000 多条书目数据库记录,发现了 13 篇文章对同一受试者的多项冲动任务进行了评估。在 31 对冲动任务中,28 项任务之间的表现未发现相关性,3 项任务之间发现了相关性。在 15 项冲动性任务与狗主人对其爱犬冲动性的感知相关的研究中,有 10 项不相关,有 5 项相关。对 7 项不同研究中的一对任务(A-not-B 任务和圆柱体任务)进行的正式荟萃分析表明,这些任务之间总体上没有相关性。我们的系统回顾和荟萃分析几乎没有发现狗的冲动水平在不同任务之间存在一致的关系。因此,目前我们还没有很好的证据表明冲动是一种可以在不同情境下转移的行为特征,这表明我们也许应该把重点放在狗冲动的特定情境性质上。 (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Impulsivity as a trait in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): A systematic review and meta-analysis.","authors":"Jessica Barela, Yasmin Worth, Jeffrey R Stevens","doi":"10.1037/com0000352","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000352","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Impulsivity is a critical component of dog (<i>Canis familiaris</i>) behavior that owners often want to curtail. Though studies of dog impulsivity have examined their inability to wait and to inhibit inappropriate behaviors, it is not clear whether impulsivity is a behavioral trait with consistent characteristics across contexts. For this project, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis to investigate whether impulsivity exists as a behavioral trait in domestic dogs. Under a preregistered protocol, we processed over 10,000 bibliographic database records to uncover 13 articles with multiple impulsivity tasks assessed in the same subjects. Across 31 pairs of impulsivity tasks, 28 failed to detect a correlation in performance between tasks and three detected a correlation. For 15 correlations of impulsivity tasks with the owner's perception of their dog's impulsivity, 10 were not correlated, while five were correlated. A formal meta-analysis on one pair of tasks (A-not-B task and cylinder task) tested across seven different studies showed no overall correlation between the tasks. Our systematic review and meta-analysis found little indication of consistent relationships between impulsivity levels across tasks for dogs. Therefore, at the moment, we do not have good evidence of impulsivity as a behavioral trait that transfers across contexts, suggesting that perhaps we should focus on the context-specific nature of impulsivity in dogs. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"20-31"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10308405","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Gaze in cats (Felis catus) and dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). 凝视猫(猫)和狗(家犬狼疮)。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-31 DOI: 10.1037/com0000359
Michael J Bogese, Angie M Johnston, Sarah-Elizabeth Byosiere

Within human-animal dyadic interactions, dog-human gaze has been identified as the crux of several important visual behaviors, such as looking back, gaze-following, and participation in an oxytocin feedback loop. It has been posited that this gaze behavior may have been motivated and sustained by cooperative relationships between dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) and humans (e.g., hunting, service roles), however, to investigate why gaze evolved, a comparison to a domesticated species that lacks a protracted history of cooperative companionship is needed: the domestic cat (Felis catus). In this study, we compare the gaze duration to owners of cats and dogs in a community science setting. We replicated previous gaze studies with dogs, wolves (Nagasawa et al., 2015), and dingoes (Johnston et al., 2017), requesting owners to sit with their pets for 5 min and interact as they normally would. Cats and dogs gazed at their owners for similar durations, but durations of petting and physical contact were significantly lower with cats. Gaze correlated significantly with vocalizations in dogs; however, no other correlations were significant. Dogs gazed less in our community science setting than dogs tested previously in-lab (Nagasawa et al., 2015). Ultimately, cats resemble dogs in their general gaze patterns, but not in most interactions with their owner. Future research should aim to include feral cats or wild cat species to shed light on gaze behavior development in the genus, while more community science work can identify the behaviors that shift for dogs between familiar and unfamiliar environments. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在人与动物之间的互动中,狗与人之间的凝视被认为是几种重要视觉行为的关键,如回望、凝视追随和参与催产素反馈回路。有人认为,狗(Canis lupus familiaris)与人类之间的合作关系(如狩猎、服务角色)可能促使并维持了这种凝视行为,然而,要研究凝视行为进化的原因,需要与缺乏长期合作陪伴历史的驯化物种--家猫(Felis catus)--进行比较。在这项研究中,我们比较了社区科学环境中猫和狗主人的注视持续时间。我们复制了之前对狗、狼(Nagasawa 等人,2015 年)和野狗(Johnston 等人,2017 年)的凝视研究,要求主人与宠物坐在一起 5 分钟,并像平常一样进行互动。猫和狗注视主人的时间相似,但猫抚摸和身体接触的时间明显较短。狗的凝视与发声有明显的相关性,但其他相关性并不明显。在我们的社区科学环境中,狗的凝视比之前在实验室中测试的狗(Nagasawa 等人,2015 年)要少。归根结底,猫的一般凝视模式与狗相似,但在与主人的大多数互动中却不尽相同。未来的研究应着眼于野猫或野生猫科动物,以揭示猫科动物凝视行为的发展,而更多的社区科学工作则可以确定狗在熟悉和陌生环境中的行为转变。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Gaze in cats (Felis catus) and dogs (Canis lupus familiaris).","authors":"Michael J Bogese, Angie M Johnston, Sarah-Elizabeth Byosiere","doi":"10.1037/com0000359","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000359","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Within human-animal dyadic interactions, dog-human gaze has been identified as the crux of several important visual behaviors, such as looking back, gaze-following, and participation in an oxytocin feedback loop. It has been posited that this gaze behavior may have been motivated and sustained by cooperative relationships between dogs (<i>Canis lupus familiaris</i>) and humans (e.g., hunting, service roles), however, to investigate why gaze evolved, a comparison to a domesticated species that lacks a protracted history of cooperative companionship is needed: the domestic cat (<i>Felis catus</i>). In this study, we compare the gaze duration to owners of cats and dogs in a community science setting. We replicated previous gaze studies with dogs, wolves (Nagasawa et al., 2015), and dingoes (Johnston et al., 2017), requesting owners to sit with their pets for 5 min and interact as they normally would. Cats and dogs gazed at their owners for similar durations, but durations of petting and physical contact were significantly lower with cats. Gaze correlated significantly with vocalizations in dogs; however, no other correlations were significant. Dogs gazed less in our community science setting than dogs tested previously in-lab (Nagasawa et al., 2015). Ultimately, cats resemble dogs in their general gaze patterns, but not in most interactions with their owner. Future research should aim to include feral cats or wild cat species to shed light on gaze behavior development in the genus, while more community science work can identify the behaviors that shift for dogs between familiar and unfamiliar environments. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"68-76"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9910776","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Evaluation of Decision-Making Behavior Under Uncertainty in Capuchin Monkeys (Sapajus apella) and Humans (Homo sapiens) Using a Modified Balloon Analogue Risk Task 使用改良气球模拟风险任务评估僧帽猴(Sapajus apella)和人类(Homo sapiens)在不确定性下的决策行为》补充材料
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-12-07 DOI: 10.1037/com0000368.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Evaluation of Decision-Making Behavior Under Uncertainty in Capuchin Monkeys (Sapajus apella) and Humans (Homo sapiens) Using a Modified Balloon Analogue Risk Task","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/com0000368.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000368.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"32 16","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-12-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138590932","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Do Standard Behavioral Assays Predict Foraging Behavior of Individual Black-Capped Chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) in Response to a Predator Model or Calls? 标准行为分析能否预测个体黑冠山雀(Poecile atricapillus)对捕食者模型或叫声的觅食行为?
4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/com0000364.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Do Standard Behavioral Assays Predict Foraging Behavior of Individual Black-Capped Chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) in Response to a Predator Model or Calls?","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/com0000364.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000364.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"56 5","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135875141","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Comparative Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1