首页 > 最新文献

Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte最新文献

英文 中文
Did Werner Heisenberg Understand How Atomic Bombs Worked? 维尔纳·海森堡知道原子弹是如何工作的吗?
IF 0.6 2区 哲学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-06-02 DOI: 10.1002/bewi.202100032
Mark Walker

Drawing upon primary sources and using a comparison with the American Manhattan Project for context, this article examines the question whether Werner Heisenberg understood how atomic bombs work.

根据第一手资料,并与美国曼哈顿计划进行比较,本文探讨了维尔纳·海森堡是否了解原子弹的工作原理。
{"title":"Did Werner Heisenberg Understand How Atomic Bombs Worked?","authors":"Mark Walker","doi":"10.1002/bewi.202100032","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bewi.202100032","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Drawing upon primary sources and using a comparison with the American Manhattan Project for context, this article examines the question whether Werner Heisenberg understood how atomic bombs work.</p>","PeriodicalId":55388,"journal":{"name":"Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte","volume":"45 1-2","pages":"219-244"},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2022-06-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48199004","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"哲学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Histories of Ethology: Methods, Sites, and Dynamics of an Unbound Discipline 动物行为学的历史:一门不受约束的学科的方法、地点和动态
IF 0.6 2区 哲学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-06-01 DOI: 10.1002/bewi.202200026
Sophia Gräfe, Cora Stuhrmann

Ethology is considered the leading biological discipline within behavioral research in the 20th century. Its history is told as a seemingly straightforward narrative: Ethology has its roots in the 1930s in German-speaking countries, a disciplinary heyday in the 1950s and 1960s, after which it slowly lost relevance. It employs a distinct approach to the comparative study of animal behavior, which is characterized by a physiological method of non-invasive, often observational studies of natural behavioral patterns, which were conceived of as shaped by evolution. Ethology contains stories of charismatic research animals such as the jackdaw Tschock or the goose Martina,1 draws on academic disciplines such as ornithology, ichthyology, and entomology,2 and also incorporates contexts and practices of animal lovers, bird watchers, and hunters, as well as those involved in animal husbandry, wildlife preservation, and livestock farming, or who work in nature reserves or zoological gardens.3 Ethology is further connected to the development of certain visual media, such as chronophotography and the film loop,4 and corresponding forms of perception, such as pattern recognition5 or comparative visual analysis.6 Other methodical highlights include the ethogram,7 dummies,8 and the Kaspar Hauser experiment.9 The history of ethology conventionally focuses on several elements: an illustrious circle of founding figures, indeed founding “fathers,” such as Konrad Lorenz (1903–1989), Nikolaas Tinbergen (1907–1988), Karl von Frisch (1886–1982), Jakob von Uexküll (1864–1944),10 Oskar Heinroth (1871–1945),11 Erwin Stresemann (1889–1972),12 and Otto Koehler (1889–1974),13 the importance of the Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie (later renamed Ethology),14 legendary encounters of individual scholars with their research subjects15 and colleagues during conferences16, and towering intellectual achievements such as famous talks17 or foundational monographs such as Tinbergen's The Study of Instinct, published in 195118. Further markers of ethology's disciplinary history are the recognition of its achievement and disciplinary status with the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine awarded to Lorenz, Tinbergen and von Frisch in 1973 and the ensuing controversy about Lorenz's political background.19

All scientific disciplines, of course, are outlined by a set of protagonists, places, publications, and practices.20 However, ethology is characterized by an unusual preoccupation with its own disciplinary status, demarcating its core and periphery, its borders and boundaries as well as delineating its historical lineage and possible trajectories—all adding to the sense that ethologists’ historical accounts are full of strategic se

行为学被认为是20世纪行为研究中领先的生物学学科。动物行为学的历史以一种看似直截了当地的叙述方式讲述:动物行为学起源于20世纪30年代的德语国家,在20世纪50年代和60年代是学科的鼎盛时期,之后它慢慢失去了相关性。它采用一种独特的方法对动物行为进行比较研究,其特点是一种非侵入性的生理方法,通常是对自然行为模式的观察性研究,这种研究被认为是由进化形成的。动物行为学包含有魅力的研究动物的故事,如寒鸦Tschock或鹅Martina,1借鉴了鸟类学、鱼类学和昆虫学等学科,2还结合了动物爱好者、鸟类观察者和猎人的背景和实践,以及那些参与畜牧业、野生动物保护和畜牧业的人,或在自然保护区或动物园工作的人动物行为学还与某些视觉媒介的发展有进一步的联系,如时间摄影和电影循环,以及相应的感知形式,如模式识别或比较视觉分析其他有系统的亮点包括直方图,假人,和卡斯帕·豪瑟实验动物行为学的历史通常集中在以下几个方面:一群杰出的奠基人,实际上是奠基人,如康拉德·洛伦兹(1903-1989)、尼古拉斯·廷伯根(1907-1988)、卡尔·冯·弗里施(1886-1982)、雅各布·冯·尤克斯k<e:2>(1864-1944)、奥斯卡·海因罗斯(1871-1945)、欧文·施特雷泽曼(1889-1972)、奥托·克勒(1889-1974)、时代心理学(后更名为动物行为学)的重要性、学者个人在会议期间与其研究对象和同事的传奇遭遇。以及杰出的智力成就,如著名的演讲,或奠基性的专著,如丁伯根于1951年出版的《本能的研究》。行为学学科历史的进一步标志是1973年诺贝尔生理学或医学奖授予洛伦兹、丁伯根和冯·弗里施,以及随后关于洛伦兹政治背景的争议,从而承认了它的成就和学科地位。当然,所有的科学学科都是由一系列的主角、地点、出版物和实践来概括的然而,动物行为学的特点是不寻常地关注自己的学科地位,划定其核心和外围,边界和边界,以及描绘其历史谱系和可能的轨迹——所有这些都增加了动物行为学家的历史叙述充满战略自我历史化和有目的的叙述的感觉。因此,科学史家一直在研究学科认同这一核心问题,揭示了参与者所采用的战略叙事的层次,并增加了他们对学科成功故事的描述的复杂性。从科学史家、动物行为学家转变为历史学家,动物行为学家自己也提供了动物行为学的叙述,有些是出于学术好奇心,有些是出于纪念、历史合法性或战略考虑的愿望。这期特刊对超越古典阶段的动物行为学史学做出了贡献,反映了动物行为学作为一门不受约束的学科的本质,它不像洛伦兹和其他早期动物行为学家所做的历史努力那样统一和有凝聚力它邀请跨学科的观众从认识论、动物研究、媒体研究以及出版和数据历史的角度重新发现动物行为学的历史。本期的贡献集中在新的方法、隐藏的地点和被忽视的动态,并举例说明了广泛的方法,包括档案工作、多媒体分析、口述历史访谈和多焦点动物行为学历史的出版追踪。我们从事与中央国家在历史和行为学的史学:德语国家,英国和美国。虽然我们承认这种选择的局限性,但动物行为学的史学是由德国和英国的洛伦兹-丁伯根轴心塑造的,其中一些最重要的动物行为学和历史研究与他们在每个国家的科学影响有关或接续进行因此,写动物行为学的历史包括处理这些局部学科历史的历史背景和社会动机。我们将叙述动物行为学史学的中心作者、阶段和流派,并以对当前主题的反思结束。 历史学家和动物行为学家在过去几十年的工作表明,动物行为学从来不是一个单一的、有凝聚力的框架,而是社会动态、战略议程和偶然发展的结果,包含了不同的方法和不同的理论前提。它的学科界限是多孔的和可渗透的。因此,我们建议从一开始就把动物行为学理解为一门不受约束的学科。这使我们能够为其进一步的突破增加新的视角,特别是通过观察那些不被认为是动物行为学主流的研究人员,以及在被认为是其经典阶段之后的研究人员。我们的贡献探讨了动物行为学发展的不同动态。新的方法允许从时间和寿命的限制中解脱出来:摄影,卡片索引和长期数据收集将研究对象从个体动物行为转移到合成行为库,时间跨度不断增加。同样,对研究对象和问题的限制也出现了突破,例如将人类行为或动物认知纳入动物行为学研究的范围。这些变化也意味着从行为学知识生产的经典地点脱离出来,我们的贡献探索了新的地点,如肯尼亚的长期野外站,柏林东德部分的研究综合体,美国的优生思想狗研究站以及慕尼黑幼儿园。这些动态增加了动物行为学作为一个学科整体的复杂性,相反,提出了动物行为学如何作为一门学科保持在一起的问题。我们的贡献表明,通过共享的机构和出版机构、学术谱系和血统、公共研究站和充满活力的大学院系,动物行为学作为一门学科的本质是社会性质。凯勒·戴恩阐明了卡尔·冯·弗里施迄今未澄清的关于实验方法的动物行为学的关系。戴恩文章的重点是冯·弗里施在20世纪10年代发展起来的灰卡实验方法。冯·弗里施将他的研究对象的行为的整体观点与受控的、有时是简化的实验设计相结合,成功地产生了可量化的研究结果,这些结果提供了有关实验中蜜蜂感官知觉的信息。在其他方面,蜜蜂的条件反射,这是他当时的实验所必需的,代表了对经典动物行为学方法的明显背离,甚至导致冯·弗里施被排除在动物行为学圈子之外。这是行为学历史上的一个悖论,正是这个实验设计将成为该学科历史后期阶段的关键组成部分,并从20世纪70年代开始,在与神经行为学领域相关的研究项目的成功中发挥了重要作用。在考察一个实验如何“有自己的生命”时,从而穿越了不同的前提、问题和研究方向,戴恩成功地在动物行为学的历史上开辟了一条新的路线。Sophia Gräfe的贡献也采用了行动学的方法。她在古典动物行为学中对视觉媒体的研究为该学科历史上现有的重要地点目录增添了档案。她以柏林Forschungsstätte <s:2> Tierpsychologie的g<s:1> nter Tembrock建立的动物行为学图像集为研究对象,展示了这些图像以及其他视觉媒体(如绘画和电影记录)在多大程度上显著推进了对先天行为形式的动物行为学研究。图像被用来辅助格式塔感知和模式识别。它们还可以作为记忆辅助工具,以及向同事展示科学家详细的知识和研究经验。此外,图像集合为发展最清晰的行为命名提供了基础,行为作为一种短暂的现象,需要新的表现形式。由于动物行为学收藏中的图像通常来自不同的来源,并且它们的主题跨越了很长一段时间,Gräfe的分析强调了比较行为研究的主要集体特征。最后,从视觉媒介的角度考察动物行为学的历史,探讨生物行为知识的综合特征。Tembrock对红狐行为的开创性研究是他在行为实验室对动物进行实验和对档案图像进行专家分析的结果。布拉德·博尔曼通过使用模式生物研究的方法,在美国实现了犬类行为研究的多层肖像。到目前为止,以前的史学编纂将这些研究排除在动物行为学的历史之外,因此忽视了这一行为研究领域的犬类历史。
{"title":"Histories of Ethology: Methods, Sites, and Dynamics of an Unbound Discipline","authors":"Sophia Gräfe,&nbsp;Cora Stuhrmann","doi":"10.1002/bewi.202200026","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bewi.202200026","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Ethology is considered the leading biological discipline within behavioral research in the 20th century. Its history is told as a seemingly straightforward narrative: Ethology has its roots in the 1930s in German-speaking countries, a disciplinary heyday in the 1950s and 1960s, after which it slowly lost relevance. It employs a distinct approach to the comparative study of animal behavior, which is characterized by a physiological method of non-invasive, often observational studies of natural behavioral patterns, which were conceived of as shaped by evolution. Ethology contains stories of charismatic research animals such as the jackdaw <i>Tschock</i> or the goose <i>Martina</i>,<sup>1</sup> draws on academic disciplines such as ornithology, ichthyology, and entomology,<sup>2</sup> and also incorporates contexts and practices of animal lovers, bird watchers, and hunters, as well as those involved in animal husbandry, wildlife preservation, and livestock farming, or who work in nature reserves or zoological gardens.<sup>3</sup> Ethology is further connected to the development of certain visual media, such as chronophotography and the film loop,<sup>4</sup> and corresponding forms of perception, such as pattern recognition<sup>5</sup> or comparative visual analysis.<sup>6</sup> Other methodical highlights include the ethogram,<sup>7</sup> dummies,<sup>8</sup> and the Kaspar Hauser experiment.<sup>9</sup> The history of ethology conventionally focuses on several elements: an illustrious circle of founding figures, indeed founding “fathers,” such as Konrad Lorenz (1903–1989), Nikolaas Tinbergen (1907–1988), Karl von Frisch (1886–1982), Jakob von Uexküll (1864–1944),<sup>10</sup> Oskar Heinroth (1871–1945),<sup>11</sup> Erwin Stresemann (1889–1972),<sup>12</sup> and Otto Koehler (1889–1974),<sup>13</sup> the importance of the <i>Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie</i> (later renamed <i>Ethology</i>),<sup>14</sup> legendary encounters of individual scholars with their research subjects<sup>15</sup> and colleagues during conferences<sup>16</sup>, and towering intellectual achievements such as famous talks<sup>17</sup> or foundational monographs such as Tinbergen's <i>The Study of Instinct, published in 1951</i><sup>18</sup>. Further markers of ethology's disciplinary history are the recognition of its achievement and disciplinary status with the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine awarded to Lorenz, Tinbergen and von Frisch in 1973 and the ensuing controversy about Lorenz's political background.<sup>19</sup></p><p>All scientific disciplines, of course, are outlined by a set of protagonists, places, publications, and practices.<sup>20</sup> However, ethology is characterized by an unusual preoccupation with its own disciplinary status, demarcating its core and periphery, its borders and boundaries as well as delineating its historical lineage and possible trajectories—all adding to the sense that ethologists’ historical accounts are full of strategic se","PeriodicalId":55388,"journal":{"name":"Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte","volume":"45 1-2","pages":"10-29"},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2022-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bewi.202200026","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43335523","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"哲学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Bausteine zu einer Oral History der Wissenschaftsgeschichte Wissenschaft als Arbeitsprozess. Interview mit Wolfgang Lefèvre 它包含了人们口述科学历史的"支架"采访Wolfgang Lefèvre
IF 0.6 2区 哲学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-05-26 DOI: 10.1002/bewi.202200013
Mathias Grote, Anke te Heesen, Wolfgang Lefèvre

Wie kann man einen historischen Blick auf das eigene Fach werfen? Diese Frage ist nicht einfach zu beantworten – will man einerseits nicht in einer Nabelschau und Hagiographie enden, andererseits aber auch keinen umfassenden Entwurf einer zukünftigen Historiographie vorlegen. Die hier als Bausteine zu einer Oral History der Wissenschaftsgeschichte in loser Folge publizierten Interviews mit bekannten Protagonisten der Berliner Wissenschaftsgeschichte von ca. 1970–1990 in West und Ost rücken die Geschichte des Faches deshalb in einem bestimmten Milieu in den Fokus und versuchen, die Historiographie jenseits einer Institutionen- oder Theoriegeschichte voranzutreiben. Welche Motivationen oder Probleme bewegten einzelne Wissenschaftler:innen, sich der Geschichte ihres Faches zu widmen oder sich etwa aus der Soziologie oder Philosophie in die Wissenschaftsgeschichte zu bewegen? Welche Ausbildungspraxen existierten in diesem heterogenen, zwischen den Disziplinen angesiedelten Feld, welche Anregungen bezog man aus welchen Kontexten? Wie war Lehre strukturiert und welche Netzwerke bildeten sich mit der Zeit? Kurz: Mit welchem Interesse kam man zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte und was wurde daraus? Die Auswahl der Interviewees erfolgt ohne Anspruch auf Vollständigkeit oder Proporz; der Fragenkatalog der Interviews richtet sich individuell nach den Biographien und dem Werk und entfaltet sich oft spontan im Gespräch. Die Interviews wurden digital aufgezeichnet, transkribiert, der Schriftsprache angepasst, gegebenenfalls gekürzt, annotiert und von den Interviewees authentifiziert. Wir beabsichtigen mit dieser Serie von Interviews zunächst die Dokumentation rezenter Geschichte durch eine Oral History, die subjektive Wahrnehmungen und persönliche Erlebnisse einschließt. Auf diese Weise werden Segmente einer größtenteils ungeschriebenen Geschichte anhand von Biographien erfahrbar und damit auch einer weiteren kritischen Bearbeitung und Integration in ein Gesamtbild zugänglich. Da uns im Zuge der jeweiligen Vorbereitung und Durchführung, Transkription und Abstimmung der Interviews daran gelegen war, aus Sicht der Akteure wichtige Sammelbände und Aufsätze, Graue Literatur oder Monographien zu erfassen, wird nebenbei eine kommentierte Bibliographie zur Geschichte der Wissenschaftsgeschichte entstehen. Unsere Hoffnung besteht darin, mittels dieser Sammlung mit Berlin einen fruchtbaren Raum und mit den 1970er und 1980er Jahren eine produktive Zeit des Faches jenseits von Reminiszenz oder Nostalgie zu erkunden nicht zuletzt auch, um den Blick für gegenwärtige Herausforderungen des Faches zu schärfen.

Mathias Grote, Anke te Heesen

你可以从历史中学到什么?这个问题不好回答,一方面人们可能不想摆出哀伤的神秘感,另一方面又不想在未来的史书中提供详尽的蓝图。这里作为基石的口腔历史却谢谢我在公布访问柏林的谢谢我已知的主角们的ca . 1970-1990西和东背后的故事在某一帮人做所学专业所以关注和试图Historiographie之外另一个机构推进Theoriegeschichte .个别科学家们往往会产生什么样的动机或者难题:或者在社会学或者哲学中进入科学史。这堆散乱的学科之间建立了什么训练课程,根据什么背景提供了什么建议?教义如何构造,哪些网络随时间推移而建立?基本上,人们对科学的历史有什么兴趣?后来怎样?访谈系统由哪位?王牌主播,无法全部掌握;面试目录总是针对个人的传记和作品,常常自发的进行着面试。访谈经数码记录,对调,训练训练,精进训练,训练,并由访谈训练训练。接下来,我们希望能以包括主观体验和个人经历的口述历史作为背景,完成这一系列采访。这样,通过传记,大部分未写成历史的片段就能记录出整个片段,这使片段能够同时被广泛的片段记录和整合。由于在相关文献的准备和执行、手稿和编辑采访中,以及在作案者看来,我们希望涵盖重要的文献和论文、灰色文献和文献,并附带参考了关于科学历史的参考文献。我们希望利用发现者小组与柏林之间的合作,找到一个肥沃的地区,并利用70年代和80年代在没有回顾或怀旧的情况下使用现代技术的现代空间,包括着眼于今天面对现代意义上的挑战。马蒂亚斯·格罗夫斯
{"title":"Bausteine zu einer Oral History der Wissenschaftsgeschichte Wissenschaft als Arbeitsprozess. Interview mit Wolfgang Lefèvre","authors":"Mathias Grote,&nbsp;Anke te Heesen,&nbsp;Wolfgang Lefèvre","doi":"10.1002/bewi.202200013","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bewi.202200013","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Wie kann man einen historischen Blick auf das eigene Fach werfen? Diese Frage ist nicht einfach zu beantworten – will man einerseits nicht in einer Nabelschau und Hagiographie enden, andererseits aber auch keinen umfassenden Entwurf einer zukünftigen Historiographie vorlegen. Die hier als Bausteine zu einer Oral History der Wissenschaftsgeschichte in loser Folge publizierten Interviews mit bekannten Protagonisten der Berliner Wissenschaftsgeschichte von ca. 1970–1990 in West und Ost rücken die Geschichte des Faches deshalb in einem bestimmten Milieu in den Fokus und versuchen, die Historiographie jenseits einer Institutionen- oder Theoriegeschichte voranzutreiben. Welche Motivationen oder Probleme bewegten einzelne Wissenschaftler:innen, sich der Geschichte ihres Faches zu widmen oder sich etwa aus der Soziologie oder Philosophie in die Wissenschaftsgeschichte zu bewegen? Welche Ausbildungspraxen existierten in diesem heterogenen, zwischen den Disziplinen angesiedelten Feld, welche Anregungen bezog man aus welchen Kontexten? Wie war Lehre strukturiert und welche Netzwerke bildeten sich mit der Zeit? Kurz: Mit welchem Interesse kam man zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte und was wurde daraus? Die Auswahl der Interviewees erfolgt ohne Anspruch auf Vollständigkeit oder Proporz; der Fragenkatalog der Interviews richtet sich individuell nach den Biographien und dem Werk und entfaltet sich oft spontan im Gespräch. Die Interviews wurden digital aufgezeichnet, transkribiert, der Schriftsprache angepasst, gegebenenfalls gekürzt, annotiert und von den Interviewees authentifiziert. Wir beabsichtigen mit dieser Serie von Interviews zunächst die Dokumentation rezenter Geschichte durch eine Oral History, die subjektive Wahrnehmungen und persönliche Erlebnisse einschließt. Auf diese Weise werden Segmente einer größtenteils ungeschriebenen Geschichte anhand von Biographien erfahrbar und damit auch einer weiteren kritischen Bearbeitung und Integration in ein Gesamtbild zugänglich. Da uns im Zuge der jeweiligen Vorbereitung und Durchführung, Transkription und Abstimmung der Interviews daran gelegen war, aus Sicht der Akteure wichtige Sammelbände und Aufsätze, Graue Literatur oder Monographien zu erfassen, wird nebenbei eine kommentierte Bibliographie zur Geschichte der Wissenschaftsgeschichte entstehen. Unsere Hoffnung besteht darin, mittels dieser Sammlung mit Berlin einen fruchtbaren Raum und mit den 1970er und 1980er Jahren eine produktive Zeit des Faches jenseits von Reminiszenz oder Nostalgie zu erkunden nicht zuletzt auch, um den Blick für gegenwärtige Herausforderungen des Faches zu schärfen.</p><p>Mathias Grote, Anke te Heesen</p>","PeriodicalId":55388,"journal":{"name":"Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte","volume":"45 1-2","pages":"265-280"},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2022-05-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43878374","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"哲学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Red Foxes in the Filing Cabinet: Günter Tembrock's Image Collection and Media Use in Mid-Century Ethology** 档案柜里的红狐:<s:1>内特·坦布罗克在中世纪动物行为学中的图像收集和媒介使用**
IF 0.6 2区 哲学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-05-18 DOI: 10.1002/bewi.202200004
Sophia Gräfe

This paper considers the epistemic career of visual media in ethology in the mid-20th century. Above all, ethologists claimed close contact with research animals and drew scientific evidence from these human-animal communities, particularly in public relations. However, if we look into the toolboxes of comparative behavioral biologists, it becomes evident that scientifically valid research results were primarily obtained by experimenting with model images. These visual specimens tell a technical story of the methodological requirements in behavioral science necessary to bridge everyday observations between the laboratory and the field. By neutralizing individual traces of animal bodies as well as their observers, they prompted the abstraction of ethological hypotheses. The case study of East-German biologist Günter Tembrock (1918–2011), who maintained his own collection of newspaper clippings, drawings, photographs, and films, offers a new perspective on the methodological development of this field. Furthermore, this article contributes to a scholarly discussion geared toward expanding the spaces of ethological research. My analysis of the image collections of the Forschungsstätte für Tierpsychologie presents the archive as a relevant site of study in the history of ethology.

本文回顾了20世纪中期视觉媒介在行为学上的认识论历程。最重要的是,行为学家声称与研究动物有过密切接触,并从这些人类-动物群体中提取了科学证据,尤其是在公共关系方面。然而,如果我们看看比较行为生物学家的工具箱,很明显,科学有效的研究结果主要是通过实验模型图像获得的。这些视觉标本讲述了行为科学中必要的方法要求的技术故事,这些方法要求连接了实验室和现场之间的日常观察。通过消除动物身体上的个体痕迹以及它们的观察者,他们促进了动物行为学假说的抽象。对东德生物学家g nter Tembrock(1918-2011)的案例研究为该领域的方法论发展提供了一个新的视角,他保存了自己的剪报、绘画、照片和电影。此外,本文有助于扩大行为学研究空间的学术讨论。我对Forschungsstätte r Tierpsychologie的图像集的分析将该档案作为动物行为学历史研究的相关站点。
{"title":"Red Foxes in the Filing Cabinet: Günter Tembrock's Image Collection and Media Use in Mid-Century Ethology**","authors":"Sophia Gräfe","doi":"10.1002/bewi.202200004","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bewi.202200004","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This paper considers the epistemic career of visual media in ethology in the mid-20th century. Above all, ethologists claimed close contact with research animals and drew scientific evidence from these human-animal communities, particularly in public relations. However, if we look into the toolboxes of comparative behavioral biologists, it becomes evident that scientifically valid research results were primarily obtained by experimenting with model images. These visual specimens tell a technical story of the methodological requirements in behavioral science necessary to bridge everyday observations between the laboratory and the field. By neutralizing individual traces of animal bodies as well as their observers, they prompted the abstraction of ethological hypotheses. The case study of East-German biologist Günter Tembrock (1918–2011), who maintained his own collection of newspaper clippings, drawings, photographs, and films, offers a new perspective on the methodological development of this field. Furthermore, this article contributes to a scholarly discussion geared toward expanding the spaces of ethological research. My analysis of the image collections of the <i>Forschungsstätte für Tierpsychologie</i> presents the archive as a relevant site of study in the history of ethology.</p>","PeriodicalId":55388,"journal":{"name":"Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte","volume":"45 1-2","pages":"55-86"},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2022-05-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bewi.202200004","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45955800","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"哲学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
The Farm Hall Transcripts: The Smoking Gun That Wasn't 农场大厅记录:事实并非如此
IF 0.6 2区 哲学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-05-05 DOI: 10.1002/bewi.202100033
Ryan Dahn

Over 75 years after their creation, the Farm Hall transcripts remain a tantalizing source from the dawn of the atomic age in 1945. Declassified in 1992, the transcripts document ten prominent German nuclear physicists, including Werner Heisenberg, Carl Friedrich von Weizsäcker, and Otto Hahn, contemplating the Nazi defeat, their complicity in the German war machine, and – after the atomic bomb was dropped on Hiroshima – whether they truly intended to build a nuclear weapon for Adolf Hitler. As a written record of conversations, one might expect the transcripts to be the proverbial smoking gun that determines, once and for all, whether German physicists intended to build a nuclear weapon for the Nazi regime. Yet the Farm Hall transcripts have been used to support starkly divergent arguments. Some have used them to assert that the Germans would have willingly provided Hitler with a bomb if only they could; others view them as evidence of scientific resistance inside the Nazi regime. This article explores why the Farm Hall transcripts are not the smoking gun they appear to be.

从1945年原子时代的黎明开始,在75年后的今天,Farm Hall的手稿仍然是一个诱人的来源。1992年解密的文件记录了10位杰出的德国核物理学家,包括维尔纳·海森堡、卡尔·弗里德里希·冯Weizsäcker和奥托·哈恩,他们思考纳粹的失败,他们在德国战争机器中的同谋,以及在广岛投下原子弹后,他们是否真的打算为阿道夫·希特勒制造核武器。作为对话的书面记录,人们可能会期望这些文字记录是众所周知的确凿证据,可以一劳永逸地确定德国物理学家是否打算为纳粹政权制造核武器。然而,农场大厅的记录却被用来支持截然不同的观点。一些人利用它们断言,只要有可能,德国人会心甘情愿地向希特勒提供炸弹;其他人则将其视为纳粹政权内部科学抵抗的证据。这篇文章探讨了为什么农场大厅的记录并不是他们看起来的确凿证据。
{"title":"The Farm Hall Transcripts: The Smoking Gun That Wasn't","authors":"Ryan Dahn","doi":"10.1002/bewi.202100033","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bewi.202100033","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Over 75 years after their creation, the Farm Hall transcripts remain a tantalizing source from the dawn of the atomic age in 1945. Declassified in 1992, the transcripts document ten prominent German nuclear physicists, including Werner Heisenberg, Carl Friedrich von Weizsäcker, and Otto Hahn, contemplating the Nazi defeat, their complicity in the German war machine, and – after the atomic bomb was dropped on Hiroshima – whether they truly intended to build a nuclear weapon for Adolf Hitler. As a written record of conversations, one might expect the transcripts to be the proverbial smoking gun that determines, once and for all, whether German physicists intended to build a nuclear weapon for the Nazi regime. Yet the Farm Hall transcripts have been used to support starkly divergent arguments. Some have used them to assert that the Germans would have willingly provided Hitler with a bomb if only they could; others view them as evidence of scientific resistance inside the Nazi regime. This article explores why the Farm Hall transcripts are not the smoking gun they appear to be.</p>","PeriodicalId":55388,"journal":{"name":"Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte","volume":"45 1-2","pages":"202-218"},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2022-05-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44676393","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"哲学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
“It Felt More like a Revolution.” How Behavioral Ecology Succeeded Ethology, 1970–1990 “感觉更像是一场革命。”行为生态学如何继承行为学,1970-1990
IF 0.6 2区 哲学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-04-25 DOI: 10.1002/bewi.202200002
Cora Stuhrmann

As soon as ethology's status diminished in the early 1970s, it was confronted with two successor disciplines, sociobiology and behavioral ecology. They were able to challenge ethology because it no longer provided markers of strong disciplinarity such as theoretical coherence, leading figures and a clear identity. While behavioral ecology developed organically out of the UK ethological research community into its own disciplinary standing, sociobiology presented itself as a US competitor to the ethological tradition. I will show how behavioral ecology took the role of legitimate heir to ethology by rebuilding a theoretical core and an intellectual sense of community, while sociobiology failed to use its public appeal to reach disciplinary status. Meanwhile, ethology changed its disciplinary identity to encompass all biological studies of animal behavior.

行为学的地位在20世纪70年代初一下降,它就面临着两个后继学科,社会生物学和行为生态学。他们之所以能够挑战动物行为学,是因为它不再提供强学科性的标志,如理论一致性、领导人物和明确的身份。当行为生态学从英国的动物行为学研究界有机地发展成自己的学科地位时,社会生物学表现出自己是美国动物行为学传统的竞争对手。我将展示行为生态学如何通过重建理论核心和社区的知识意识而成为行为学的合法继承人,而社会生物学未能利用其公众吸引力达到学科地位。与此同时,动物行为学改变了它的学科身份,涵盖了所有动物行为的生物学研究。
{"title":"“It Felt More like a Revolution.” How Behavioral Ecology Succeeded Ethology, 1970–1990","authors":"Cora Stuhrmann","doi":"10.1002/bewi.202200002","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bewi.202200002","url":null,"abstract":"<p>As soon as ethology's status diminished in the early 1970s, it was confronted with two successor disciplines, sociobiology and behavioral ecology. They were able to challenge ethology because it no longer provided markers of strong disciplinarity such as theoretical coherence, leading figures and a clear identity. While behavioral ecology developed organically out of the UK ethological research community into its own disciplinary standing, sociobiology presented itself as a US competitor to the ethological tradition. I will show how behavioral ecology took the role of legitimate heir to ethology by rebuilding a theoretical core and an intellectual sense of community, while sociobiology failed to use its public appeal to reach disciplinary status. Meanwhile, ethology changed its disciplinary identity to encompass all biological studies of animal behavior.</p>","PeriodicalId":55388,"journal":{"name":"Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte","volume":"45 1-2","pages":"135-163"},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2022-04-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bewi.202200002","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44974220","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"哲学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
Telemedical Second Opinions in Germany: A Customer Survey of an Online Portal. 德国远程医疗第二意见:在线门户网站客户调查。
IF 4.7 2区 哲学 Q2 HISTORY & PHILOSOPHY OF SCIENCE Pub Date : 2022-04-08 DOI: 10.1089/tmj.2022.0070
Nadja Könsgen, Barbara Prediger, Anna Schlimbach, Ana-Mihaela Bora, Victoria Weißflog, Jan-Christoph Loh, Dunja Bruch, Dawid Pieper

Introduction: Second medical opinions (SOs) can strengthen patients' certainty in decision making. In Germany, both personally delivered and telemedical SOs (often based on documents only) are provided. Our aim was to analyze the experiences of people who obtained telemedical SOs. We also investigated different routes of SO delivery (personally/by phone/documents only). Materials and Methods: German residents who obtained a telemedical SO via an online portal between January 2016 and February 2019 (n = 1,247) were contacted by post between August and November 2019 up to three times. The results were analyzed descriptively. Results: The 368 participants (response rate 30%) were 54% male, 95% statutory health insured, and 61 years old (median; interquartile range 51-72). Approximately 75% were (rather) satisfied with obtaining the SO via the online portal. The most preferred route of SO delivery was a personally delivered SO, which 80% would (rather) consider, followed by 70% (rather) considering SOs based on documents only and 48% (rather) considering SOs by phone. The most often mentioned advantage of telemedical SOs was independence of time and place, while the most important disadvantage was the standardized process resulting in a lack of direct and personal contact between the patient and the physician. Discussion: Although our results show that SOs (based on documents only) support patients and that patient satisfaction was high, personally delivered SOs were still preferred. Future research on the use of SOs based on documents only (in which patient population and in what situations) is needed.

引言第二医疗意见(SO)可以增强患者做出决定时的确定性。在德国,既有亲自送达的第二医疗意见,也有远程医疗意见(通常仅基于文件)。我们的目的是分析人们获得远程医疗第二医疗意见的经历。我们还调查了提供《手术申请表》的不同途径(亲自递送/电话递送/仅凭文件递送)。材料和方法:从 2016 年 1 月到 2019 年 2 月,通过在线门户网站获得远程医疗 SO 的德国居民(n = 1,247 人)在 2019 年 8 月到 11 月期间通过邮局联系了他们,最多联系了三次。对结果进行了描述性分析。结果:368 名参与者(回复率为 30%)中,54% 为男性,95% 有法定医疗保险,61 岁(中位数;四分位数间距为 51-72)。约 75% 的人对通过在线门户网站获取《战略计划》感到(相当)满意。最喜欢的《战略计划书》递送途径是亲自递送,80% 的人会(更愿意)考虑这种途径,其次是 70%(更愿意)考虑仅根据文件递送《战略计划书》,48%(更愿意)考虑通过电话递送《战略计划书》。人们最常提到的远程医疗手术的优点是不受时间和地点的限制,而最重要的缺点是标准化流程导致患者与医生之间缺乏直接的个人接触。讨论:尽管我们的研究结果表明,SO(仅基于文件)为患者提供了支持,而且患者的满意度很高,但亲自提供的 SO 仍是首选。今后需要对仅以文件为基础的《手术指南》的使用情况(在哪些患者群体和哪些情况下)进行研究。
{"title":"Telemedical Second Opinions in Germany: A Customer Survey of an Online Portal.","authors":"Nadja Könsgen, Barbara Prediger, Anna Schlimbach, Ana-Mihaela Bora, Victoria Weißflog, Jan-Christoph Loh, Dunja Bruch, Dawid Pieper","doi":"10.1089/tmj.2022.0070","DOIUrl":"10.1089/tmj.2022.0070","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p><b>Introduction:</b> Second medical opinions (SOs) can strengthen patients' certainty in decision making. In Germany, both personally delivered and telemedical SOs (often based on documents only) are provided. Our aim was to analyze the experiences of people who obtained telemedical SOs. We also investigated different routes of SO delivery (personally/by phone/documents only). <b>Materials and Methods:</b> German residents who obtained a telemedical SO via an online portal between January 2016 and February 2019 (<i>n</i> = 1,247) were contacted by post between August and November 2019 up to three times. The results were analyzed descriptively. <b>Results:</b> The 368 participants (response rate 30%) were 54% male, 95% statutory health insured, and 61 years old (median; interquartile range 51-72). Approximately 75% were (rather) satisfied with obtaining the SO via the online portal. The most preferred route of SO delivery was a personally delivered SO, which 80% would (rather) consider, followed by 70% (rather) considering SOs based on documents only and 48% (rather) considering SOs by phone. The most often mentioned advantage of telemedical SOs was independence of time and place, while the most important disadvantage was the standardized process resulting in a lack of direct and personal contact between the patient and the physician. <b>Discussion:</b> Although our results show that SOs (based on documents only) support patients and that patient satisfaction was high, personally delivered SOs were still preferred. Future research on the use of SOs based on documents only (in which patient population and in what situations) is needed.</p>","PeriodicalId":55388,"journal":{"name":"Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte","volume":"19 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2022-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"73101934","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"哲学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Critical Periods in Science and the Science of Critical Periods: Canine Behavior in America 科学的关键时期和关键时期的科学:美国的犬类行为
IF 0.6 2区 哲学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-03-09 DOI: 10.1002/bewi.202100025
Brad Bolman

This article offers a canine history of the “critical period” concept, situating its emergence within a growing, interdisciplinary network of canine behavior studies that connected eugenically minded American veterinarians, behavioral geneticists, and dog lovers with large institutional benefactors. These studies established both logistical and conceptual foundations for large-scale science with dogs while establishing a lingering interdependence between American dog science and eugenics. The article emphasizes the importance of dogs as subjects of ethological study, particularly in the United States, where some of the earliest organized efforts to analyze canine behavior began. Further, the article argues that the “critical period” is important not only for its lasting prominence in multiple fields of scientific inquiry, but also as a historiographical tool, one that invites reflection on the tendency of historians to emphasize a particular narrative structure of scientific advancement.

这篇文章提供了犬类“关键时期”概念的历史,将其出现在犬类行为研究的一个不断发展的跨学科网络中,该网络将具有优生学思想的美国兽医、行为遗传学家、爱狗人士与大型机构捐助者联系起来。这些研究为大规模的狗科学奠定了后勤和概念基础,同时在美国狗科学和优生学之间建立了持久的相互依存关系。这篇文章强调了狗作为动物行为学研究对象的重要性,特别是在美国,最早的一些有组织的分析犬类行为的努力开始于美国。此外,本文还认为,“关键时期”的重要性不仅在于它在多个科学探究领域的持久突出,而且还在于它是一种史学工具,它促使人们反思历史学家强调科学进步的特定叙事结构的倾向。
{"title":"Critical Periods in Science and the Science of Critical Periods: Canine Behavior in America","authors":"Brad Bolman","doi":"10.1002/bewi.202100025","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bewi.202100025","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This article offers a canine history of the “critical period” concept, situating its emergence within a growing, interdisciplinary network of canine behavior studies that connected eugenically minded American veterinarians, behavioral geneticists, and dog lovers with large institutional benefactors. These studies established both logistical and conceptual foundations for large-scale science with dogs while establishing a lingering interdependence between American dog science and eugenics. The article emphasizes the importance of dogs as subjects of ethological study, particularly in the United States, where some of the earliest organized efforts to analyze canine behavior began. Further, the article argues that the “critical period” is important not only for its lasting prominence in multiple fields of scientific inquiry, but also as a historiographical tool, one that invites reflection on the tendency of historians to emphasize a particular narrative structure of scientific advancement.</p>","PeriodicalId":55388,"journal":{"name":"Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte","volume":"45 1-2","pages":"112-134"},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2022-03-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bewi.202100025","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42739102","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"哲学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Landscapes of Time: Building Long-Term Perspectives in Animal Behavior* 时间景观:建立动物行为的长期视角*
IF 0.6 2区 哲学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-03-08 DOI: 10.1002/bewi.202100026
Erika Lorraine Milam

In the 1960s, scientists fascinated by the behavior of free-living animals founded research projects that expanded into multi-generation investigations. This paper charts the history of three scientists’ projects to uncover the varied reasons for investing in a “long-term” perspective when studying animal behavior: Kenneth Armitage's study of marmots in the Rocky Mountains, Jeanne Altmann's analysis of baboons in Kenya, and Timothy Hugh Clutton-Brock's studies (among others) of red deer on the island of Rhum and meerkats in the Kalahari. The desire to study the behavior of the same group of animals over extended periods of time, I argue, came from different methodological traditions – population biology, primatology, and sociobiology – even as each saw themselves as contributing to the legacy of ethology. As scientists embraced and combined these approaches, a small number of long-running behavioral ecology projects like these grew from short pilot projects into decades-long centers of intellectual gravity within behavioral ecology as a discipline. By attending to time as well as place, we can see how this long-term perspective was crucial to their success; they measured evolutionary changes over generations of animals and their data provided insights into how the animals they studied were adapting (or not) to changing local and global environmental factors.

在20世纪60年代,被自由生活动物的行为所吸引的科学家们建立了研究项目,并扩展到多代研究。这篇论文描绘了三位科学家项目的历史,以揭示在研究动物行为时投资于“长期”观点的各种原因:肯尼斯·阿米蒂奇对落基山脉土拨鼠的研究,珍妮·奥尔特曼对肯尼亚狒狒的分析,以及蒂莫西·休·克拉顿-布洛克对拉姆岛马鹿和喀拉哈里沙漠猫鼬的研究(以及其他研究)。我认为,研究同一群动物长期行为的愿望来自不同的方法传统——种群生物学、灵长类学和社会生物学——尽管每一种方法都认为自己对动物行为学的遗产有所贡献。随着科学家们接受并结合这些方法,像这样的少数长期运行的行为生态学项目从短期的试点项目发展成为行为生态学作为一门学科长达数十年的智力重心中心。通过关注时间和地点,我们可以看到这种长期视角对他们的成功至关重要;他们测量了几代动物的进化变化,他们的数据提供了他们所研究的动物如何适应(或不适应)不断变化的当地和全球环境因素的见解。
{"title":"Landscapes of Time: Building Long-Term Perspectives in Animal Behavior*","authors":"Erika Lorraine Milam","doi":"10.1002/bewi.202100026","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bewi.202100026","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In the 1960s, scientists fascinated by the behavior of free-living animals founded research projects that expanded into multi-generation investigations. This paper charts the history of three scientists’ projects to uncover the varied reasons for investing in a “long-term” perspective when studying animal behavior: Kenneth Armitage's study of marmots in the Rocky Mountains, Jeanne Altmann's analysis of baboons in Kenya, and Timothy Hugh Clutton-Brock's studies (among others) of red deer on the island of Rhum and meerkats in the Kalahari. The desire to study the behavior of the same group of animals over extended periods of time, I argue, came from different methodological traditions – population biology, primatology, and sociobiology – even as each saw themselves as contributing to the legacy of ethology. As scientists embraced and combined these approaches, a small number of long-running behavioral ecology projects like these grew from short pilot projects into decades-long centers of intellectual gravity within behavioral ecology as a discipline. By attending to time as well as place, we can see how this long-term perspective was crucial to their success; they measured evolutionary changes over generations of animals and their data provided insights into how the animals they studied were adapting (or not) to changing local and global environmental factors.</p>","PeriodicalId":55388,"journal":{"name":"Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte","volume":"45 1-2","pages":"164-188"},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2022-03-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49272378","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"哲学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
From Karl von Frisch to Neuroethology: A Methodological Perspective on the Frischean Tradition's Expansion into Neuroethology** 从卡尔·冯·弗里希到神经行为学:弗里希传统向神经行为学扩展的方法论视角**
IF 0.6 2区 哲学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-03-08 DOI: 10.1002/bewi.202200003
Kelle Dhein

This paper examines a tradition of eusocial insect research stemming from the Austrian zoologist Karl von Frisch. As I show in this paper, one of the most enduring features of the Frischean tradition has been an experimental methodology developed by Frisch in the early 1910s. By tracing this methodology's use through Frisch's student, Martin Lindauer, and two of Lindauer's students, Rüdiger Wehner and Randolf Menzel, this paper illuminates a surprising aspect of ethology's development during the last half of the 20th century. Namely, it sheds light on how the Frischean tradition, a tradition that had a complicated relationship with ethology since the discipline's formation in the 1930s, produced scientists who became leading figures in neuroethology, the most prominent contemporary field of behavioral research to retain the label of “ethology.” Some of the features that distinguished Frisch's training method from the program of classical ethology and the work of his contemporaries later helped his academic descendants adapt the method to the neuroethological program.

本文考察了源于奥地利动物学家卡尔·冯·弗里施的真社会性昆虫研究传统。正如我在本文中所展示的,弗里希传统最持久的特征之一是弗里希在20世纪10年代早期开发的实验方法。通过追踪弗里施的学生马丁·林道尔(Martin Lindauer)和林道尔的两个学生,rdiger Wehner和Randolf Menzel对这一方法论的使用,本文阐明了行为学在20世纪后半叶发展的一个令人惊讶的方面。也就是说,它揭示了弗里舍恩传统是如何培养出神经行为学(当代最杰出的行为研究领域,保留了“行为学”的标签)领域的领军人物的。自20世纪30年代该学科形成以来,弗里舍恩传统与行为学有着复杂的关系。弗里希的训练方法不同于古典动物行为学和他同时代的工作的一些特点后来帮助他的学术后代将这种方法应用于神经动物行为学项目。
{"title":"From Karl von Frisch to Neuroethology: A Methodological Perspective on the Frischean Tradition's Expansion into Neuroethology**","authors":"Kelle Dhein","doi":"10.1002/bewi.202200003","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bewi.202200003","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This paper examines a tradition of eusocial insect research stemming from the Austrian zoologist Karl von Frisch. As I show in this paper, one of the most enduring features of the Frischean tradition has been an experimental methodology developed by Frisch in the early 1910s. By tracing this methodology's use through Frisch's student, Martin Lindauer, and two of Lindauer's students, Rüdiger Wehner and Randolf Menzel, this paper illuminates a surprising aspect of ethology's development during the last half of the 20<sup>th</sup> century. Namely, it sheds light on how the Frischean tradition, a tradition that had a complicated relationship with ethology since the discipline's formation in the 1930s, produced scientists who became leading figures in <i>neuroethology</i>, the most prominent contemporary field of behavioral research to retain the label of “ethology.” Some of the features that distinguished Frisch's training method from the program of classical ethology and the work of his contemporaries later helped his academic descendants adapt the method to the neuroethological program.</p>","PeriodicalId":55388,"journal":{"name":"Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte","volume":"45 1-2","pages":"30-54"},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2022-03-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47500776","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"哲学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
期刊
Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1