首页 > 最新文献

Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology最新文献

英文 中文
Do Early Life Experiences Predict Variation in the General Factor of Personality (GFP)? 早期的生活经历可以预测人格的一般因素(GFP)的变化吗?
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-09-29 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-021-00177-1
Kristine J. Chua, Aaron W. Lukaszewski, Joseph H. Manson

Objective

The controversial General Factor of Personality (GFP) has been proposed as an indicator of social effectiveness and a slower life history strategy. An alternative hypothesis holds that only meta-trait alpha, comprising agreeableness, conscientiousness, and emotional stability, is a slow life history indicator. This study tested whether the GFP and/or alpha emerges from both self- and stranger-ratings, and whether either is predicted by indicators of harsh childhood ecologies.

Methods

U.S. undergraduate participants (N = 366) completed a Big Five instrument, a measure of socially desirable response bias, and brief (thin slice) videotaped interviews. Raters scored the interviews using the same Big Five instrument.

Results

Structural equation modeling of the self-report data yielded a well-fitting GFP, which was positively associated with father closeness. Meta-trait alpha, based on self-report, was associated with both father closeness and neighborhood stress, but showed positive loadings only for agreeableness and emotional stability. Stranger-rating data failed to yield either a well-fitting GFP or metatrait alpha.

Conclusions

Our findings are equivocal regarding the usefulness of the GFP specifically, and higher-order personality factors generally, in evolutionary personality science.

目的有争议的一般人格因素(GFP)被认为是社会有效性的指标,也是一种较慢的生活史策略。另一种假设认为,只有包括宜人性、尽责性和情绪稳定性在内的元特质阿尔法是一个缓慢的生活史指标。这项研究测试了GFP和/或α是否来自自我和陌生人的评分,以及两者是否都是由严酷的儿童生态指标预测的。方法美国本科生(N = 366)完成了一个五大工具,一个衡量社会期望的反应偏见的指标,以及简短的(薄片)录像采访。评分者使用相同的五大工具对采访进行评分。结果自我报告数据的结构方程模型产生了一个拟合良好的GFP,它与父亲的亲密度呈正相关。基于自我报告的元特质α与父亲亲密度和邻里压力都相关,但仅对宜人性和情绪稳定性表现出正负荷。Stranger评级数据未能产生良好拟合的GFP或metatraitα。结论关于GFP在进化人格科学中的作用,我们的发现是模棱两可的。
{"title":"Do Early Life Experiences Predict Variation in the General Factor of Personality (GFP)?","authors":"Kristine J. Chua,&nbsp;Aaron W. Lukaszewski,&nbsp;Joseph H. Manson","doi":"10.1007/s40750-021-00177-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-021-00177-1","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>The controversial General Factor of Personality (GFP) has been proposed as an indicator of social effectiveness and a slower life history strategy. An alternative hypothesis holds that only meta-trait <i>alpha</i>, comprising agreeableness, conscientiousness, and emotional stability, is a slow life history indicator. This study tested whether the GFP and/or <i>alpha</i> emerges from both self- and stranger-ratings, and whether either is predicted by indicators of harsh childhood ecologies.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>U.S. undergraduate participants (<i>N</i> = 366) completed a Big Five instrument, a measure of socially desirable response bias, and brief (thin slice) videotaped interviews. Raters scored the interviews using the same Big Five instrument.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>Structural equation modeling of the self-report data yielded a well-fitting GFP, which was positively associated with father closeness. Meta-trait <i>alpha</i>, based on self-report, was associated with both father closeness and neighborhood stress, but showed positive loadings only for agreeableness and emotional stability. Stranger-rating data failed to yield either a well-fitting GFP or metatrait <i>alpha.</i></p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Our findings are equivocal regarding the usefulness of the GFP specifically, and higher-order personality factors generally, in evolutionary personality science.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2021-09-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-021-00177-1.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50524670","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Crowd Salience Heightens Tolerance to Healthy Facial Features 人群突出提高对健康面部特征的容忍度
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-09-21 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-021-00176-2
Mitch Brown, Ryan E. Tracy, Steven G. Young, Donald F. Sacco

Objective

Recent findings suggest crowd salience heightens pathogen-avoidant motives, serving to reduce individuals’ infection risk through interpersonal contact. Such experiences may similarly facilitate the identification, and avoidance, of diseased conspecifics. The current experiment sought to replicate and extend previous crowding research.

Methods

In this experiment, we primed participants at two universities with either a crowding or control experience before having them evaluate faces manipulated to appear healthy or diseased by indicating the degree to which they would want to interact with them.

Results

Crowding-primed participants reported a more heightened preferences for healthy faces than control-primed participants. Additionally, crowd salience reduced aversion toward healthy faces but did not heighten aversion to diseased faces.

Conclusion

Results suggest crowding appears to heighten tolerance for health cues given the heightened proximal threat of infections through interpersonal contact within crowded environments. Conversely, this work extends previous findings by indicating this preference is not rooted in an aversion to cues of poor health. We frame findings from a threat management perspective in understanding how crowding fosters sensitivity toward pathogenic threats.

最近的研究结果表明,群体显著性增强了病原体回避动机,有助于降低个体通过人际接触感染的风险。类似地,这些经验可能有助于识别和避免患病的同种动物。目前的实验试图复制和扩展以前的拥挤研究。方法在这项实验中,我们让两所大学的参与者有拥挤或控制的经历,然后让他们通过指示他们想与他们互动的程度来评估被操纵的脸是否看起来健康或有病。结果人群引导的参与者比对照引导的参与者对健康面孔的偏好更高。此外,人群显著性降低了对健康面孔的厌恶,但并没有增加对患病面孔的厌恶。结论研究结果表明,考虑到在拥挤环境中通过人际接触感染的近端威胁增加,拥挤似乎提高了对健康提示的耐受性。相反,这项工作扩展了之前的发现,表明这种偏好并非源于对健康状况不佳的厌恶。我们从威胁管理的角度来理解拥挤如何培养对致病威胁的敏感性。
{"title":"Crowd Salience Heightens Tolerance to Healthy Facial Features","authors":"Mitch Brown,&nbsp;Ryan E. Tracy,&nbsp;Steven G. Young,&nbsp;Donald F. Sacco","doi":"10.1007/s40750-021-00176-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-021-00176-2","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Recent findings suggest crowd salience heightens pathogen-avoidant motives, serving to reduce individuals’ infection risk through interpersonal contact. Such experiences may similarly facilitate the identification, and avoidance, of diseased conspecifics. The current experiment sought to replicate and extend previous crowding research.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>In this experiment, we primed participants at two universities with either a crowding or control experience before having them evaluate faces manipulated to appear healthy or diseased by indicating the degree to which they would want to interact with them.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>Crowding-primed participants reported a more heightened preferences for healthy faces than control-primed participants. Additionally, crowd salience reduced aversion toward healthy faces but did not heighten aversion to diseased faces.</p><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>Results suggest crowding appears to heighten tolerance for health cues given the heightened proximal threat of infections through interpersonal contact within crowded environments. Conversely, this work extends previous findings by indicating this preference is not rooted in an aversion to cues of poor health. We frame findings from a threat management perspective in understanding how crowding fosters sensitivity toward pathogenic threats.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2021-09-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-021-00176-2.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39454383","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Maternal Cortisol and Paternal Testosterone Correlated with Infant Growth via Mini Puberty 母亲皮质醇和父亲睾酮通过小青春期与婴儿生长相关
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-09-18 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-021-00175-3
Randy Corpuz, Sophia E. D’Alessandro, Julia M. Moon

Objectives

Infant testosterone (T) surges early in life. This period (known as mini puberty) is crucial to development. Little is known as to what the ultimate function of mini puberty might be. We predicted that parents with putative endocrine signatures of challenging environments (elevated levels of maternal cortisol and paternal T) may be related to elevated levels of infant T. In turn, these endocrine relationships are hypothesized to influence infant growth.

Methods

In a U.S. sample (n = 225 families) of first-time parents and their infants, we measured infant length and weight at three occasions—birth, 3 months old, and 10 months old. We conducted salivary assays of infants for T, mothers for cortisol, and paternal T during the early postnatal period.

Results

We utilized latent growth curve modeling to explore changes in length and weight as predicted by infant T. Infant T predicted the slope of length gains across the study period. Maternal cortisol and paternal T (positively correlated with one another) were positively related to infant T. Neither maternal cortisol nor paternal T predicted the slope of length gains. In an exploratory model, temperament was not related to neuroendocrine measures. Gains in weight—unlike length—were not related to infant T.

Conclusions

The ultimate function of mini puberty in infant growth is nuanced. In addition—at a time of rapid hormone changes across mothers, fathers, and infants—our results suggest that a tripartite neuroendocrine relationship is conceivable. Discussion surrounds the potential role of mini puberty and the numerous limitations of the study.

目的胎儿睾酮(T)在生命早期激增。这个时期(称为小青春期)对发育至关重要。关于小青春期的最终功能,我们知之甚少。我们预测,具有挑战性环境内分泌特征的父母(母体皮质醇和父亲T水平升高)可能与婴儿T水平升高有关。反过来,这些内分泌关系被假设会影响婴儿的生长。方法在美国样本(n = 225个家庭),我们在三个场合测量了婴儿的身高和体重——出生、3个月大和10个月大。在出生后早期,我们对婴儿的T、母亲的皮质醇和父亲的T进行了唾液检测。结果我们利用潜在生长曲线模型来探索婴儿T预测的长度和体重的变化。婴儿T预测了整个研究期间长度增加的斜率。母亲的皮质醇和父亲的T(彼此呈正相关)与婴儿T呈正相关。母亲的皮质醇或父亲的T都不能预测长度增加的斜率。在一个探索性模型中,气质与神经内分泌指标无关。与身高不同,体重的增加与婴儿T无关。结论青春期对婴儿生长的最终作用是微妙的。此外,在母亲、父亲和婴儿荷尔蒙快速变化的时候,我们的研究结果表明,三方神经内分泌关系是可以想象的。讨论围绕着小青春期的潜在作用和该研究的众多局限性。
{"title":"Maternal Cortisol and Paternal Testosterone Correlated with Infant Growth via Mini Puberty","authors":"Randy Corpuz,&nbsp;Sophia E. D’Alessandro,&nbsp;Julia M. Moon","doi":"10.1007/s40750-021-00175-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-021-00175-3","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objectives</h3><p>Infant testosterone (T) surges early in life. This period (known as mini puberty) is crucial to development. Little is known as to what the ultimate function of mini puberty might be. We predicted that parents with putative endocrine signatures of challenging environments (elevated levels of maternal cortisol and paternal T) may be related to elevated levels of infant T. In turn, these endocrine relationships are hypothesized to influence infant growth.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>In a U.S. sample (n = 225 families) of first-time parents and their infants, we measured infant length and weight at three occasions—birth, 3 months old, and 10 months old. We conducted salivary assays of infants for T, mothers for cortisol, and paternal T during the early postnatal period.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>We utilized latent growth curve modeling to explore changes in length and weight as predicted by infant T. Infant T predicted the slope of length gains across the study period. Maternal cortisol and paternal T (positively correlated with one another) were positively related to infant T. Neither maternal cortisol nor paternal T predicted the slope of length gains. In an exploratory model, temperament was not related to neuroendocrine measures. Gains in weight—unlike length—were not related to infant T.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>The ultimate function of mini puberty in infant growth is nuanced. In addition—at a time of rapid hormone changes across mothers, fathers, and infants—our results suggest that a tripartite neuroendocrine relationship is conceivable. Discussion surrounds the potential role of mini puberty and the numerous limitations of the study.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2021-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-021-00175-3.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43742504","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Testing Mate Choice Hypotheses in a Transitional Small Scale Population 在过渡小规模人群中检验配偶选择假设
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-09-03 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-021-00173-5
Lynda G. Boothroyd, Jean-Luc Jucker, Tracey Thornborrow, Martin J. Tovee, Carlota Batres, Ian Penton-Voak

Objective

Tests of theories of mate choice often rely on data gathered in White, industrialised samples and this is especially the case for studies of facial attraction. Our understanding of preferences for sexual dimorphism is currently in flux and a number of hypotheses require testing in more diverse participant samples. The current study uses opportunistically gathered facial dimorphism preference data from 271 participants in rural Nicaragua, and 40 from the national capital Managua. We assess pre-registered hypotheses drawn from sexual selection theory, and from more recent approaches which consider the impacts of economic development and cultural ‘modernisation’ on mate preferences.

Methods

Participants verbally reported demographic data, and indicated preferences for five male and five female pairs of faces manipulated to differ in sexually dimorphic facial structure based on a sample of Salvadoran individuals.

Results

While urban participants showed a preference for more feminine female faces, this preference was not evident in the rural participants. Neither urban nor rural participants showed any directional preference for masculinised/feminised male faces. Furthermore, there was no support for any other pre-registered hypothesis.

Conclusions

Our results are consistent with previous studies showing no interest in facial dimorphism in less globally-acculturated, or market integrated, populations. Together, this suggests that while facial dimorphism may be subject to systematically varying preferences amongst some low-fertility, industrialised populations, it is not a feature which is likely to have been important in ancestral populations. We call for further work attempting to replicate well known mate choice phenomena in more diverse samples.

择偶理论的客观测试通常依赖于从白人工业化样本中收集的数据,尤其是面部吸引力的研究。我们对两性异形偏好的理解目前正在不断变化,许多假设需要在更多样的参与者样本中进行测试。目前的研究使用了机会主义收集的来自尼加拉瓜农村271名参与者和首都马那瓜40名参与者的面部二态偏好数据。我们评估了从性选择理论和最近的方法中得出的预先注册的假设,这些方法考虑了经济发展和文化“现代化”对配偶偏好的影响。方法参与者口头报告人口统计数据,并根据萨尔瓦多个体的样本,表示对五对男性和五对女性面孔的偏好,这两对面孔被操纵以使面部结构在性二型方面有所不同。结果虽然城市参与者表现出对女性面孔的偏好,但这种偏好在农村参与者中并不明显。无论是城市还是农村的参与者都没有表现出对男性化/女性化男性面孔的任何定向偏好。此外,没有任何其他预先登记的假设得到支持。结论我们的研究结果与之前的研究一致,之前的研究表明,在全球文化适应程度较低或市场整合程度较低的人群中,对面部二型性不感兴趣。总之,这表明,虽然在一些低生育率的工业化人群中,面部二型可能会受到系统性的不同偏好的影响,但这在祖先群体中可能并不重要。我们呼吁进一步努力,试图在更多样化的样本中复制众所周知的择偶现象。
{"title":"Testing Mate Choice Hypotheses in a Transitional Small Scale Population","authors":"Lynda G. Boothroyd,&nbsp;Jean-Luc Jucker,&nbsp;Tracey Thornborrow,&nbsp;Martin J. Tovee,&nbsp;Carlota Batres,&nbsp;Ian Penton-Voak","doi":"10.1007/s40750-021-00173-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-021-00173-5","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Tests of theories of mate choice often rely on data gathered in White, industrialised samples and this is especially the case for studies of facial attraction. Our understanding of preferences for sexual dimorphism is currently in flux and a number of hypotheses require testing in more diverse participant samples. The current study uses opportunistically gathered facial dimorphism preference data from 271 participants in rural Nicaragua, and 40 from the national capital Managua. We assess pre-registered hypotheses drawn from sexual selection theory, and from more recent approaches which consider the impacts of economic development and cultural ‘modernisation’ on mate preferences.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>Participants verbally reported demographic data, and indicated preferences for five male and five female pairs of faces manipulated to differ in sexually dimorphic facial structure based on a sample of Salvadoran individuals.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>While urban participants showed a preference for more feminine female faces, this preference was not evident in the rural participants. Neither urban nor rural participants showed any directional preference for masculinised/feminised male faces. Furthermore, there was no support for any other pre-registered hypothesis.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Our results are consistent with previous studies showing no interest in facial dimorphism in less globally-acculturated, or market integrated, populations. Together, this suggests that while facial dimorphism may be subject to systematically varying preferences amongst some low-fertility, industrialised populations, it is not a feature which is likely to have been important in ancestral populations. We call for further work attempting to replicate well known mate choice phenomena in more diverse samples.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2021-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-021-00173-5.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46076062","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Self-Perceived Facial Attractiveness, Fluctuating Asymmetry, and Minor Ailments Predict Mental Health Outcomes 自我感知的面部吸引力、波动不对称和小病预测心理健康结果
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-08-31 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-021-00172-6
Javier I. Borráz-León, Markus J. Rantala, Severi Luoto, Indrikis A. Krams, Jorge Contreras-Garduño, Tatjana Krama, Ana Lilia Cerda-Molina

Objective

Phenotypic markers associated with developmental stability such as fluctuating asymmetry, facial attractiveness, and reports of minor ailments can also act as indicators of overall physical health. However, few studies have assessed whether these markers might also be cues of mental health. We tested whether self- and other-perceived facial attractiveness, fluctuating asymmetry, and minor ailments are associated with psychopathological symptoms in a mixed sample of 358 college students, controlling for the effects of body mass index, age, and sex.

Methods

We applied the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (SCL-90-R) questionnaire to assess psychopathological symptoms, a battery of questionnaires about self-perceptions of facial attractiveness, and gathered information about the number of previous minor ailments as well as demographic data. Other-perceived attractiveness was assessed by an independent mixed sample of 109 subjects. Subjects’ facial fluctuating asymmetry was determined by geometric morphometrics.

Results

The results revealed that in both men and women, higher self-perceived attractiveness and fewer minor ailments predicted lower scores of Somatization, Obsessive–Compulsive, Interpersonal Sensitivity, Depression, Anxiety, Phobic Anxiety, Paranoid Ideation, Psychoticism, and a General Psychopathology Index. Higher facial fluctuating asymmetry was associated with higher Interpersonal Sensitivity, but did not contribute to its prediction when controlling for the other studied variables.

Conclusions

The observed strong associations between self-perceived attractiveness, minor ailments, and psychopathology indicate common developmental pathways between physiological and psychological symptomatology which may reflect broader life history (co)variation between genetics, developmental environment, and psychophysiological functioning.

与发育稳定性相关的表型标志物,如波动的不对称性、面部吸引力和小病报告,也可以作为整体身体健康的指标。然而,很少有研究评估这些标志物是否也可能是心理健康的线索。我们在358名大学生的混合样本中测试了自我和其他感知的面部吸引力、波动的不对称性和轻微疾病是否与精神病理学症状有关,并控制了体重指数、年龄和性别的影响。方法我们采用症状自评量表(SCL-90-R)来评估精神病理学症状,这是一组关于面部吸引力自我感知的问卷,并收集了有关既往小病数量的信息以及人口统计数据。其他感知吸引力由109名受试者组成的独立混合样本进行评估。受试者的面部波动不对称性由几何形态计量学确定。结果结果显示,在男性和女性中,自我感知吸引力越高,小病越少,预示着躯体化、强迫症、人际敏感度、抑郁、焦虑、恐惧焦虑、偏执理想、精神病和一般精神病理学指数的得分越低。较高的面部波动不对称性与较高的人际敏感性相关,但在控制其他研究变量时,对其预测没有贡献。结论观察到的自我感知吸引力、小病和精神病理学之间的强烈关联表明,生理和心理症状学之间存在共同的发展途径,这可能反映了遗传学、发育环境和心理生理功能之间更广泛的生活史(共)变化。
{"title":"Self-Perceived Facial Attractiveness, Fluctuating Asymmetry, and Minor Ailments Predict Mental Health Outcomes","authors":"Javier I. Borráz-León,&nbsp;Markus J. Rantala,&nbsp;Severi Luoto,&nbsp;Indrikis A. Krams,&nbsp;Jorge Contreras-Garduño,&nbsp;Tatjana Krama,&nbsp;Ana Lilia Cerda-Molina","doi":"10.1007/s40750-021-00172-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-021-00172-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Phenotypic markers associated with developmental stability such as fluctuating asymmetry, facial attractiveness, and reports of minor ailments can also act as indicators of overall physical health. However, few studies have assessed whether these markers might also be cues of mental health. We tested whether self- and other-perceived facial attractiveness, fluctuating asymmetry, and minor ailments are associated with psychopathological symptoms in a mixed sample of 358 college students, controlling for the effects of body mass index, age, and sex.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>We applied the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (SCL-90-R) questionnaire to assess psychopathological symptoms, a battery of questionnaires about self-perceptions of facial attractiveness, and gathered information about the number of previous minor ailments as well as demographic data. Other-perceived attractiveness was assessed by an independent mixed sample of 109 subjects. Subjects’ facial fluctuating asymmetry was determined by geometric morphometrics.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>The results revealed that in both men and women, higher self-perceived attractiveness and fewer minor ailments predicted lower scores of Somatization, Obsessive–Compulsive, Interpersonal Sensitivity, Depression, Anxiety, Phobic Anxiety, Paranoid Ideation, Psychoticism, and a General Psychopathology Index. Higher facial fluctuating asymmetry was associated with higher Interpersonal Sensitivity, but did not contribute to its prediction when controlling for the other studied variables.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>The observed strong associations between self-perceived attractiveness, minor ailments, and psychopathology indicate common developmental pathways between physiological and psychological symptomatology which may reflect broader life history (co)variation between genetics, developmental environment, and psychophysiological functioning.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2021-08-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-021-00172-6.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44526751","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Do 3D Face Images Capture Cues of Strength, Weight, and Height Better than 2D Face Images do? 3D人脸图像比2D人脸图像更好地捕捉力量、体重和身高的线索吗?
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-08-26 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-021-00170-8
Iris J Holzleitner, Alex L Jones, Kieran J O’Shea, Rachel Cassar, Vanessa Fasolt, Victor Shiramizu, Benedict C Jones, Lisa M DeBruine

Objectives

A large literature exists investigating the extent to which physical characteristics (e.g., strength, weight, and height) can be accurately assessed from face images. While most of these studies have employed two-dimensional (2D) face images as stimuli, some recent studies have used three-dimensional (3D) face images because they may contain cues not visible in 2D face images. As equipment required for 3D face images is considerably more expensive than that required for 2D face images, we here investigated how perceptual ratings of physical characteristics from 2D and 3D face images compare.

Methods

We tested whether 3D face images capture cues of strength, weight, and height better than 2D face images do by directly comparing the accuracy of strength, weight, and height ratings of 182 2D and 3D face images taken simultaneously. Strength, height and weight were rated by 66, 59 and 52 raters respectively, who viewed both 2D and 3D images.

Results

In line with previous studies, we found that weight and height can be judged somewhat accurately from faces; contrary to previous research, we found that people were relatively inaccurate at assessing strength. We found no evidence that physical characteristics could be judged more accurately from 3D than 2D images.

Conclusion

Our results suggest physical characteristics are perceived with similar accuracy from 2D and 3D face images. They also suggest that the substantial costs associated with collecting 3D face scans may not be justified for research on the accuracy of facial judgments of physical characteristics.

目的大量文献研究了从面部图像中准确评估身体特征(如力量、体重和身高)的程度。虽然这些研究中的大多数都使用了二维(2D)人脸图像作为刺激,但最近的一些研究使用了三维(3D)人脸图像,因为它们可能包含在2D人脸图像中不可见的线索。由于3D人脸图像所需的设备比2D人脸图像所需要的设备贵得多,我们在这里研究了2D和3D人脸图像对身体特征的感知评级是如何比较的。方法通过直接比较182张同时拍摄的二维和三维人脸图像的力量、重量和身高评级的准确性,我们测试了三维人脸图像是否比二维人脸图像更好地捕捉力量、体重和身高的线索。分别由66名、59名和52名评分者对力量、身高和体重进行评分,他们同时观看了2D和3D图像。结果与以往的研究一致,我们发现通过面部可以在一定程度上准确地判断体重和身高;与之前的研究相反,我们发现人们在评估力量方面相对不准确。我们没有发现任何证据表明,从3D图像中可以比从2D图像中更准确地判断物理特征。结论我们的研究结果表明,从2D和3D人脸图像中可以以相似的精度感知身体特征。他们还认为,与收集3D人脸扫描相关的大量成本可能不足以用于研究面部对身体特征判断的准确性。
{"title":"Do 3D Face Images Capture Cues of Strength, Weight, and Height Better than 2D Face Images do?","authors":"Iris J Holzleitner,&nbsp;Alex L Jones,&nbsp;Kieran J O’Shea,&nbsp;Rachel Cassar,&nbsp;Vanessa Fasolt,&nbsp;Victor Shiramizu,&nbsp;Benedict C Jones,&nbsp;Lisa M DeBruine","doi":"10.1007/s40750-021-00170-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-021-00170-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objectives</h3><p>A large literature exists investigating the extent to which physical characteristics (e.g., strength, weight, and height) can be accurately assessed from face images. While most of these studies have employed two-dimensional (2D) face images as stimuli, some recent studies have used three-dimensional (3D) face images because they may contain cues not visible in 2D face images. As equipment required for 3D face images is considerably more expensive than that required for 2D face images, we here investigated how perceptual ratings of physical characteristics from 2D and 3D face images compare.\u0000</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>We tested whether 3D face images capture cues of strength, weight, and height better than 2D face images do by directly comparing the accuracy of strength, weight, and height ratings of 182 2D and 3D face images taken simultaneously. Strength, height and weight were rated by 66, 59 and 52 raters respectively, who viewed both 2D and 3D images.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>In line with previous studies, we found that weight and height can be judged somewhat accurately from faces; contrary to previous research, we found that people were relatively inaccurate at assessing strength. We found no evidence that physical characteristics could be judged more accurately from 3D than 2D images.</p><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>Our results suggest physical characteristics are perceived with similar accuracy from 2D and 3D face images. They also suggest that the substantial costs associated with collecting 3D face scans may not be justified for research on the accuracy of facial judgments of physical characteristics.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2021-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s40750-021-00170-8","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39368876","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to: Do Testosterone and Cortisol Jointly Relate to Adolescent Dominance? A Pre‑registered Multi‑method Interrogation of the Dual‑Hormone Hypothesis 修正:睾丸激素和皮质醇是否共同与青少年支配感有关?双激素假说的预登记多方法询问
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-08-19 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-021-00171-7
Allison N. Shields, Cassandra M. Brandes, Kathleen W. Reardon, Raul A. España, Jennifer L. Tackett
{"title":"Correction to: Do Testosterone and Cortisol Jointly Relate to Adolescent Dominance? A Pre‑registered Multi‑method Interrogation of the Dual‑Hormone Hypothesis","authors":"Allison N. Shields,&nbsp;Cassandra M. Brandes,&nbsp;Kathleen W. Reardon,&nbsp;Raul A. España,&nbsp;Jennifer L. Tackett","doi":"10.1007/s40750-021-00171-7","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-021-00171-7","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2021-08-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s40750-021-00171-7","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41533451","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Beards Increase the Speed, Accuracy, and Explicit Judgments of Facial Threat 胡须可以提高面部威胁的速度、准确性和明确判断
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-06-22 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-021-00169-1
Barnaby J. W. Dixson, Claire L. Barkhuizen, Belinda M. Craig

Objectives

To test whether intra-sexual selection has influenced perceptions of male facial hair. We predicted that beards would increase the speed and accuracy of perceptions of angry but not happy facial expressions. We also predicted that bearded angry faces would receive the highest explicit ratings of masculinity and aggressiveness, whereas higher prosociality ratings would be ascribed to clean-shaven happy faces.

Methods

A total of 106 participants, ranging from 17 to 59 years of age (M = 27.27, SD = 10.03); 59 were female and 47 were male (44.3%) completed an emotion categorization tasks and an explicit ratings task. Participants viewed faces of the same men when bearded, clean-shaven, and 10 days of natural growth (i.e. stubble) when posing angry and happy facial expressions.

Results

Angry facial expressions were categorised most rapidly and with the greatest accuracy on bearded faces, followed by faces with stubble then clean-shaven faces. Conversely, happy facial expressions were categorised most rapidly and with the greatest accuracy on clean-shaven faces, followed by stubbled faces then bearded faces. Irrespective of facial expression, full bearded faces received the highest ratings of masculinity followed by faces with stubble then clean-shaven faces. Aggressiveness ratings were highest for angry faces with full beards, followed by angry faces with stubble, with clean-shaven angry faces receiving the lowest ratings. In contrast to our prediction, bearded smiling faces were rated as significantly more prosocial than stubbled and clean-shaven smiling faces.

Conclusions

These findings contribute further evidence that men’s beardedness represents an intra-sexually selected badge of status that enhances nonverbal threat potentially by augmenting underlying masculine facial structures.

目的检验性内选择是否影响了对男性面部毛发的感知。我们预测,胡须会提高人们对愤怒但不快乐的面部表情的感知速度和准确性。我们还预测,留胡子的愤怒脸将获得最高的男性气概和攻击性的明确评分,而更高的亲社会性评分将归因于剃干净的快乐脸。方法共有106名参与者,年龄从17岁到59岁不等(M = 27.27,标准差 = 10.03);59名女性和47名男性(44.3%)完成了情绪分类任务和明确评分任务。参与者在留着胡子、刮得干干净净时观察同一个男人的脸,在摆出愤怒和快乐的面部表情时观察10天的自然生长(即胡茬)。结果愤怒的面部表情在留胡子的脸上分类最快、准确度最高,其次是留胡茬的脸,然后是剃干净的脸。相反,快乐的面部表情在剃干净的脸上被分类得最快、最准确,其次是胡子脸,然后是胡子脸。无论面部表情如何,留着胡子的脸都获得了最高的男性气质评价,其次是留着胡茬的脸,然后是剃得干干净净的脸。蓄满胡子的愤怒脸的攻击性评分最高,其次是留着胡茬的愤怒脸,剃干净的愤怒脸评分最低。与我们的预测相反,留胡子的笑脸被认为比留胡子、剃干净的笑脸更亲社会。结论这些发现进一步证明,男性的胡须代表了一种性别内部选择的身份徽章,通过增强潜在的男性面部结构,可能会增强非语言威胁。
{"title":"Beards Increase the Speed, Accuracy, and Explicit Judgments of Facial Threat","authors":"Barnaby J. W. Dixson,&nbsp;Claire L. Barkhuizen,&nbsp;Belinda M. Craig","doi":"10.1007/s40750-021-00169-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-021-00169-1","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objectives</h3><p>To test whether intra-sexual selection has influenced perceptions of male facial hair. We predicted that beards would increase the speed and accuracy of perceptions of angry but not happy facial expressions. We also predicted that bearded angry faces would receive the highest explicit ratings of masculinity and aggressiveness, whereas higher prosociality ratings would be ascribed to clean-shaven happy faces.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>A total of 106 participants, ranging from 17 to 59 years of age (M = 27.27, SD = 10.03); 59 were female and 47 were male (44.3%) completed an emotion categorization tasks and an explicit ratings task. Participants viewed faces of the same men when bearded, clean-shaven, and 10 days of natural growth (i.e. stubble) when posing angry and happy facial expressions.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>Angry facial expressions were categorised most rapidly and with the greatest accuracy on bearded faces, followed by faces with stubble then clean-shaven faces. Conversely, happy facial expressions were categorised most rapidly and with the greatest accuracy on clean-shaven faces, followed by stubbled faces then bearded faces. Irrespective of facial expression, full bearded faces received the highest ratings of masculinity followed by faces with stubble then clean-shaven faces. Aggressiveness ratings were highest for angry faces with full beards, followed by angry faces with stubble, with clean-shaven angry faces receiving the lowest ratings. In contrast to our prediction, bearded smiling faces were rated as significantly more prosocial than stubbled and clean-shaven smiling faces.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>These findings contribute further evidence that men’s beardedness represents an intra-sexually selected badge of status that enhances nonverbal threat potentially by augmenting underlying masculine facial structures.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2021-06-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s40750-021-00169-1","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47225268","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
Mate Availability and Sexual Disgust 伴侣可得性与性厌恶
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-05-13 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-021-00168-2
Courtney L. Crosby, Patrick K. Durkee, Anna G. B. Sedlacek, David M. Buss

Objective

One of the factors that sexual disgust should be calibrated to is the size of the mating pool. This study tested this hypothesis by examining whether perceptions of mate availability explain variance in levels of sexual disgust towards potential mates.

Methods

Participants (N = 853; 373 women) rated how sexually disgusting they found 60 potential mates that have previously been rated on attractiveness by a separate group of raters. We also measured participants’ perceptions of mate availability in their local environment, self-perceived attractiveness and mate value, and relevant control variables.

Results

Multilevel models revealed a negative association between sexual disgust towards potential mates and perceived mate availability—the opposite of what we predicted. We found support for our prediction that women had higher levels of sexual disgust than men, but only after addressing the confounding sex difference in target attractiveness. We also found the predicted negative association between target attractiveness and sexual disgust. Finally, as predicted, sexual disgust levels were more strongly related to potential mates’ attractiveness in individuals who perceived there to be many available mates in their local environment.

Conclusions

These findings generally bolster functional accounts of sexual disgust while highlighting the need for more evidence to ascertain the role of mate availability in the calibration of sexual disgust. Specifically, future research should examine the extent to which disgust levels may truncate mental representations of the mating pool instead of being calibrated by them.

性厌恶应该根据交配池的大小来衡量。这项研究通过检验对配偶可用性的感知是否可以解释对潜在配偶的性厌恶程度的差异来检验这一假设。方法参与者(N = 853;373名女性)对他们发现的60名潜在伴侣的性厌恶程度进行了评分,这些伴侣之前曾被另一组评分者根据吸引力进行过评分。我们还测量了参与者对当地环境中伴侣可用性的感知、自我感知的吸引力和伴侣价值,以及相关的控制变量。结果多层次模型显示,对潜在伴侣的性厌恶与感知到的伴侣可用性之间存在负相关,这与我们的预测相反。我们发现,我们的预测得到了支持,即女性的性厌恶程度高于男性,但这是在解决了目标吸引力中令人困惑的性别差异之后。我们还发现了目标吸引力和性厌恶之间的预测负相关。最后,正如预测的那样,在那些认为当地环境中有很多可用伴侣的人中,性厌恶程度与潜在伴侣的吸引力更密切相关。结论这些发现总体上支持了性厌恶的功能解释,同时强调需要更多的证据来确定配偶可得性在性厌恶校准中的作用。具体来说,未来的研究应该考察厌恶程度在多大程度上会截断交配池的心理表征,而不是由它们来校准。
{"title":"Mate Availability and Sexual Disgust","authors":"Courtney L. Crosby,&nbsp;Patrick K. Durkee,&nbsp;Anna G. B. Sedlacek,&nbsp;David M. Buss","doi":"10.1007/s40750-021-00168-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-021-00168-2","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>One of the factors that sexual disgust should be calibrated to is the size of the mating pool. This study tested this hypothesis by examining whether perceptions of mate availability explain variance in levels of sexual disgust towards potential mates.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>Participants (<i>N</i> = 853; 373 women) rated how sexually disgusting they found 60 potential mates that have previously been rated on attractiveness by a separate group of raters. We also measured participants’ perceptions of mate availability in their local environment, self-perceived attractiveness and mate value, and relevant control variables.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>Multilevel models revealed a negative association between sexual disgust towards potential mates and perceived mate availability—the opposite of what we predicted. We found support for our prediction that women had higher levels of sexual disgust than men, but only after addressing the confounding sex difference in target attractiveness. We also found the predicted negative association between target attractiveness and sexual disgust. Finally, as predicted, sexual disgust levels were more strongly related to potential mates’ attractiveness in individuals who perceived there to be many available mates in their local environment.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>These findings generally bolster functional accounts of sexual disgust while highlighting the need for more evidence to ascertain the role of mate availability in the calibration of sexual disgust. Specifically, future research should examine the extent to which disgust levels may truncate mental representations of the mating pool instead of being calibrated by them.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2021-05-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s40750-021-00168-2","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"38993612","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Do Testosterone and Cortisol Jointly Relate to Adolescent Dominance? A Pre-registered Multi-method Interrogation of the Dual-Hormone Hypothesis 睾丸激素和皮质醇是否与青少年支配力有关?双激素假说的预登记多方法质疑
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-04-29 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-021-00167-3
Allison N. Shields, Cassandra M. Brandes, Kathleen W. Reardon, Raul A. España, Jennifer L. Tackett

Objective

The goal of the present study was to extend the findings of the dual-hormone hypothesis (DHH) literature by assessing whether the interaction between testosterone (T) and cortisol (C) is associated with dominance in an adolescent sample via multiple methods of measuring T, C, and dominance, and with pre-registration of hypotheses and analyses.

Methods

In a sample of 337 adolescents (Mage = 14.98, SD = 1.51; 191 girls) and their caregivers, hormonal assays were obtained from hair and saliva, and dominance behavior was assessed across four operationalizations (behavioral ratings in a leadership task, self- and caregiver reported dominance motivations, and self-reported social potency).

Results

T and C main effects were generally null across hormone and dominance operationalizations, except that observer-rated dominance was negatively associated with salivary T, and social potency was positively associated with salivary T and negatively associated with salivary C. Support for the DHH was weak. Point estimates reflected a small negative T × C interaction for behavioral ratings of dominance, consistent with the DHH, whereas interaction effects for report-based dominance measures were close to zero or positive.

Conclusions

The results contribute to a growing evidence base suggesting T × C interaction effects are variable across measures and methods used to assess hormones and dominance and highlight the need for comprehensive, multi-method examinations employing best practices in scientific openness and transparency to reduce uncertainty in estimates. Measurement of hormones and dominance outcomes vary across labs and studies, and the largely null results should be considered in that context.

目的本研究的目的是通过多种测量T、C和优势的方法,以及假设和分析的预先登记,评估睾酮(T)和皮质醇(C)之间的相互作用是否与青少年样本中的优势有关,从而扩展双激素假说(DHH)文献的发现。方法对337名青少年(Mage = 14.98,标准差 = 1.51;191名女孩)及其照顾者,从头发和唾液中获得激素测定,并在四个操作中评估支配行为(领导任务中的行为评级、自我和照顾者报告的支配动机以及自我报告的社会效能),除了观察者评定的优势与唾液T呈负相关,社会效能与唾液T呈正相关,与唾液C呈负相关。对DHH的支持较弱。点估计值反映了一个小的负T × C支配行为评级的交互作用,与DHH一致,而基于报告的支配测量的交互作用效应接近于零或正。结论这些结果有助于增加证据基础,表明T × C相互作用效应在用于评估激素和优势的措施和方法中是可变的,并强调需要采用科学公开和透明的最佳实践进行全面、多方法的检查,以减少估计的不确定性。激素和优势结果的测量在实验室和研究中各不相同,在这种情况下应该考虑基本无效的结果。
{"title":"Do Testosterone and Cortisol Jointly Relate to Adolescent Dominance? A Pre-registered Multi-method Interrogation of the Dual-Hormone Hypothesis","authors":"Allison N. Shields,&nbsp;Cassandra M. Brandes,&nbsp;Kathleen W. Reardon,&nbsp;Raul A. España,&nbsp;Jennifer L. Tackett","doi":"10.1007/s40750-021-00167-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-021-00167-3","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>The goal of the present study was to extend the findings of the dual-hormone hypothesis (DHH) literature by assessing whether the interaction between testosterone (T) and cortisol (C) is associated with dominance in an adolescent sample via multiple methods of measuring T, C, and dominance, and with pre-registration of hypotheses and analyses.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>In a sample of 337 adolescents (<i>M</i><sub><i>age</i></sub> = 14.98, <i>SD</i> = 1.51; 191 girls) and their caregivers, hormonal assays were obtained from hair and saliva, and dominance behavior was assessed across four operationalizations (behavioral ratings in a leadership task, self- and caregiver reported dominance motivations, and self-reported social potency).</p><h3>Results</h3><p>T and C main effects were generally null across hormone and dominance operationalizations, except that observer-rated dominance was negatively associated with salivary T, and social potency was positively associated with salivary T and negatively associated with salivary C. Support for the DHH was weak. Point estimates reflected a small negative T × C interaction for behavioral ratings of dominance, consistent with the DHH, whereas interaction effects for report-based dominance measures were close to zero or positive.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>The results contribute to a growing evidence base suggesting T × C interaction effects are variable across measures and methods used to assess hormones and dominance and highlight the need for comprehensive, multi-method examinations employing best practices in scientific openness and transparency to reduce uncertainty in estimates. Measurement of hormones and dominance outcomes vary across labs and studies, and the largely null results should be considered in that context.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2021-04-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s40750-021-00167-3","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48470190","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1