首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Comparative Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Humans' (Homo sapiens), capuchin monkeys' (Sapajus [Cebus] apella), and rhesus macaques' (Macaca mulatta) size judgments shift when stimuli change in frequency. 当刺激频率发生变化时,人类(智人)、卷尾猴(Sapajus[Cebus]apella)和恒河猴(Macaca mulatta)对体型的判断会发生变化。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-23 DOI: 10.1037/com0000365
Sierra M V Simmons, Sarah F Brosnan

When making decisions, humans often strive to uphold objective, absolute standards, such as about what is small versus large, blue versus purple, or unfair versus fair, suggesting that our judgments should not be swayed by extraneous factors such as the sequence or frequency of events to be judged. Yet in previous research, when some items (e.g., threatening faces) became less frequent, humans responded by expanding their concept (of "threatening") to include more ambiguous stimuli. We assessed the origins of this perceptual frequency bias by testing 25 capuchins, seven rhesus monkeys, and 102 humans on a computer task in which they had to classify one circle at a time (pulled from a continuum of 50 circle sizes) as either small or large. Small and large circles initially appeared with equal probability but over time small circles either became less frequent, more frequent, or did not change in frequency. All three species showed changes in judgment, but contrary to predictions, they contracted, rather than expanded, their size judgments of the less frequent category. In other words, when small circles became rare, participants were more likely to judge ambiguous circles sizes as large (and vice versa). This may have been due to the immediate explicit feedback, as has recently been found in humans, and we consider possible mechanisms driving our participants' responses. These results suggest that humans' difficulties in maintaining absolute standards are shared with other animals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在做出决定时,人类通常努力维护客观、绝对的标准,例如关于什么是小与大、蓝色与紫色、不公平与公平,这表明我们的判断不应受到外部因素的影响,例如要判断的事件的顺序或频率。然而,在之前的研究中,当一些项目(例如威胁性面孔)变得不那么频繁时,人类的反应是扩大他们的概念(“威胁”),包括更模糊的刺激。我们通过在一项计算机任务中测试25只卷尾猴、7只恒河猴和102个人来评估这种感知频率偏差的起源,在这项任务中,他们必须一次将一个圆圈(从50个圆圈大小的连续体中提取)分为小圆圈或大圆圈。小圆圈和大圆圈最初出现的概率相等,但随着时间的推移,小圆圈要么变得不那么频繁,要么变得更频繁,要么频率没有变化。这三个物种的判断都发生了变化,但与预测相反,它们缩小而不是扩大了对不太常见类别的大小判断。换句话说,当小圆圈变得罕见时,参与者更有可能将模糊的圆圈大小判断为大(反之亦然)。这可能是由于直接的明确反馈,正如最近在人类身上发现的那样,我们考虑了驱动参与者反应的可能机制。这些结果表明,人类在保持绝对标准方面的困难与其他动物一样。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Humans' (Homo sapiens), capuchin monkeys' (Sapajus [Cebus] apella), and rhesus macaques' (Macaca mulatta) size judgments shift when stimuli change in frequency.","authors":"Sierra M V Simmons, Sarah F Brosnan","doi":"10.1037/com0000365","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000365","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When making decisions, humans often strive to uphold objective, absolute standards, such as about what is small versus large, blue versus purple, or unfair versus fair, suggesting that our judgments should not be swayed by extraneous factors such as the sequence or frequency of events to be judged. Yet in previous research, when some items (e.g., threatening faces) became less frequent, humans responded by expanding their concept (of \"threatening\") to include more ambiguous stimuli. We assessed the origins of this perceptual frequency bias by testing 25 capuchins, seven rhesus monkeys, and 102 humans on a computer task in which they had to classify one circle at a time (pulled from a continuum of 50 circle sizes) as either small or large. Small and large circles initially appeared with equal probability but over time small circles either became less frequent, more frequent, or did not change in frequency. All three species showed changes in judgment, but contrary to predictions, they contracted, rather than expanded, their size judgments of the less frequent category. In other words, when small circles became rare, participants were more likely to judge ambiguous circles sizes as large (and vice versa). This may have been due to the immediate explicit feedback, as has recently been found in humans, and we consider possible mechanisms driving our participants' responses. These results suggest that humans' difficulties in maintaining absolute standards are shared with other animals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"99-107"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49694100","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pitch affects human (Homo sapiens) perception of emotional arousal from diverse animal calls. 音高会影响人类(智人)对不同动物叫声的情感唤起的感知。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-23 DOI: 10.1037/com0000366
Jay W Schwartz, Kayleigh H Pierson, Alexander K Reece

A growing body of research demonstrates that humans can accurately perceive the emotional states of animals solely by listening to their calls, highlighting shared evolutionary ancestry. Yet, the cognitive and perceptual mechanisms underlying heterospecific emotion perception have remained open to investigation. One hypothesis is that humans rely on simple acoustic heuristics to make such judgments, for example, perceiving higher-pitched calls as reflecting heightened emotional arousal (the "pitch rule"). This could lead to accurate judgments of emotion since in most mammals, as in humans, vocal fundamental frequency (the acoustic determinant of the pitch percept) does objectively correlate with emotional arousal. In the present study, we used digital pitch manipulation to create pairs of animal calls that were perceptually identical except for pitch, and we measured human perceptions of the caller's emotional arousal using an online survey. Calls of six phylogenetically diverse species were included as stimuli. Participants attributed slightly but statistically significantly higher arousal to higher-pitched versions of the same calls. Variation in application of the pitch rule across species was not well explained by familiarity, and prior experience with cats did not significantly predict sensitivity to pitch in cat vocalizations. Cross-species variation also did not align with phylogenetic distance from humans, or the hypothetical usefulness of pitch for making accurate judgments. Thus, the pitch rule may be a "mammalomorphic" heuristic leading to accurate emotion judgments in some taxa and call types and erroneous judgments in others, depending in part on phylogenetic distance and the mechanisms of call production. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

越来越多的研究表明,人类仅通过倾听动物的叫声就能准确感知动物的情绪状态,这突出了共同的进化祖先。然而,异性情感感知背后的认知和感知机制仍有待研究。一种假设是,人类依靠简单的声学启发式来做出这样的判断,例如,将音调更高的叫声感知为反映了情绪唤醒的增强(“音调规则”)。这可能会导致对情绪的准确判断,因为在大多数哺乳动物中,就像在人类中一样,发声基频(音高感知的声学决定因素)确实与情绪唤醒客观相关。在本研究中,我们使用数字音高操纵来创建除音高外在感知上完全相同的动物叫声对,并通过在线调查测量了人类对来电者情绪唤醒的感知。六个系统发育不同物种的叫声被包括在内作为刺激。参与者将轻微但统计上显著较高的唤醒归因于相同叫声的高音版本。不同物种之间音高规则应用的变化并不能很好地用熟悉度来解释,之前与猫相处的经验也不能显著预测猫发声对音高的敏感性。跨物种变异也不符合与人类的系统发育距离,也不符合音高对做出准确判断的假设有用性。因此,音高规则可能是一种“长毛象”启发式,导致某些分类群和叫声类型的情绪判断准确,而另一些分类群和声调类型的判断错误,这在一定程度上取决于系统发育距离和叫声产生的机制。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Pitch affects human (Homo sapiens) perception of emotional arousal from diverse animal calls.","authors":"Jay W Schwartz, Kayleigh H Pierson, Alexander K Reece","doi":"10.1037/com0000366","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000366","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A growing body of research demonstrates that humans can accurately perceive the emotional states of animals solely by listening to their calls, highlighting shared evolutionary ancestry. Yet, the cognitive and perceptual mechanisms underlying heterospecific emotion perception have remained open to investigation. One hypothesis is that humans rely on simple acoustic heuristics to make such judgments, for example, perceiving higher-pitched calls as reflecting heightened emotional arousal (the \"pitch rule\"). This could lead to accurate judgments of emotion since in most mammals, as in humans, vocal fundamental frequency (the acoustic determinant of the pitch percept) does objectively correlate with emotional arousal. In the present study, we used digital pitch manipulation to create pairs of animal calls that were perceptually identical except for pitch, and we measured human perceptions of the caller's emotional arousal using an online survey. Calls of six phylogenetically diverse species were included as stimuli. Participants attributed slightly but statistically significantly higher arousal to higher-pitched versions of the same calls. Variation in application of the pitch rule across species was not well explained by familiarity, and prior experience with cats did not significantly predict sensitivity to pitch in cat vocalizations. Cross-species variation also did not align with phylogenetic distance from humans, or the hypothetical usefulness of pitch for making accurate judgments. Thus, the pitch rule may be a \"mammalomorphic\" heuristic leading to accurate emotion judgments in some taxa and call types and erroneous judgments in others, depending in part on phylogenetic distance and the mechanisms of call production. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"80-87"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49694101","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of goal-landmark distance on overshadowing: A replication in humans (Homo sapiens) of Goodyear and Kamil (2004). 目标里程碑距离对遮蔽的影响:Goodyear和Kamil(2004)在人类(智人)中的复制。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-21 DOI: 10.1037/com0000362
Estibaliz Herrera, Joe M Austen, Gonzalo P Urcelay

Goodyear and Kamil (2004) assessed the ability of Clark's nutcrackers to find buried food based on a cross-shaped array of landmarks at different distances from the goal. Their findings suggested that proximal landmarks overshadowed learning about distal landmarks, and this was attenuated when assessing the effect of distal landmarks on learning about proximal landmarks. In this study, we aimed to replicate their findings in human spatial navigation by using a virtual environment. Three groups of participants were trained in an open environment featuring orientation cues, and they had to find a hidden goal with reference to four landmarks that were arranged in the shape of a cross and placed at different distances from the goal. Two of the four landmark distances were common across all three groups to allow a comparison of the extent of overshadowing under comparable conditions. Following training, all participants were tested with each of the four landmarks individually. Consistent with the results in birds, we observed better performance in the groups with more distal landmarks, suggesting that overshadowing was greater in the groups with closer landmarks and thus dependent on the spatial distance between the landmarks and the goal. Landmarks near the goal more effectively overshadowed landmarks far from the goal. A second experiment, in which landmarks and orientation cues were misaligned in order to prevent the use of a straightforward solution to the task, replicated the results. The results are discussed in terms of a modification of Pearce's configural model. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

Goodyear和Kamil(2004)根据距离球门不同距离的十字形地标阵列,评估了克拉克胡桃夹子寻找埋藏食物的能力。他们的研究结果表明,近端标志物盖过了对远端标志物的学习,在评估远端标志物对近端标志性学习的影响时,这一点有所减弱。在这项研究中,我们旨在通过使用虚拟环境在人类空间导航中复制他们的发现。三组参与者在一个以方位线索为特征的开放环境中接受训练,他们必须参照四个排列成十字架形状并与目标相距不同距离的地标找到一个隐藏的目标。四个标志距离中的两个在所有三组中都是常见的,以便比较在可比较条件下的遮蔽程度。培训结束后,所有参与者分别接受了四个里程碑中的每一个的测试。与鸟类的研究结果一致,我们观察到具有更多远端标志的组表现更好,这表明具有较近标志的组阴影更大,因此取决于标志和目标之间的空间距离。球门附近的地标更有效地盖过了远离球门的地标。第二个实验复制了结果,在该实验中,为了防止使用简单的解决方案来完成任务,地标和方向线索没有对齐。对皮尔斯的构形模型进行了修正,对结果进行了讨论。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The effects of goal-landmark distance on overshadowing: A replication in humans (Homo sapiens) of Goodyear and Kamil (2004).","authors":"Estibaliz Herrera, Joe M Austen, Gonzalo P Urcelay","doi":"10.1037/com0000362","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000362","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Goodyear and Kamil (2004) assessed the ability of Clark's nutcrackers to find buried food based on a cross-shaped array of landmarks at different distances from the goal. Their findings suggested that proximal landmarks overshadowed learning about distal landmarks, and this was attenuated when assessing the effect of distal landmarks on learning about proximal landmarks. In this study, we aimed to replicate their findings in human spatial navigation by using a virtual environment. Three groups of participants were trained in an open environment featuring orientation cues, and they had to find a hidden goal with reference to four landmarks that were arranged in the shape of a cross and placed at different distances from the goal. Two of the four landmark distances were common across all three groups to allow a comparison of the extent of overshadowing under comparable conditions. Following training, all participants were tested with each of the four landmarks individually. Consistent with the results in birds, we observed better performance in the groups with more distal landmarks, suggesting that overshadowing was greater in the groups with closer landmarks and thus dependent on the spatial distance between the landmarks and the goal. Landmarks near the goal more effectively overshadowed landmarks far from the goal. A second experiment, in which landmarks and orientation cues were misaligned in order to prevent the use of a straightforward solution to the task, replicated the results. The results are discussed in terms of a modification of Pearce's configural model. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"108-117"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41179420","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do standard behavioral assays predict foraging behavior of individual Black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) in response to a predator model or calls? 标准的行为分析是否可以预测个体黑帽山雀(Poecile atricapillo)对捕食者模型或叫声的觅食行为?
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/com0000364
Jodilyn R Jenkins, Ian G Pope, Madeline A Dykstra, Jennifer J Jenkins, Cheryl R Dykstra, Kelly A Williams

Black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) and other species that feed at bird feeders balance the benefit of easy foraging with the added risk of predation. Individual birds respond differently to risky situations, and these differences have been attributed to the birds' personalities, which researchers commonly assess with an "open-field" behavioral assay. However, these behavioral assays in birds have not been compared to behavior in the wild in the context of foraging in the presence of a predator (i.e., risk-taking behavior). We color-banded chickadees in a wild population and conducted behavioral assays in the field. We later used foraging trials to investigate these color-banded individuals' responses to a predator (Cooper's hawk, Accipiter cooperii) model or a series of Cooper's hawk calls. We found that foraging black-capped chickadees responded more strongly to the presence of a predator model than to predator calls. Individual birds differed in their responses, and the behavioral assays (activity and exploration) predicted individual behavior in the wild during the foraging experiments. Activity and exploration assay scores were only weakly related, suggesting these two assays represent different traits. Both highly active birds and fast explorers exhibited some reluctance to visit the feeder (either reduced number of visits or greater latency to visit) when the predator model was present, a relationship that was somewhat unexpected. Our results suggest that standard behavioral assays predict behavior in the wild, but care should be taken when generalizing among species and studies. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

黑帽山雀(Poecile atricapillo)和其他在鸟类饲养器中觅食的物种平衡了容易觅食的好处和增加的捕食风险。鸟类个体对危险情况的反应不同,这些差异被归因于鸟类的性格,研究人员通常通过“开放领域”行为分析来评估这些性格。然而,鸟类的这些行为测定尚未与捕食者存在下觅食的野外行为(即冒险行为)进行比较。我们对野生种群中的山雀进行了染色,并在野外进行了行为测定。后来,我们使用觅食试验来调查这些色带个体对捕食者(库伯鹰,Accipiter-cooperii)模型或一系列库伯鹰叫声的反应。我们发现,觅食的黑帽山雀对捕食者模型的存在比对捕食者的叫声反应更强烈。鸟类个体的反应各不相同,行为分析(活动和探索)预测了觅食实验期间野外的个体行为。活性测定和探索测定得分仅呈弱相关,这表明这两种测定代表了不同的特征。当捕食者模型存在时,高度活跃的鸟类和快速探索者都表现出不愿意访问喂食器(要么访问次数减少,要么访问延迟更长),这种关系有些出乎意料。我们的研究结果表明,标准的行为测定可以预测野生动物的行为,但在物种和研究之间进行推广时应注意。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Do standard behavioral assays predict foraging behavior of individual Black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) in response to a predator model or calls?","authors":"Jodilyn R Jenkins, Ian G Pope, Madeline A Dykstra, Jennifer J Jenkins, Cheryl R Dykstra, Kelly A Williams","doi":"10.1037/com0000364","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000364","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Black-capped chickadees (<i>Poecile atricapillus</i>) and other species that feed at bird feeders balance the benefit of easy foraging with the added risk of predation. Individual birds respond differently to risky situations, and these differences have been attributed to the birds' personalities, which researchers commonly assess with an \"open-field\" behavioral assay. However, these behavioral assays in birds have not been compared to behavior in the wild in the context of foraging in the presence of a predator (i.e., risk-taking behavior). We color-banded chickadees in a wild population and conducted behavioral assays in the field. We later used foraging trials to investigate these color-banded individuals' responses to a predator (Cooper's hawk, <i>Accipiter cooperii</i>) model or a series of Cooper's hawk calls. We found that foraging black-capped chickadees responded more strongly to the presence of a predator model than to predator calls. Individual birds differed in their responses, and the behavioral assays (activity and exploration) predicted individual behavior in the wild during the foraging experiments. Activity and exploration assay scores were only weakly related, suggesting these two assays represent different traits. Both highly active birds and fast explorers exhibited some reluctance to visit the feeder (either reduced number of visits or greater latency to visit) when the predator model was present, a relationship that was somewhat unexpected. Our results suggest that standard behavioral assays predict behavior in the wild, but care should be taken when generalizing among species and studies. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"130-146"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71429298","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evaluation of decision-making behavior under uncertainty in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella) and humans (Homo sapiens) using a modified Balloon Analogue Risk Task. 使用改良气球模拟风险任务评估卷尾猴(Sapajus apella)和人类(Homo sapiens)在不确定性下的决策行为。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-14 DOI: 10.1037/com0000368
Olivia T Reilly, Sarah F Brosnan

Many animals, including humans, must make decisions when outcomes involve risk and/or ambiguity. To explore the evolutionary roots of decision making when outcomes are unknown, we modified the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) for use with tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus [Cebus] apella), creating the Primate Analogue Risk Task (PART). Using both the BART and the PART, we first compared human performance across the two tasks using analogous parameters. Humans' performance on the two tasks was positively correlated. Next, we tested capuchin monkeys' performance on the PART to assess their decision-making strategies in the context of ambiguity. Secondarily, although it was not the main goal of the study, this allowed us to look at species differences between capuchins' and humans' performance. Finally, we investigated the influence of prior experience on human and capuchin decision-making behavior. Neither capuchins nor humans behaved differently following an unsuccessful trial compared to a successful trial. We found individual differences in capuchin monkeys' choice behavior, though as a whole they demonstrated a pattern of reward maximization over time. Finally, as a species, capuchins had lower PART risk scores than humans. This paradigm presents a useful way to assess behavior in a context with uncertain outcomes using a comparative approach. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

包括人类在内的许多动物都必须在结果具有风险和/或模糊性的情况下做出决策。为了探索未知结果下决策制定的进化根源,我们对气球模拟风险任务(BART)进行了修改,将其用于簇毛卷尾猴(Sapajus [Cebus] apella),创建了灵长类模拟风险任务(PART)。利用 BART 和 PART,我们首先使用类比参数比较了人类在这两项任务中的表现。人类在这两项任务中的表现呈正相关。接下来,我们测试了卷尾猴在 PART 任务中的表现,以评估它们在模棱两可情况下的决策策略。其次,虽然这并不是研究的主要目的,但这让我们得以观察卷尾猴和人类在表现上的物种差异。最后,我们研究了先前经验对人类和卷尾猴决策行为的影响。与成功的试验相比,卷尾猴和人类在试验失败后的行为都没有什么不同。我们发现卷尾猴的选择行为存在个体差异,但整体而言,它们表现出一种随时间推移奖励最大化的模式。最后,作为一个物种,卷尾猴的 PART 风险得分低于人类。这种范例提供了一种有用的方法,可以在结果不确定的情况下使用比较法来评估行为。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Evaluation of decision-making behavior under uncertainty in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella) and humans (Homo sapiens) using a modified Balloon Analogue Risk Task.","authors":"Olivia T Reilly, Sarah F Brosnan","doi":"10.1037/com0000368","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000368","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many animals, including humans, must make decisions when outcomes involve risk and/or ambiguity. To explore the evolutionary roots of decision making when outcomes are unknown, we modified the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) for use with tufted capuchin monkeys (<i>Sapajus [Cebus] apella</i>), creating the Primate Analogue Risk Task (PART). Using both the BART and the PART, we first compared human performance across the two tasks using analogous parameters. Humans' performance on the two tasks was positively correlated. Next, we tested capuchin monkeys' performance on the PART to assess their decision-making strategies in the context of ambiguity. Secondarily, although it was not the main goal of the study, this allowed us to look at species differences between capuchins' and humans' performance. Finally, we investigated the influence of prior experience on human and capuchin decision-making behavior. Neither capuchins nor humans behaved differently following an unsuccessful trial compared to a successful trial. We found individual differences in capuchin monkeys' choice behavior, though as a whole they demonstrated a pattern of reward maximization over time. Finally, as a species, capuchins had lower PART risk scores than humans. This paradigm presents a useful way to assess behavior in a context with uncertain outcomes using a comparative approach. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"88-98"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138813214","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Within the sound of trouble: Do humans use pitch to correctly assess emotional arousal across species? 在麻烦的声音中:人类是否会利用音调来正确评估不同物种的情绪唤醒?
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000389
Alice Auersperg

Comments on an article by Jay W. Schwartz , Kayleigh H. Pierson, and Alexander K. Reece (see record 2024-19488-001). In this issue, Schwartz et al. (2024) tackle the pitch rule in humans by testing to what extent we use pitch alone to judge emotional arousal across closely and distantly related animal species. The findings of Schwartz et al. open a number of intriguing possibilities for future research: Notably important additional steps would include to further investigate the accuracy of the pitch rule across closely and distantly related species. Upon this, in order to study the evolutionary ancestry of the pitch rule, it will be necessary to study its applicability across nonhumans. Particularly interesting would be the inclusion of subject species that have been found to eavesdrop on heterospecific alarm calls. Previous research (see Hoeschele, 2017 for a review) as well as present findings on human ratings of macaque versus cricket calls also suggest that we should additionally focus on sound features that compliment emotional arousal rating beyond pitch such as spectral information. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

对 Jay W. Schwartz、Kayleigh H. Pierson 和 Alexander K. Reece 的文章(见记录 2024-19488-001)的评论。在本期中,Schwartz 等人(2024 年)通过测试我们在多大程度上仅使用音调来判断近亲和远亲动物物种的情绪唤醒,解决了人类的音调规则问题。Schwartz 等人的发现为今后的研究提供了许多引人入胜的可能性:值得注意的是,进一步研究音调规则在近亲和远亲物种中的准确性将是重要的补充步骤。在此基础上,为了研究音高规则的进化祖先,有必要研究它在非人类中的适用性。尤其有趣的是,研究对象物种也会被发现偷听异种警报声。以前的研究(见 Hoeschele, 2017 年综述)以及目前人类对猕猴和蟋蟀叫声的评分结果也表明,我们还应该关注除音调以外的其他声音特征,如频谱信息,以补充情绪唤醒评分。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Within the sound of trouble: Do humans use pitch to correctly assess emotional arousal across species?","authors":"Alice Auersperg","doi":"10.1037/com0000389","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000389","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Comments on an article by Jay W. Schwartz , Kayleigh H. Pierson, and Alexander K. Reece (see record 2024-19488-001). In this issue, Schwartz et al. (2024) tackle the pitch rule in humans by testing to what extent we use pitch alone to judge emotional arousal across closely and distantly related animal species. The findings of Schwartz et al. open a number of intriguing possibilities for future research: Notably important additional steps would include to further investigate the accuracy of the pitch rule across closely and distantly related species. Upon this, in order to study the evolutionary ancestry of the pitch rule, it will be necessary to study its applicability across nonhumans. Particularly interesting would be the inclusion of subject species that have been found to eavesdrop on heterospecific alarm calls. Previous research (see Hoeschele, 2017 for a review) as well as present findings on human ratings of macaque versus cricket calls also suggest that we should additionally focus on sound features that compliment emotional arousal rating beyond pitch such as spectral information. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"138 2","pages":"77-79"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141201519","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Editorial. 社论
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000370
Michael J Beran

The Journal of Comparative Psychology (JCP) is the flagship APA journal dedicated to understanding psychological processes from a comparative perspective. Traditionally, "comparative" has meant comparison across species. However, "comparative" means more than just assessing as many species as possible or relating species to each other. I also think of the importance of a "comparative psychology" perspective in two other ways that should be reflected in the journal's publications. I would like to outline a few important points about how I view the mission of JCP, and how my term as chief editor will address some of the major issues that exist for the journal and for the field of comparative psychology more broadly. Preregistration, replication, and the review process are highlighted. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

比较心理学杂志》(JCP)是美国心理学会的旗舰杂志,致力于从比较的角度理解心理过程。传统上,"比较 "意味着跨物种比较。然而,"比较 "不仅仅意味着尽可能多地评估物种或将物种相互联系起来。我还认为,"比较心理学 "视角的重要性还体现在其他两个方面,这一点应该在期刊的出版物中得到体现。我想就我如何看待 JCP 的使命,以及我在担任主编期间将如何解决该期刊和更广泛的比较心理学领域存在的一些主要问题,概述几个要点。其中特别强调了预注册、复制和审稿流程。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Editorial.","authors":"Michael J Beran","doi":"10.1037/com0000370","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000370","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The <i>Journal of Comparative Psychology</i> (JCP) is the flagship APA journal dedicated to understanding psychological processes from a comparative perspective. Traditionally, \"comparative\" has meant comparison across species. However, \"comparative\" means more than just assessing as many species as possible or relating species to each other. I also think of the importance of a \"comparative psychology\" perspective in two other ways that should be reflected in the journal's publications. I would like to outline a few important points about how I view the mission of JCP, and how my term as chief editor will address some of the major issues that exist for the journal and for the field of comparative psychology more broadly. Preregistration, replication, and the review process are highlighted. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"138 1","pages":"1-4"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140307908","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A study of executive function in grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus): Experience can affect delay of gratification. 灰鹦鹉(Psittacus erithacus)的执行功能研究:经验会影响延迟满足感
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1037/com0000361
Irene M Pepperberg, Leigh Ann Hartsfield

Executive function (EF) involves several abilities often correlated with success in various aspects of human life. Similar skills could also be advantageous to nonhumans, but few studies have effectively examined the extent of their EF abilities. Studies have also examined what experiences might strengthen/weaken human EF; might specific experiences also affect nonhuman EF? One type of EF often tested in both humans and nonhumans involves a delay of gratification-the ability to forgo an immediate reward to gain one either better in quality or quantity. We compared how Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) with somewhat different experimental histories performed when required to wait for more food-a difficult task for avian species. One laboratory-raised parrot, Griffin, had previously succeeded when asked to wait for a better reward and on other tasks purportedly involving some level of EF skills but failed to wait for a larger reward. After succeeding on a task designed to improve impulse control, he consistently waited for a larger reward, more nuts, for up to 15 min-far longer than most avian species tested. Two other parrots, Pepper and Franco, companion animals, had had no experience with delayed gratification tasks, but were as successful as Griffin on other EF-related studies in which they participated. These birds, with different histories than Griffin, also waited for more food for longer periods than most other birds, though not as consistently as Griffin. We suggest that specific types of experiences may strengthen EF in Grey parrots. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

执行功能(EF)涉及多种能力,通常与人类生活各方面的成功息息相关。类似的技能对非人类也可能是有利的,但很少有研究有效地考察了非人类的执行功能。研究还探讨了哪些经历可能会增强/削弱人类的 EF;特定的经历是否也会影响非人类的 EF?在人类和非人类中经常测试的一种 EF 包括延迟满足--放弃即时奖励以获得质量或数量更好的奖励的能力。我们比较了具有不同实验经历的灰鹦鹉(Psittacus erithacus)在被要求等待更多食物时的表现--这对鸟类来说是一项艰巨的任务。其中一只在实验室饲养的鹦鹉格里芬曾在被要求等待更好的奖励和其他据称涉及某种程度的 EF 技能的任务中取得成功,但在等待更大的奖励时却失败了。在成功完成一项旨在提高冲动控制能力的任务后,它一直在等待更大的奖励、更多的坚果,等待时间长达15分钟,远远超过了大多数被测试的鸟类。另外两只鹦鹉佩珀(Pepper)和弗朗科(Franco)是伴侣动物,它们都没有完成延迟满足任务的经验,但在它们参与的其他与EF相关的研究中,它们和格里芬一样成功。这两只鸟的经历与格里芬不同,它们等待食物的时间也比其他大多数鸟类更长,但不如格里芬那么持久。我们认为,特定类型的经历可能会增强灰鹦鹉的 EF。 (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"A study of executive function in grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus): Experience can affect delay of gratification.","authors":"Irene M Pepperberg, Leigh Ann Hartsfield","doi":"10.1037/com0000361","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000361","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Executive function (EF) involves several abilities often correlated with success in various aspects of human life. Similar skills could also be advantageous to nonhumans, but few studies have effectively examined the extent of their EF abilities. Studies have also examined what experiences might strengthen/weaken human EF; might specific experiences also affect nonhuman EF? One type of EF often tested in both humans and nonhumans involves a delay of gratification-the ability to forgo an immediate reward to gain one either better in quality or quantity. We compared how Grey parrots <i>(Psittacus erithacus)</i> with somewhat different experimental histories performed when required to wait for more food-a difficult task for avian species. One laboratory-raised parrot, Griffin, had previously succeeded when asked to wait for a better reward and on other tasks purportedly involving some level of EF skills but failed to wait for a larger reward. After succeeding on a task designed to improve impulse control, he consistently waited for a larger reward, more nuts, for up to 15 min-far longer than most avian species tested. Two other parrots, Pepper and Franco, companion animals, had had no experience with delayed gratification tasks, but were as successful as Griffin on other EF-related studies in which they participated. These birds, with different histories than Griffin, also waited for more food for longer periods than most other birds, though not as consistently as Griffin. We suggest that specific types of experiences may strengthen EF in Grey parrots. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"8-19"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10321004","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On parrots, delay of gratification, executive function, and how sometimes we do the best we can. 关于鹦鹉、延迟满足、执行功能,以及有时我们如何尽力而为。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000378
Michael J Beran

Engaging executive functions provides an individual with the means to engage in cognitive control by adjusting to the environment and processing information in a way that leads to optimal outcomes. There are some claims that explicit training on certain executive functioning abilities provides benefits beyond the training tasks, but other studies indicate that this may not be true or may be limited based on age and other factors. This same mixed pattern has been reported with nonhuman species, where training or even experience in one specific area, like inhibition, sometimes leads to positive transfer in new but similar tasks that presumably also require executive functions. Pepperberg and Hartsfield (2024) sought to determine whether experience in previous tasks that required different executive functions impacted how well three African grey parrots: Griffin, Pepper, and Franco could perform in a new assessment of delayed gratification. Griffin showed a clear and consistent capacity to wait through a delay for a quantitatively better reward. This suggested that the previous experience with the tokens aided improvement in the quantitative delay of gratification task with food items as the options to choose between. The other two parrots, Pepper and Franco, never completed the intended sequence of phases in their study. Unfortunately, the testing conditions dictated by COVID restrictions were such that these two subjects appeared to exhibit stress in doing the task, and so no further testing was conducted with them. This article is an example of what can happen when two intelligent species (people and parrots) are put in difficult circumstances (a global pandemic unlike anything any of us has ever been through), and yet both species attempted to continue to engage in science. The effects of COVID-19 will remain an integral factor in comparative psychology for some time to come, and I suspect there are many other half-completed experiments that suffered because of the pandemic. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

执行功能为个人提供了一种手段,通过适应环境和处理信息来进行认知控制,从而获得最佳结果。有人声称,对某些执行功能能力的明确训练会带来训练任务之外的益处,但其他研究表明,这种说法可能并不正确,或可能因年龄和其他因素而受到限制。在非人类物种中也有这种混合模式的报道,在某一特定领域(如抑制)的训练甚至经验有时会导致在新的类似任务中的积极迁移,而这些任务可能也需要执行功能。佩珀伯格和哈茨菲尔德(2024 年)试图确定,以往需要不同执行功能的任务经验是否会影响三只非洲灰鹦鹉的表现:格里芬、佩珀和弗兰科在一项新的延迟满足评估中的表现如何。格里芬表现出明显而持续的能力,能够通过延迟等待获得数量上更好的奖励。这表明,以前使用代币的经验有助于提高在以食物为选项的定量延迟满足任务中的表现。另外两只鹦鹉佩珀和弗兰科在研究中从未完成预定的阶段顺序。不幸的是,受 COVID 限制的测试条件使这两只受试者在完成任务时似乎表现出了压力,因此没有对它们进行进一步的测试。这篇文章举例说明了当两种聪明的物种(人和鹦鹉)被置于困难的环境中(我们从未经历过的全球大流行),但这两种物种都试图继续从事科学研究时会发生什么。在未来的一段时间里,COVID-19 的影响仍将是比较心理学中不可或缺的因素,我猜想还有许多其他半途而废的实验也会因为这场大流行病而受到影响。(PsycInfo 数据库记录 (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"On parrots, delay of gratification, executive function, and how sometimes we do the best we can.","authors":"Michael J Beran","doi":"10.1037/com0000378","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000378","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Engaging executive functions provides an individual with the means to engage in cognitive control by adjusting to the environment and processing information in a way that leads to optimal outcomes. There are some claims that explicit training on certain executive functioning abilities provides benefits beyond the training tasks, but other studies indicate that this may not be true or may be limited based on age and other factors. This same mixed pattern has been reported with nonhuman species, where training or even experience in one specific area, like inhibition, sometimes leads to positive transfer in new but similar tasks that presumably also require executive functions. Pepperberg and Hartsfield (2024) sought to determine whether experience in previous tasks that required different executive functions impacted how well three African grey parrots: Griffin, Pepper, and Franco could perform in a new assessment of delayed gratification. Griffin showed a clear and consistent capacity to wait through a delay for a quantitatively better reward. This suggested that the previous experience with the tokens aided improvement in the quantitative delay of gratification task with food items as the options to choose between. The other two parrots, Pepper and Franco, never completed the intended sequence of phases in their study. Unfortunately, the testing conditions dictated by COVID restrictions were such that these two subjects appeared to exhibit stress in doing the task, and so no further testing was conducted with them. This article is an example of what can happen when two intelligent species (people and parrots) are put in difficult circumstances (a global pandemic unlike anything any of us has ever been through), and yet both species attempted to continue to engage in science. The effects of COVID-19 will remain an integral factor in comparative psychology for some time to come, and I suspect there are many other half-completed experiments that suffered because of the pandemic. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"138 1","pages":"5-7"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140307909","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Primate socio-ecology shapes the evolution of distinctive facial repertoires. 灵长类动物的社会生态塑造了独特面部动作的进化。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-11 DOI: 10.1037/com0000350
Brittany N Florkiewicz, Linda S Oña, Leonardo Oña, Matthew W Campbell

Primate facial musculature enables a wide variety of movements during bouts of communication, but how these movements contribute to signal construction and repertoire size is unclear. The facial mobility hypothesis suggests that morphological constraints shape the evolution of facial repertoires: species with higher facial mobility will produce larger and more complex repertoires. In contrast, the socio-ecological complexity hypothesis suggests that social needs shape the evolution of facial repertoires: as social complexity increases, so does communicative repertoire size. We tested these two hypotheses by comparing chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and gibbons (family Hylobatidae), two distantly related apes who vary in their facial mobility and social organization. While gibbons have higher facial mobility than chimpanzees, chimpanzees live in more complex social groups than gibbons. We compared the morphology and complexity of facial repertoires for both apes using Facial Action Coding Systems designed for chimpanzees and gibbons. Our comparisons were made at the level of individual muscle movements (action units [AUs]) and the level of muscle movement combinations (AU combinations). Our results show that the chimpanzee facial signaling repertoire was larger and more complex than gibbons, consistent with the socio-ecological complexity hypothesis. On average, chimpanzees produced AU combinations consisting of more morphologically distinct AUs than gibbons. Moreover, chimpanzees also produced more morphologically distinct AU combinations than gibbons, even when focusing exclusively on AUs present in both apes. Therefore, our results suggest that socio-ecological factors were more important than anatomical ones to the evolution of facial signaling repertoires in chimpanzees and gibbons. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

灵长类动物的面部肌肉在交流过程中可以做出多种多样的动作,但这些动作如何有助于信号的构建和语汇的大小尚不清楚。面部活动性假说认为,形态上的限制决定了面部动作集的进化:面部活动性较高的物种会产生更大、更复杂的动作集。与此相反,社会生态复杂性假说认为,社会需求决定了面部信号的进化:随着社会复杂性的增加,交流信号的规模也会增加。我们通过比较黑猩猩(Pan troglodytes)和长臂猿(Hylobatidae科)来验证这两种假说。长臂猿的面部活动度比黑猩猩高,但黑猩猩生活在比长臂猿更复杂的社会群体中。我们使用专为黑猩猩和长臂猿设计的面部动作编码系统,比较了这两种猿类面部动作的形态和复杂程度。我们从单个肌肉动作(动作单元 [AU])和肌肉动作组合(AU 组合)两个层面进行了比较。我们的结果表明,黑猩猩的面部信号库比长臂猿更大、更复杂,这与社会生态复杂性假说一致。平均而言,黑猩猩比长臂猿能做出更多形态各异的 AU 组合。此外,黑猩猩也比长臂猿产生了更多形态上不同的AU组合,即使只关注两种猿类都存在的AU也是如此。因此,我们的研究结果表明,在黑猩猩和长臂猿面部信号系统的进化过程中,社会生态因素比解剖学因素更为重要。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Primate socio-ecology shapes the evolution of distinctive facial repertoires.","authors":"Brittany N Florkiewicz, Linda S Oña, Leonardo Oña, Matthew W Campbell","doi":"10.1037/com0000350","DOIUrl":"10.1037/com0000350","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Primate facial musculature enables a wide variety of movements during bouts of communication, but how these movements contribute to signal construction and repertoire size is unclear. The <i>facial mobility hypothesis</i> suggests that morphological constraints shape the evolution of facial repertoires: species with higher facial mobility will produce larger and more complex repertoires. In contrast, the <i>socio-ecological complexity hypothesis</i> suggests that social needs shape the evolution of facial repertoires: as social complexity increases, so does communicative repertoire size. We tested these two hypotheses by comparing chimpanzees (<i>Pan troglodytes</i>) and gibbons (family <i>Hylobatidae</i>), two distantly related apes who vary in their facial mobility and social organization. While gibbons have higher facial mobility than chimpanzees, chimpanzees live in more complex social groups than gibbons. We compared the morphology and complexity of facial repertoires for both apes using Facial Action Coding Systems designed for chimpanzees and gibbons. Our comparisons were made at the level of individual muscle movements (action units [AUs]) and the level of muscle movement combinations (AU combinations). Our results show that the chimpanzee facial signaling repertoire was larger and more complex than gibbons, consistent with the <i>socio-ecological complexity hypothesis.</i> On average, chimpanzees produced AU combinations consisting of more morphologically distinct AUs than gibbons. Moreover, chimpanzees also produced more morphologically distinct AU combinations than gibbons, even when focusing exclusively on AUs present in both apes. Therefore, our results suggest that socio-ecological factors were more important than anatomical ones to the evolution of facial signaling repertoires in chimpanzees and gibbons. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"32-44"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9449335","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Comparative Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1