首页 > 最新文献

Affective science最新文献

英文 中文
A Louder Call for the Integration of Multiple Nonverbal Channels in the Study of Affect 在情感研究中整合多种非语言渠道的呼声更高。
IF 2.1 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-26 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00265-x
Michele Morningstar

Affective science has increasingly sought to represent emotional experiences multimodally, measuring affect through a combination of self-report ratings, linguistic output, physiological measures, and/or nonverbal expressions. However, despite widespread recognition that non-facial nonverbal cues are an important facet of expressive behavior, measures of nonverbal expressions commonly focus solely on facial movements. This Commentary represents a call for affective scientists to integrate a larger range of nonverbal cues—including gestures, postures, and vocal cues—alongside facial cues in efforts to represent the experience of emotion and its communication. Using the measurement and analysis of vocal cues as an illustrative case, the Commentary considers challenges, potential solutions, and the theoretical and translational significance of working to integrate multiple nonverbal channels in the study of affect.

情感科学越来越多地寻求以多模态的方式表现情感体验,通过自我报告评级、语言输出、生理测量和/或非语言表达的组合来测量情感。然而,尽管人们普遍认识到非面部非语言线索是表达行为的一个重要方面,但对非语言表达的测量通常只关注面部动作。这篇评论呼吁情感科学家将更多的非语言线索(包括手势、姿势和声音线索)与面部线索结合起来,努力表现情感体验及其交流。本评论以声音线索的测量和分析为例,探讨了在情感研究中整合多种非语言渠道所面临的挑战、潜在的解决方案以及理论和转化意义。
{"title":"A Louder Call for the Integration of Multiple Nonverbal Channels in the Study of Affect","authors":"Michele Morningstar","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00265-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00265-x","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Affective science has increasingly sought to represent emotional experiences multimodally, measuring affect through a combination of self-report ratings, linguistic output, physiological measures, and/or nonverbal expressions. However, despite widespread recognition that non-facial nonverbal cues are an important facet of expressive behavior, measures of nonverbal expressions commonly focus solely on facial movements. This Commentary represents a call for affective scientists to integrate a larger range of nonverbal cues—including gestures, postures, and vocal cues—alongside facial cues in efforts to represent the experience of emotion and its communication. Using the measurement and analysis of vocal cues as an illustrative case, the Commentary considers challenges, potential solutions, and the theoretical and translational significance of working to integrate multiple nonverbal channels in the study of affect.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 3","pages":"201 - 208"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142402157","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Within-Person Dynamics of Job Boredom and Counterproductive Work Behavior: A Latent Change Score Modeling Approach 工作厌倦和工作反作用行为的人际动态:潜在变化分数建模法》。
IF 2.1 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-07 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00256-y
JeongJin Kim, Seth A. Kaplan, John A. Aitken, Lida P. Ponce

Job boredom is one of the most common negative affective states experienced in the workplace, yet also among the least well-understood. One stream of research suggests that employees frequently react to job boredom by engaging in counterproductive work behaviors (CWB). However, recent studies show the converse—that engaging in CWB relates to job boredom. As studies on the job boredom-CWB relationship primarily have been cross-sectional and at the between-person level of analysis, the directionality between these constructs remains in question. Therefore, research examining the within-person dynamics of job boredom and CWB within a short timeframe is needed. In the current study, we explore whether job boredom influences subsequent changes in CWB and vice versa. We examined these relationships using latent change score (LCS) modeling with 10-day experience sampling data (N = 120 individuals providing 1,161 observations). Findings supported a reciprocal relationship. Employees’ level of job boredom on a given day was associated with a subsequent increase in CWB on the next day, and the level of CWB on a given day was associated with a subsequent increase in job boredom on the next day. We discuss the implications of our findings, study limitations, and future research directions.

工作无聊是工作场所中最常见的负面情绪状态之一,但也是最不容易理解的情绪状态之一。有一种研究认为,员工经常会通过从事适得其反的工作行为(CWB)来应对工作无聊。然而,最近的研究却显示了相反的情况--从事 CWB 与工作无聊有关。由于有关工作无聊与 CWB 关系的研究主要是横断面研究,而且是人与人之间的分析,因此这些概念之间的方向性仍然存在疑问。因此,需要在短时间内对工作厌倦和 CWB 的人际动态进行研究。在本研究中,我们探讨了工作厌倦是否会影响 CWB 的后续变化,反之亦然。我们利用 10 天的经验抽样数据(N = 120 人,提供 1,161 个观察值),使用潜在变化得分(LCS)建模研究了这些关系。研究结果支持这种互惠关系。员工在某一天的工作无聊程度与第二天 CWB 的增加有关,而某一天的 CWB 水平与第二天工作无聊程度的增加有关。我们将讨论研究结果的意义、研究的局限性以及未来的研究方向。
{"title":"Within-Person Dynamics of Job Boredom and Counterproductive Work Behavior: A Latent Change Score Modeling Approach","authors":"JeongJin Kim,&nbsp;Seth A. Kaplan,&nbsp;John A. Aitken,&nbsp;Lida P. Ponce","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00256-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00256-y","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Job boredom is one of the most common negative affective states experienced in the workplace, yet also among the least well-understood. One stream of research suggests that employees frequently react to job boredom by engaging in counterproductive work behaviors (CWB). However, recent studies show the converse—that engaging in CWB relates to job boredom. As studies on the job boredom-CWB relationship primarily have been cross-sectional and at the between-person level of analysis, the directionality between these constructs remains in question. Therefore, research examining the within-person dynamics of job boredom and CWB within a short timeframe is needed. In the current study, we explore whether job boredom influences subsequent changes in CWB and vice versa. We examined these relationships using latent change score (LCS) modeling with 10-day experience sampling data (<i>N</i> = 120 individuals providing 1,161 observations). Findings supported a reciprocal relationship. Employees’ level of job boredom on a given day was associated with a subsequent increase in CWB on the next day, and the level of CWB on a given day was associated with a subsequent increase in job boredom on the next day. We discuss the implications of our findings, study limitations, and future research directions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 3","pages":"273 - 279"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11461387/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142402168","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Complex Emotion Dynamics Contribute to the Prediction of Depression: A Machine Learning and Time Series Feature Extraction Approach 复杂情绪动态有助于预测抑郁症:机器学习和时间序列特征提取方法。
IF 2.1 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-03 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00249-x
Mackenzie Zisser, Jason Shumake, Christopher G. Beevers

Emotion dynamics have demonstrated mixed ability to predict depressive symptoms and outperform traditional metrics like the mean and standard deviation of emotion reports. Here, we expand the types of emotion dynamic features used in prior work and apply a machine learning algorithm to predict depression symptoms. We obtained seven ecological momentary assessment (EMA) studies from previous work on depression and emotion dynamics (N = 890). These studies measured self-reported sadness, positive affect, and negative affect 5 to 10 times per day for 7 to 21 days (schedule varied across studies). These data were fed through a feature extraction routine to generate hundreds of emotion dynamic features. A gradient boosting machine (GBM) using all available emotion dynamics features was the best of all models assessed. This model’s out-of-sample prediction (R2pred) for depression severity ranged from .20 to .44 depending on EMA interpolation method and samples included in the analysis. It also explained significantly more variance than a benchmark model of individuals’ mean emotion ratings over the assessment period, R2pred = .089. Comprehensive feature mining of emotion dynamics obtained during EMA may be necessary to identify processes that predict depression symptoms beyond mean emotion ratings.

情绪动态在预测抑郁症状方面的能力参差不齐,而且优于情绪报告的平均值和标准偏差等传统指标。在此,我们扩展了之前工作中使用的情绪动态特征类型,并应用机器学习算法来预测抑郁症状。我们从以前关于抑郁和情绪动态的研究中获得了七项生态瞬间评估(EMA)研究(N = 890)。这些研究测量了自我报告的悲伤情绪、积极情绪和消极情绪,每天测量 5 到 10 次,持续 7 到 21 天(不同研究的时间安排不同)。这些数据通过特征提取程序生成数百个情绪动态特征。使用所有可用情绪动态特征的梯度提升机(GBM)是所有评估模型中最好的。该模型对抑郁严重程度的样本外预测(R 2 pred)从 0.20 到 0.44 不等,具体取决于 EMA 插值方法和分析中包含的样本。与评估期间个人平均情绪评级的基准模型(R 2 pred = .089)相比,该模型对方差的解释也明显更多。要识别平均情绪评分以外的预测抑郁症状的过程,可能需要对 EMA 期间获得的情绪动态进行全面的特征挖掘。
{"title":"Complex Emotion Dynamics Contribute to the Prediction of Depression: A Machine Learning and Time Series Feature Extraction Approach","authors":"Mackenzie Zisser,&nbsp;Jason Shumake,&nbsp;Christopher G. Beevers","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00249-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00249-x","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Emotion dynamics have demonstrated mixed ability to predict depressive symptoms and outperform traditional metrics like the mean and standard deviation of emotion reports. Here, we expand the types of emotion dynamic features used in prior work and apply a machine learning algorithm to predict depression symptoms. We obtained seven ecological momentary assessment (EMA) studies from previous work on depression and emotion dynamics (<i>N</i> = 890). These studies measured self-reported sadness, positive affect, and negative affect 5 to 10 times per day for 7 to 21 days (schedule varied across studies). These data were fed through a feature extraction routine to generate hundreds of emotion dynamic features. A gradient boosting machine (GBM) using all available emotion dynamics features was the best of all models assessed. This model’s out-of-sample prediction (<i>R</i><sup>2</sup><sub>pred</sub>) for depression severity ranged from .20 to .44 depending on EMA interpolation method and samples included in the analysis. It also explained significantly more variance than a benchmark model of individuals’ mean emotion ratings over the assessment period, <i>R</i><sup>2</sup><sub>pred</sub> = .089. Comprehensive feature mining of emotion dynamics obtained during EMA may be necessary to identify processes that predict depression symptoms beyond mean emotion ratings.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 3","pages":"259 - 272"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142402161","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Emergence of Organized Emotion Dynamics in Childhood 童年时期有组织情绪动态的出现。
IF 2.1 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-31 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00248-y
Mira L. Nencheva, Erik C. Nook, Mark A. Thornton, Casey Lew-Williams, Diana I. Tamir

Emotions change from one moment to the next. They have a duration from seconds to hours and then transition to other emotions. Here, we describe the early ontology of these key aspects of emotion dynamics. In five cross-sectional studies (N = 904) combining parent surveys and ecological momentary assessment, we characterize how caregivers’ perceptions of children’s emotion duration and transitions change over the first 5 years of life and how they relate to children’s language development. Across these ages, the duration of children’s emotions increased, and emotion transitions became increasingly organized by valence, such that children were more likely to transition between similarly valenced emotions. Children with more mature emotion profiles also had larger vocabularies and could produce more emotion labels. These findings advance our understanding of emotion and communication by highlighting their intertwined nature in development and by charting how dynamic features of emotion experiences change over the first years of life.

情绪在不同的时刻会发生变化。它们的持续时间从几秒到几小时不等,然后过渡到其他情绪。在这里,我们描述了情绪动态的这些关键方面的早期本体。在五项结合家长调查和生态学瞬间评估的横断面研究(N = 904)中,我们描述了照顾者对儿童情绪持续时间和过渡的看法在儿童出生后前五年的变化情况,以及它们与儿童语言发展的关系。在这些年龄段中,儿童情绪的持续时间增加了,情绪的转换也越来越有组织,儿童更有可能在类似情绪之间转换。情绪特征更成熟的儿童的词汇量也更大,能产生更多的情绪标签。这些发现突出了情绪和交流在儿童成长过程中的相互交织性,并描绘了情绪体验的动态特征在生命最初几年的变化过程,从而加深了我们对情绪和交流的理解:在线版本包含补充材料,可在10.1007/s42761-024-00248-y上获取。
{"title":"The Emergence of Organized Emotion Dynamics in Childhood","authors":"Mira L. Nencheva,&nbsp;Erik C. Nook,&nbsp;Mark A. Thornton,&nbsp;Casey Lew-Williams,&nbsp;Diana I. Tamir","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00248-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00248-y","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Emotions change from one moment to the next. They have a <i>duration</i> from seconds to hours and then <i>transition</i> to other emotions. Here, we describe the early ontology of these key aspects of emotion dynamics. In five cross-sectional studies (<i>N</i> = 904) combining parent surveys and ecological momentary assessment, we characterize how caregivers’ perceptions of children’s emotion duration and transitions change over the first 5 years of life and how they relate to children’s language development. Across these ages, the duration of children’s emotions increased, and emotion transitions became increasingly organized by valence, such that children were more likely to transition between similarly valenced emotions. Children with more mature emotion profiles also had larger vocabularies and could produce more emotion labels. These findings advance our understanding of emotion and communication by highlighting their intertwined nature in development and by charting how dynamic features of emotion experiences change over the first years of life.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 3","pages":"246 - 258"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142402167","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
You are How You Eat: Foraging Behavior as a Potential Novel Marker of Rat Affective State 你是怎么吃的觅食行为是大鼠情感状态的潜在新标记。
IF 2.1 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-26 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00242-4
Vikki Neville, Emily Finnegan, Elizabeth S. Paul, Molly Davidson, Peter Dayan, Michael Mendl

Effective and safe foraging requires animals to behave according to the expectations they have about the rewards, threats, and costs in their environment. Since these factors are thought to be reflected in the animals’ affective states, we can use foraging behavior as a window into those states. In this study, rats completed a foraging task in which they had repeatedly to decide whether to continue to harvest a food source despite increasing time costs, or to forgo food to switch to a different food source. Rats completed this task across two experiments using manipulations designed to induce both positive and negative, and shorter- and longer- term changes in affective state: removal and return of enrichment (Experiment 1), implementation and reversal of an unpredictable housing treatment (Experiment 1), and delivery of rewards (tickling or sucrose) and punishers (air-puff or back-handling) immediately prior to testing (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, rats completed fewer trials and were more prone to switching between troughs when housed in standard, compared to enriched, housing conditions. In Experiment 2, rats completed more trials following pre-test tickling compared to pre-test sucrose delivery. However, we also found that they were prone to disengaging from the task, suggesting they were really choosing between three options: ‘harvest’, ‘switch’, or ‘not work’. This limits the straightforward interpretation of the results. At present, foraging behavior within the context of this task cannot reliably be used as an indicator of an affective state in animals.

有效和安全的觅食要求动物的行为符合它们对环境中的奖励、威胁和成本的预期。由于这些因素被认为反映在动物的情感状态中,因此我们可以利用觅食行为作为了解这些状态的窗口。在这项研究中,大鼠完成了一项觅食任务,在这项任务中,它们需要反复决定是在时间成本不断增加的情况下继续收获食物,还是放弃食物转而寻找其他食物来源。大鼠在两次实验中完成了这项任务,实验中采用了旨在诱导大鼠情绪状态发生积极和消极、短期和长期变化的操作方法:移除和恢复富集(实验 1)、实施和撤销不可预测的饲养处理(实验 1)以及在测试前立即给予奖励(搔痒或蔗糖)和惩罚(吹气或背摔)(实验 2)。在实验 1 中,与富集饲养条件相比,在标准饲养条件下大鼠完成的试验更少,而且更容易在食槽之间切换。在实验 2 中,大鼠在测试前搔痒后完成的试验比测试前蔗糖输送后完成的试验要多。但是,我们也发现,它们很容易从任务中脱离出来,这表明它们实际上是在三个选项中进行选择:收获"、"转换 "或 "不工作"。这就限制了对结果的直接解释。目前,这项任务中的觅食行为还不能可靠地用作动物情感状态的指标:在线版本包含补充材料,可查阅 10.1007/s42761-024-00242-4。
{"title":"You are How You Eat: Foraging Behavior as a Potential Novel Marker of Rat Affective State","authors":"Vikki Neville,&nbsp;Emily Finnegan,&nbsp;Elizabeth S. Paul,&nbsp;Molly Davidson,&nbsp;Peter Dayan,&nbsp;Michael Mendl","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00242-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00242-4","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Effective and safe foraging requires animals to behave according to the expectations they have about the rewards, threats, and costs in their environment. Since these factors are thought to be reflected in the animals’ affective states, we can use foraging behavior as a window into those states. In this study, rats completed a foraging task in which they had repeatedly to decide whether to continue to harvest a food source despite increasing time costs, or to forgo food to switch to a different food source. Rats completed this task across two experiments using manipulations designed to induce both positive and negative, and shorter- and longer- term changes in affective state: removal and return of enrichment (Experiment 1), implementation and reversal of an unpredictable housing treatment (Experiment 1), and delivery of rewards (tickling or sucrose) and punishers (air-puff or back-handling) immediately prior to testing (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, rats completed fewer trials and were more prone to switching between troughs when housed in standard, compared to enriched, housing conditions. In Experiment 2, rats completed more trials following pre-test tickling compared to pre-test sucrose delivery. However, we also found that they were prone to disengaging from the task, suggesting they were really choosing between three options: ‘harvest’, ‘switch’, or ‘not work’. This limits the straightforward interpretation of the results. At present, foraging behavior within the context of this task cannot reliably be used as an indicator of an affective state in animals.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 3","pages":"232 - 245"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11461729/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142402169","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Culture and Awe: Understanding Awe as a Mixed Emotion 文化与敬畏将敬畏理解为一种混合情感。
IF 2.1 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-25 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00243-3
Jennifer E. Stellar, Yang Bai, Craig L. Anderson, Amie Gordon, Galen D. McNeil, Kaiping Peng, Dacher Keltner

Recent work is establishing awe as an important positive emotion that offers physical and psychological benefits. However, early theorizing suggests that awe’s experience is often tinged with fear. How then, do we reconcile emergent positive conceptualizations of awe with its more fearful elements? We suggest that positive conceptualizations of awe may partially reflect modern Western experiences of this emotion, which make up the majority of participant samples when studying awe. To test whether awe contains more fearful qualities outside of Western cultures, we compared participants’ experiences of this emotion in China to those in the United States. In a two-week daily diary study (Study 1), Chinese participants reported greater fear than American participants during experiences of awe, but not a comparison positive emotion. In response to a standardized awe induction (Study 2), Chinese participants reported more fear, whereas American participants reported more positive emotions. Physiological changes in autonomic activity differed by culture only for heart rate, but not skin conductance or respiratory sinus arrhythmia. These findings reveal that awe may be experienced as a more fearful, mixed emotion in China than in the United States and suggest that current positive conceptualizations of awe may reflect a disproportionate reliance on modern Western samples.

最近的研究表明,敬畏是一种重要的积极情绪,能带来生理和心理上的益处。然而,早期的理论研究表明,敬畏的体验往往带有恐惧的色彩。那么,我们该如何协调新出现的积极的敬畏概念与其更多的恐惧元素呢?我们认为,敬畏的积极概念化可能部分反映了现代西方人对这种情感的体验,而在研究敬畏时,大部分参与者都是西方人。为了检验西方文化之外的敬畏是否包含更多的恐惧特质,我们比较了中国和美国参与者对这种情感的体验。在一项为期两周的每日日记研究(研究 1)中,中国受试者在体验敬畏时比美国受试者表现出更大的恐惧感,但在比较积极情绪时却没有表现出恐惧感。在对标准化敬畏诱导的反应中(研究 2),中国参与者报告了更多的恐惧,而美国参与者报告了更多的积极情绪。自律神经活动的生理变化仅在心率方面存在文化差异,而在皮肤电导率或呼吸窦性心律失常方面不存在文化差异。这些研究结果表明,在中国,敬畏可能是一种比美国人更恐惧的混合情绪,并表明目前对敬畏的积极概念可能反映了对现代西方样本的过度依赖:在线版本包含补充材料,可查阅 10.1007/s42761-024-00243-3。
{"title":"Culture and Awe: Understanding Awe as a Mixed Emotion","authors":"Jennifer E. Stellar,&nbsp;Yang Bai,&nbsp;Craig L. Anderson,&nbsp;Amie Gordon,&nbsp;Galen D. McNeil,&nbsp;Kaiping Peng,&nbsp;Dacher Keltner","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00243-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00243-3","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Recent work is establishing awe as an important positive emotion that offers physical and psychological benefits. However, early theorizing suggests that awe’s experience is often tinged with fear. How then, do we reconcile emergent positive conceptualizations of awe with its more fearful elements? We suggest that positive conceptualizations of awe may partially reflect modern Western experiences of this emotion, which make up the majority of participant samples when studying awe. To test whether awe contains more fearful qualities outside of Western cultures, we compared participants’ experiences of this emotion in China to those in the United States. In a two-week daily diary study (Study 1), Chinese participants reported greater fear than American participants during experiences of awe, but not a comparison positive emotion. In response to a standardized awe induction (Study 2), Chinese participants reported more fear, whereas American participants reported more positive emotions. Physiological changes in autonomic activity differed by culture only for heart rate, but not skin conductance or respiratory sinus arrhythmia. These findings reveal that awe may be experienced as a more fearful, mixed emotion in China than in the United States and suggest that current positive conceptualizations of awe may reflect a disproportionate reliance on modern Western samples.\u0000</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 2","pages":"160 - 170"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141763001","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Speaking Well and Feeling Good: Age-Related Differences in the Affective Language of Resting State Thought 说得好,感觉好:静息状态思维情感语言中与年龄有关的差异。
IF 2.1 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00239-z
Teodora Stoica, Eric S. Andrews, Austin M. Deffner, Christopher Griffith, Matthew D. Grilli, Jessica R. Andrews-Hanna

Despite the prevalence and importance of resting state thought for daily functioning and psychological well-being, it remains unclear how such thoughts differ between young and older adults. Age-related differences in the affective tone of resting state thoughts, including the affective language used to describe them, could be a novel manifestation of the positivity effect, with implications for well-being. To examine this possibility, a total of 77 young adults (M = 24.9 years, 18–35 years) and 74 cognitively normal older adults (M = 68.6 years, 58–83 years) spoke their thoughts freely during a think-aloud paradigm across two studies. The emotional properties of spoken words and participants’ retrospective self-reported affective experiences were computed and examined for age differences and relationships with psychological well-being. Study 1, conducted before the start of the COVID-19 pandemic, revealed that older adults exhibited more diversity of positive, but not negative, affectively tinged words compared to young adults and more positive self-reported thoughts. Despite being conducted virtually during the COVID-19 pandemic, study 2 replicated many of study 1’s findings, generalizing results across samples and study contexts. In an aggregated analysis of both samples, positive diversity predicted higher well-being beyond other metrics of affective tone, and the relationship between positive diversity and well-being was not moderated by age. Considering that older adults also exhibited higher well-being, these results hint at the possibility that cognitively healthy older adults’ propensity to experience more diverse positive concepts during natural periods of restful thought may partly underlie age-related differences in well-being and reveal a novel expression of the positivity effect.

尽管静息状态思维对日常功能和心理健康非常普遍和重要,但这种思维在年轻人和老年人之间有何不同仍不清楚。与年龄相关的静息状态思维情感基调的差异,包括用于描述静息状态思维的情感语言的差异,可能是积极效应的一种新的表现形式,并对幸福感产生影响。为了研究这种可能性,在两项研究中,共有 77 名年轻人(男 = 24.9 岁,18-35 岁)和 74 名认知正常的老年人(男 = 68.6 岁,58-83 岁)在思考-朗读范式中自由地说出了他们的想法。研究人员计算了口语的情感属性和参与者回顾性自我报告的情感体验,并考察了年龄差异以及与心理健康的关系。研究 1 是在 COVID-19 大流行开始前进行的,结果显示,与年轻人相比,老年人表现出更多积极的情感词汇,而不是消极的情感词汇,而且自我报告的想法也更积极。尽管研究 2 是在 COVID-19 大流行期间进行的,但研究 2 复制了研究 1 的许多发现,并在不同样本和研究环境中推广了研究结果。在对两个样本进行的综合分析中,积极多样性对幸福感的预测高于情感基调的其他指标,而且积极多样性与幸福感之间的关系不受年龄的影响。考虑到老年人也表现出较高的幸福感,这些结果暗示了这样一种可能性,即认知健康的老年人在自然的休息思考期间倾向于体验更多样的积极概念,这可能是幸福感中与年龄有关的差异的部分原因,并揭示了积极性效应的一种新的表现形式:在线版本包含补充材料,可在 10.1007/s42761-024-00239-z.上查阅。
{"title":"Speaking Well and Feeling Good: Age-Related Differences in the Affective Language of Resting State Thought","authors":"Teodora Stoica,&nbsp;Eric S. Andrews,&nbsp;Austin M. Deffner,&nbsp;Christopher Griffith,&nbsp;Matthew D. Grilli,&nbsp;Jessica R. Andrews-Hanna","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00239-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00239-z","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Despite the prevalence and importance of resting state thought for daily functioning and psychological well-being, it remains unclear how such thoughts differ between young and older adults. Age-related differences in the affective tone of resting state thoughts, including the affective language used to describe them, could be a novel manifestation of the positivity effect, with implications for well-being. To examine this possibility, a total of 77 young adults (<i>M</i> = 24.9 years, 18–35 years) and 74 cognitively normal older adults (<i>M</i> = 68.6 years, 58–83 years) spoke their thoughts freely during a think-aloud paradigm across two studies. The emotional properties of spoken words and participants’ retrospective self-reported affective experiences were computed and examined for age differences and relationships with psychological well-being. Study 1, conducted before the start of the COVID-19 pandemic, revealed that older adults exhibited more diversity of positive, but not negative, affectively tinged words compared to young adults and more positive self-reported thoughts. Despite being conducted virtually during the COVID-19 pandemic, study 2 replicated many of study 1’s findings, generalizing results across samples and study contexts. In an aggregated analysis of both samples, positive diversity predicted higher well-being beyond other metrics of affective tone, and the relationship between positive diversity and well-being was not moderated by age. Considering that older adults also exhibited higher well-being, these results hint at the possibility that cognitively healthy older adults’ propensity to experience more diverse positive concepts during natural periods of restful thought may partly underlie age-related differences in well-being and reveal a novel expression of the positivity effect.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 2","pages":"141 - 159"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11264499/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141763002","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mean Affect Moderates the Association between Affect Variability and Mental Health 平均情感调节情感多变性与心理健康之间的关系
IF 2.1 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-13 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00238-0
Brooke N. Jenkins, Lydia Q. Ong, Anthony D. Ong, Hee Youn (Helen) Lee, Julia K. Boehm

Increasing evidence suggests that within-person variation in affect is a dimension distinct from mean levels along which individuals can be characterized. This study investigated affect variability’s association with concurrent and longitudinal mental health and how mean affect levels moderate these associations. The mental health outcomes of depression, panic disorder, self-rated mental health, and mental health professional visits from the second and third waves of the Midlife in the United States Study were used for cross-sectional (n = 1,676) and longitudinal outcomes (n = 1,271), respectively. These participants took part in the National Study of Daily Experiences (NSDE II), where they self-reported their affect once a day for 8 days, and this was used to compute affect mean and variability. Greater positive affect variability cross-sectionally predicted a higher likelihood of depression, panic disorder, mental health professional use, and poorer self-rated mental health. Greater negative affect variability predicted higher panic disorder probability. Longitudinally, elevated positive and negative affect variability predicted higher depression likelihood and worse self-rated mental health over time, while greater positive affect variability also predicted increased panic disorder probability. Additionally, mean affect moderated associations between variability and health such that variability-mental health associations primarily took place when mean positive affect was high (for concurrent mental health professional use and longitudinal depression) and when mean negative affect was low (for concurrent depression, panic disorder, self-rated mental health, and longitudinal self-rated mental health). Taken together, affect variability may have implications for both short- and long-term health and mean levels should be considered.

越来越多的证据表明,人与人之间的情感变化是有别于平均水平的一个维度,可以根据平均水平来描述个体。本研究调查了情感变异与并发和纵向心理健康的关系,以及平均情感水平如何缓和这些关系。美国中年研究 "第二波和第三波中的抑郁症、恐慌症、自评心理健康和心理健康专业就诊等心理健康结果分别用于横截面结果(n = 1,676 人)和纵向结果(n = 1,271 人)。这些参与者参加了全国日常经历研究(NSDE II),他们在8天内每天自我报告一次自己的情绪,并以此计算情绪的平均值和变异性。从横截面来看,积极情绪变异性越大,患抑郁症、恐慌症、使用心理健康专业人员的可能性就越大,自我评价的心理健康程度就越差。负面情绪变异性越大,预测恐慌症的概率越高。从纵向来看,随着时间的推移,积极情绪和消极情绪变异性的升高预示着抑郁可能性的升高和自我评定的心理健康状况的恶化,而积极情绪变异性的升高也预示着恐慌症概率的升高。此外,平均情感调节了变异性与健康之间的关联,因此变异性与心理健康之间的关联主要发生在平均积极情感较高时(对于同时使用心理健康专业人员和纵向抑郁而言),以及平均消极情感较低时(对于同时抑郁、恐慌症、自评心理健康和纵向自评心理健康而言)。综上所述,情绪的变化可能会对短期和长期健康产生影响,因此应考虑平均水平。
{"title":"Mean Affect Moderates the Association between Affect Variability and Mental Health","authors":"Brooke N. Jenkins,&nbsp;Lydia Q. Ong,&nbsp;Anthony D. Ong,&nbsp;Hee Youn (Helen) Lee,&nbsp;Julia K. Boehm","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00238-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00238-0","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Increasing evidence suggests that within-person variation in affect is a dimension distinct from mean levels along which individuals can be characterized. This study investigated affect variability’s association with concurrent and longitudinal mental health and how mean affect levels moderate these associations. The mental health outcomes of depression, panic disorder, self-rated mental health, and mental health professional visits from the second and third waves of the Midlife in the United States Study were used for cross-sectional (<i>n</i> = 1,676) and longitudinal outcomes (<i>n</i> = 1,271), respectively. These participants took part in the National Study of Daily Experiences (NSDE II), where they self-reported their affect once a day for 8 days, and this was used to compute affect mean and variability. Greater positive affect variability cross-sectionally predicted a higher likelihood of depression, panic disorder, mental health professional use, and poorer self-rated mental health. Greater negative affect variability predicted higher panic disorder probability. Longitudinally, elevated positive and negative affect variability predicted higher depression likelihood and worse self-rated mental health over time, while greater positive affect variability also predicted increased panic disorder probability. Additionally, mean affect moderated associations between variability and health such that variability-mental health associations primarily took place when mean positive affect was high (for concurrent mental health professional use and longitudinal depression) and when mean negative affect was low (for concurrent depression, panic disorder, self-rated mental health, and longitudinal self-rated mental health). Taken together, affect variability may have implications for both short- and long-term health and mean levels should be considered.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 2","pages":"99 - 114"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s42761-024-00238-0.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141350174","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attuned to the Flux of Life: Relations Between Ability Emotional Intelligence and Emotional Reactivity 适应生活的变化:能力情商与情绪反应之间的关系
IF 2.1 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00241-5
Michael D. Robinson, Roberta L. Irvin, Michelle R. Persich Durham

The field of ability-related emotional intelligence (ability EI) could benefit from new perspectives concerning dynamic operations. According to a recent perspective, variations in ability EI are likely to be linked to variations in skills related to evaluation. This perspective contends, perhaps counterintuitively, that higher levels of ability EI are likely to be linked to higher levels of emotional reactivity, defined in terms of stronger event-emotion relationships. Two studies (total N = 245) pursue such ideas in the context of multilevel models involving event valence and emotional experience. Variations in ability EI modulated event-emotion relationships in the context of laboratory inductions involving hypothetical events (Study 1), affective images varying in valence (Study 1), and with respect to naturally occurring variations in positive and negative daily events (Study 2), such that higher levels of ability EI were linked to stronger event-emotion relationships, regardless of whether events and emotions were positive or negative in valence. These results provide new evidence for recent theorizing concerning ability EI while speaking to functional versus dysfunctional perspectives on emotional reactivity.

与能力相关的情商(ability EI)领域可以从有关动态操作的新视角中获益。根据最近的一种观点,能力情商的变化很可能与评价技能的变化有关。这种观点认为(也许是反直觉的),较高水平的能力情感指数可能与较高水平的情感反应性有关,情感反应性的定义是事件与情感之间更强的关系。有两项研究(总人数= 245)在涉及事件价值和情绪体验的多层次模型中探讨了这一观点。在涉及假定事件的实验室诱导(研究 1)、情绪价位变化的情感图像(研究 1)以及自然发生的积极和消极日常事件变化(研究 2)的背景下,能力 EI 的变化调节了事件-情感关系,因此,无论事件和情绪的价位是积极还是消极,较高水平的能力 EI 都与较强的事件-情感关系相关联。这些结果为近期有关能力情感指数的理论研究提供了新的证据,同时也说明了情绪反应性的功能性与功能障碍观点。
{"title":"Attuned to the Flux of Life: Relations Between Ability Emotional Intelligence and Emotional Reactivity","authors":"Michael D. Robinson,&nbsp;Roberta L. Irvin,&nbsp;Michelle R. Persich Durham","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00241-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00241-5","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The field of ability-related emotional intelligence (ability EI) could benefit from new perspectives concerning dynamic operations. According to a recent perspective, variations in ability EI are likely to be linked to variations in skills related to evaluation. This perspective contends, perhaps counterintuitively, that higher levels of ability EI are likely to be linked to higher levels of emotional reactivity, defined in terms of stronger event-emotion relationships. Two studies (total <i>N</i> = 245) pursue such ideas in the context of multilevel models involving event valence and emotional experience. Variations in ability EI modulated event-emotion relationships in the context of laboratory inductions involving hypothetical events (Study 1), affective images varying in valence (Study 1), and with respect to naturally occurring variations in positive and negative daily events (Study 2), such that higher levels of ability EI were linked to stronger event-emotion relationships, regardless of whether events and emotions were positive or negative in valence. These results provide new evidence for recent theorizing concerning ability EI while speaking to functional versus dysfunctional perspectives on emotional reactivity.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 2","pages":"115 - 128"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141277319","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction To: Subjective Socioeconomic Status Moderates How Resting Heart Rate Variability Predicts Pain Response 更正主观社会经济地位如何调节静息心率变异性对疼痛反应的预测
IF 2.1 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-02-12 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00236-2
Jacinth J. X. Tan, Chin Hong Tan, Michael W. Kraus
{"title":"Correction To: Subjective Socioeconomic Status Moderates How Resting Heart Rate Variability Predicts Pain Response","authors":"Jacinth J. X. Tan,&nbsp;Chin Hong Tan,&nbsp;Michael W. Kraus","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00236-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00236-2","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 1","pages":"67 - 67"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s42761-024-00236-2.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139783233","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Affective science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1